Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2014 Jun 30;8:459. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00459

Precision Grip in Congenital and Acquired Hemiparesis: Similarities in Impairments and Implications for Neurorehabilitation

Yannick Bleyenheuft 1,*, Andrew M Gordon 2
PMCID: PMC4074995  PMID: 25071502

Abstract

Background: Patients with congenital and acquired hemiparesis incur long-term functional deficits, among which the loss of prehension that may impact their functional independence. Identifying, understanding, and comparing the underlying mechanisms of prehension impairments represent an opportunity to better adapt neurorehabilitation.

Objective: The present review aims to provide a better understanding of precision grip deficits in congenital and acquired hemiparesis and to determine whether the severity and type of fine motor control impairments depend on whether or not the lesions are congenital or acquired in adulthood.

Methods: Using combinations of the following key words: fingertip force, grip force, precision grip, cerebral palsy, stroke, PubMed, and Scopus databases were used to search studies from 1984 to 2013.

Results: Individuals with both congenital and acquired hemiparesis were able to some extent to use anticipatory motor control in precision grip tasks, even if this control was impaired in the paretic hand. In both congenital and acquired hemiparesis, the ability to plan efficient anticipatory motor control when the less-affected hand is used provides a possibility to remediate impairments in anticipatory motor control of the paretic hand.

Conclusion: Surprisingly, we observed very few differences between the results of studies in children with congenital hemiplegia and stroke patients. We suggest that the underlying specific strategies of neurorehabilitation developed for each one could benefit the other.

Keywords: fingertip force, grip force, precision grip, cerebral palsy, stroke

Introduction

Brain lesion is the most prevalent cause of physical disability: cerebral palsy occurs in 1 out of 303 live births (Murphy et al., 1993; Stanley et al., 2000; Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014a), and stroke occurs in 0.7% of adults below 45 years, 3% of adults 45–64 years old, and in 8.3% of adults older than 65 years (Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014b). Manual dexterity is frequently impaired in these patients, resulting in long-term functional deficits. Thus, it is important to identify the underlying causes of these deficits in order to better focus neurorehabilitation based on concrete scientific evidence, with the aim of improving patients’ function during typical daily activities.

Various levels of sensorimotor processing can be affected by unilateral brain lesions and influence dexterity. During the last 25 years, precision grip tasks, allowing the measure of fingertip forces during manipulation, have been developed to study skilled manual dexterity (Johansson and Westling, 1984, 1988a; Johansson, 2002). This approach has also been used to delineate the mechanisms underlying prehensile control in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy (HCP) and adults with hemiplegic stroke. The present review provides an overview of precision grip impairments in these patients, with the aim of comparing impaired mechanisms in congenital versus adult occurrence of the lesion. Surprisingly, despite many congruent disabilities in these pathologies, we found only a few papers in the literature including both children with HCP and adult stroke patients: one describing the effects of upper-extremity casting (Lanin et al., 2007) and one on oral anti-spasticity medication (Montané et al., 2004). The similarities and differences in the mechanisms underlying precision grip in both pathologies have not been examined. Due to differences in the pathophysiology after lesion, differential impairments are generally expected. In children with HCP, depending on the age the CNS damage occurs in utero, there may be a complete reorganization of the corticospinal projections, whereby movements of the paretic hand may be controlled entirely by the contralesional hemisphere (Staudt et al., 2004; Eyre et al., 2007; Gordon et al., 2013). Such structural plasticity does not occur following CNS injury later in life (Newton et al., 2006). Despite the great disparity in age (generally 6–14 years in children and more than 60 years in adults) and the variation in lesion locations, we document a remarkable similarity in fingertip force impairments, highlighting impairments in timing, coupling, and amplitude of prehensile forces. The specific impairments documented may allow us to more precisely focus neurorehabilitation.

Predictive and reactive grip force control

Two types of control mechanisms are used to perform skilled hand movements: predictive mechanisms – that enable one to anticipate the movement on the basis of sensorimotor memories of the manipulated object, and reactive mechanisms – that allow for correction of the movements/forces. Reactive mechanisms are typically feedback loops emanating from sensory and/or visual afferents. It is generally accepted that predictive mechanisms, also called feedforward or anticipatory mechanisms, are based on internal models in the central nervous system (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000). These include: (1) an inverse model that uses the current state of the limb and the specific context to generate an appropriate motor command, (2) a forward sensory and motor model that predicts the movement resulting from the motor command and estimates the sensory feedback of the new state of the limb, allowing a comparison (3) with actual feedback (4) and subsequent adjustment of the motor command (Figure 1). It is the close interaction between predictive and reactive mechanisms that allows for the production of smooth movements.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

To achieve a precision grip movement, the goal of the task is sent to an inverse model (1) that generates a motor command. Due to this motor command, a movement of the upper limb is generated. In parallel, a forward sensory and motor model (2) is generated. This forward model predicts the movement induced by the motor command and estimates the sensory feedback of the new state of the hand and arm. It allows comparison with actual feedback (4) and consequently there is an updating of the motor command. Actual feedback emanates from sensors and is transmitted to the feedback controller (4) after sensory processing (3). The red dotted frame represents the feedforward components, and the green frame denotes the feedback components. Both can be affected at different levels in unilateral brain lesions, with consequential impairment to precision grip.

In the context of brain lesions, prehension deficits could be explained by impairments in predictive and/or reactive mechanisms. This is of particular interest for the design of rehabilitation programs. These mechanisms have been well-studied and described in healthy adults and typically developing children, especially through analysis of the timing and magnitude of grip force (GF) and the tangential load forces (LF) during grip–lift manipulation (Johansson and Westling, 1984, 1988a; Forssberg et al., 1991, 1992, 1995; Gordon et al., 1992; Witney et al., 2004). In healthy adults, there is a well-described sequence of events comprising a typical coordination pattern (Johansson and Westling, 1984, 1988a) during a grip–lift task (see Figure 2): first the contact between fingers and the object is initiated in a quick succession (T0–T2). During the preload phase (T2–T3), GF increases prior to LF onset. GF and LF subsequently increase in parallel during the loading phase (T3–T4). Forces rates are characterized by single peaks that are well-timed. After the parallel increase of forces, the forces are steadily maintained during a static phase (T5–T6) in which the object is held in the air, that is followed by the release of the object including a replacement phase (T6–T7) an a subsequent rapid decrease in the grip and LF (T7–T8) until the thumb and index fingers are released from the object (T8–T9).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Representation of the grip (red) and load (blue) forces applied on a handheld object during a grip–lift task, as well as the vertical position (lower panel) of the handheld object. The different phases of the grip–lift task are highlighted with dotted lines. T0–T2, the contact between fingers and the object is initiated in a quick succession. T2–T3, preload phase, GF increases prior to LF onset. T3–T4, loading phase, GF and LF subsequently increase in parallel. T5–T6, static phase, followed by the release of the object including a replacement phase (T6–T7) of a subsequent rapid decrease in the grip and load forces (T7–T8) until the thumb and index fingers are released from the object (T8–T9).

The adaptation of the forces to the object’s properties during precision grip tasks requires the use of tactile (Westling and Johansson, 1987) and weight-related information (Johansson, 1996). Adaptation to various load and frictional conditions has been well-established. However, such information about the object’s physical properties is not instantly available due to delays in the transmission of sensory information. Therefore, to manage objects with adequate forces in everyday life, GF and LF forces are planned in an anticipatory manner on the basis of internal representations of the objects from prior manipulatory experience (Johansson and Westling, 1987, 1988a; Gordon et al., 1993), i.e., before initiating the movement.

In typically developing children, this anticipatory coordination of GF and LF matures until approximating that of adults at the age of 6–8 years (Forssberg et al., 1991). Before that age, the forces do not increase in parallel, and there are negative LFs at the start and excessive and multiple increments in GF during the loading phase (Forssberg et al., 1991).

This immature strategy has been interpreted as a control strategy relying mainly on feedback. Due to the delay of the feedback, the adjustment of the motor command is sequential, which does not allow smooth movements. With continuing maturation and experience during childhood, children utilize internal representations of the objects, allowing a predictive (anticipatory) control of the movement (Forssberg et al., 1991).

Predictive and reactive GF control in a bimanual context

The integration of the precise GF and LF coupling in a bimanual context have been studied recently when opposite or concomitant patterns of force are required in the hands: a handheld object had to be placed on the top of another object statically held in the other hand (Islam et al., 2011) or two pieces of a handheld object had to be pulled apart (Smits-Engelsman et al., 2011). These tasks, when performed with healthy adults, demonstrated coordinated actions of both hands.

The use of one hand to generate a rapid increase of LF to an object held by the opposite hand has also been used as a model of coordination. In that context, a predictive increase in GF is observed before the rapid LF increase (Eliasson et al., 1995; Bleyenheuft et al., 2009). After the brisk LF increase, there is a systematic second increase in GF, programed as a predictive action that allows an optimal stabilization of the object around the brisk force increase. Self-induced brisk load increases also mature during childhood, approaching adult values at 9–10 years of age (Eliasson et al., 1995; Bleyenheuft and Thonnard, 2010a).

Methods

Data sources and literature selection

PubMed and Scopus electronic databases were searched using combinations of the following terms: GF, fingertip force, and precision grip either with “cerebral palsy” or “stroke.” Studies from 1984 to 2013 were retained. An additional hand-search was conducted in the reference lists of the articles meeting the search criteria. The search procedure included only studies in English. Studies focused on heat and pain, as well as animal studies and studies dedicated to other clinical forms of CP (e.g., diplegic or bilateral CP) were excluded.

Results

Skilled hand movements both in pediatric HCP and adult stroke patients are impaired (Eliasson et al., 1991, 1992, 1995a, 2006; Steenbergen et al., 1998, 2008; Forssberg et al., 1999; Gordon and Duff, 1999; Gordon et al., 1999, 2003, 2006; Eliasson and Gordon, 2000; Duff and Gordon, 2003; Duque et al., 2003; Hermsdörfer et al., 2003; Nowak et al., 2003; Takahashi and Reinkensmeyer, 2003; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2004; Wenzelburger et al., 2005; Blennerhassett et al., 2006, 2008; McDonnell et al., 2006; Mutsaarts et al., 2006; Raghavan et al., 2006; Mackenzie et al., 2009; Quaney et al., 2010; Seo et al., 2010; van Elk et al., 2010; Naik et al., 2011; Prabhu et al., 2011; Bleyenheuft and Gordon, 2013). Table 1 highlights the precision grip impairments that have been observed on the more affected (paretic) hand of children with HCP and adults with stroke. Figure 3 provides examples of forces coordination in the more affected hand of a child with congenital hemiplegia (A) and a stroke patient (B).

Table 1.

Deficits of precision grip in children with HCP and stroke patients.

HCP Stroke
a. Preload phase
Push down object before lifting Eliasson et al. (1991); Duque et al. (2003) McDonnell et al. (2006)
Longer duration Eliasson et al. (1991); Duque et al. (2003) Hermsdörfer et al. (2003); Nowak et al. (2003); Takahashi and Reinkensmeyer (2003); Wenzelburger et al. (2005); Blennerhassett et al. (2006); McDonnell et al. (2006); Raghavan et al. (2006); Quaney et al. (2010); Naik et al. (2011)
b. Loading phase
Asynchronous onset of GF and LF Eliasson et al. (1991, 1992, 1995a, 2006); Forssberg et al. (1999); Gordon and Duff (1999); Gordon et al. (1999); Duque et al. (2003) Blennerhassett et al. (2006); McDonnell et al. (2006)
Excessive GF at LF increase Eliasson et al. (1991, 1992); Duque et al. (2003) Hermsdörfer et al. (2003); Wenzelburger et al., 2005; Blennerhassett et al. (2006); McDonnell et al. (2006)
Multiple increments in force rates Eliasson et al. (1991, 1992); Duque et al. (2003) Blennerhassett et al. (2006); McDonnell et al. (2006)
c. GF
Higher Eliasson et al. (1991, 1992, 1995a); Forssberg et al. (1999); Gordon and Duff (1999); Gordon et al., 1999; Duque et al. (2003) Hermsdörfer et al. (2003); Nowak et al. (2003); Wenzelburger et al. (2005); Blennerhassett et al. (2006); McDonnell et al. (2006)
Altered digit direction Seo et al. (2010)
d. Release
Sequential force coordination Duff and Gordon (2003); Gordon et al. (2003) Naik et al. (2011)
e. Adaptation to different weights
Need more trials to adapt Eliasson et al. (1992); Steenbergen et al. (1998); Gordon and Duff (1999) McDonnell et al. (2006); Raghavan et al. (2006)
f. Adaptation to different frictions
Need more trials to adapt Eliasson et al. (1995a); Duff and Gordon (2003)
g. Predictive abilities
Demonstrated Gordon et al. (1999); Duff and Gordon (2003); Gordon et al. (2006); Steenbergen et al. (2008) Hermsdörfer et al. (2003); Nowak et al. (2003); Hermsdörfer et al. (2004)
Anticipation perturbed Eliasson et al. (1992); Gordon et al. (2006); Mutsaarts et al. (2006); Steenbergen and Gordon (2006); Bleyenheuft and Thonnard (2010a) Nowak et al. (2003); Takahashi and Reinkensmeyer (2003); Hermsdörfer et al. (2004); McDonnell et al. (2006); Blennerhassett et al. (2007)
Transfer from non-paretic to paretic Gordon et al. (1999); Steenbergen et al. (2008) Raghavan et al. (2006)

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Representation of typical traces on the paretic hand of (A) a child with congenital hemiparesis and (B) a stroke patient. T2–T3, preload phase, T3–T4, loading phase, T5 start of static phase.

Children with HCP

In children with HCP, the fingertip forces deviate from that observed in age-matched typically developing children: the timing of GF and LF onset is disrupted, GF is excessive, especially at the onset of LF increase (Eliasson et al., 1991, 1992, 1995a; Forssberg et al., 1999; Gordon and Duff, 1999; Gordon et al., 1999; Hermsdörfer et al., 2003) and present multiple successive increments that can also be observed in the force rates. The object is frequently pushed down toward the support, generating a negative LF before initiating the lifting phase. While encountering these impairments in precision grip, children with HCP demonstrated some ability to adapt to various weight and friction conditions, provided they had a greater number of trials to adapt to new conditions (Steenbergen et al., 1998; Gordon and Duff, 1999). As shown in a longitudinal study, these impairments are not increasing with time (Eliasson et al., 2006), which is congruent with other studies on deficits in maximal grip strength and isometric finger forces (Smits-Engelsman et al., 2004). During the release of a handheld object, children with HCP demonstrate also a sequential coordination of forces (Eliasson and Gordon, 2000). However these forces are adapted to the weight of the object (Eliasson and Gordon, 2000). Impairments in replacement and release of handheld objects are increased when speed and/or accuracy are imposed (Gordon et al., 2003). However, it is not clear if the impairments observed are linked to predictive or reactive control of precision grip.

Predictive abilities in grasp control were demonstrated in children with cerebral palsy, since they are able to form and retain internal representations of new objects (Duff and Gordon, 2003); there is an influence of bimanual tasks on the performance of the more and less-affected hand (Steenbergen et al., 2008) and these children have the ability to transfer learned forces from the less-affected to the more affected hand (Gordon et al., 1999, 2006). However these predictive abilities (Mutsaarts et al., 2006; Steenbergen and Gordon, 2006; Bleyenheuft and Thonnard, 2010b), as well as their motor imagery (van Elk et al., 2010), are clearly impaired. It has been suggested that the impaired precision grip of children with HCP is the consequence of an inability to use internal models of manipulated objects. This could be due to inefficient feedback from the paretic hand (Gordon and Duff, 1999; Gordon et al., 1999) or to high-level deficits in sensorimotor integration (Eliasson et al., 1992; Bleyenheuft and Thonnard, 2010b). The role of predictive and/or reactive deficits in the precision grip of children with HCP has been further studied using a paradigm (self-or-examiner-imposed load to a handheld object) where both were tested independently (Bleyenheuft and Thonnard, 2010b). Predictive and reactive conditions were observed in the paretic hand: in the paretic hand, children were able to anticipate events prior to the brisk load increase, but were unable to pursue predictive control afterward (Bleyenheuft and Thonnard, 2010b). Interestingly, these predictive motor control deficits were not observed in the less-affected hand. The less-affected hand had been previously studied in paradigms showing that while anticipatory control is present (Gordon et al., 1999, 2006; Steenbergen et al., 2008), subtle deficits can be observed (Steenbergen et al., 1998; Forssberg et al., 1999; Gordon et al., 1999; Mutsaarts et al., 2006; van Elk et al., 2010). These subtle deficits are congruent with those observed on the more affected hand but are not as pronounced: compared to controls, manipulation with the less-affected hand demonstrates lower force rates, increased duration of preload phase, negative LF during the preload phase, overall higher GF, and an increase of forces that is less parallel (Gordon et al., 1999). Nevertheless, anticipatory control is present (Gordon et al., 1999, 2006; Steenbergen et al., 2008) suggesting lateralized impairments in motor planning underlying precision grip in HCP, which could reflect a lateralized deficit in the sensorimotor integration (Prabhu et al., 2011). In bimanual tasks, however, children with HCP demonstrate less accurate performance in both hands when compared to unimanual use (Islam et al., 2011; Smits-Engelsman et al., 2011), suggesting that while dissociated in unimanual motor planning, each effector can be influenced by the other in a bimanual context.

These results, consistent with previous findings, suggest that the abilities of the less-affected side could be used in neurorehabilitation to improve motor control of the paretic side. For a recent systematic review on sensorimotor deficits in children with HCP, see also Bleyenheuft and Gordon (2013).

Stroke patients

Precision grip in the paretic hand of stroke patients is also impaired. The GF is generally higher – which is also seen already at the onset of the LF increase – and prolongations are observed in the timing of the movement: there is an asynchronous onset of GF and LF, leading to a longer preload phase and discontinuous force increase (Hermsdörfer et al., 2003; Nowak et al., 2003; Takahashi and Reinkensmeyer, 2003; Wenzelburger et al., 2005; Blennerhassett et al., 2006, 2008; McDonnell et al., 2006; Raghavan et al., 2006; Quaney et al., 2010). This longer preload phase is also correlated to clinical measures of handgrip limitations (Blennerhassett et al., 2008). Similar to congenital hemiparesis, the stroke patients push the object on the table before lifting it (McDonnell et al., 2006). There is a reduced ability to adapt to changes in the object’s weight (McDonnell et al., 2006; Raghavan et al., 2006), although some adaptations still occur. The release phase is also impaired, showing sequential force coordination (Naik et al., 2011). Altered digit GF direction during prehension tasks in stroke patients has a direct impact on grip (Seo et al., 2010), but can be improved through visual force feedback (Seo et al., 2011). Deficits in prediction of the inertial load profile arising from voluntary movement with handheld objects were also demonstrated in patients with acute stroke performing lift-and-hold tasks and point-to-point movements (Nowak et al., 2003). Since the same type of impairments has been delineated in patients with cortical and subcortical stroke – increased GF and deficits in temporal coupling – the authors suggested that the internal models responsible for precise regulation of forces were impaired. While these models are thought to be formed in the cerebellum (Wolpert et al., 1998; Wolpert and Flanagan, 2001), Nowak et al. (2003) argued that cortical and subcortical structures should be involved in the subsequent process of issuing motor commands. Studies investigating the ability of patients with chronic stroke to learn anticipation (either by force disturbance during a reaching task (Wenzelburger et al., 2005), or presenting objects with different shapes (Raghavan et al., 2010) showed an impaired ability to adapt. However, a partial ability to form and use internal models was retained (Wenzelburger et al., 2005). In contrast, Hermsdörfer et al. (2003, 2004) suggested that internal models are preserved in patients with chronic cerebral stroke since the feedforward nature of the GF coupling with LF was preserved during object oscillations. The difference observed in these two paradigms may be linked to the fact that the GF–LF coordination was studied, respectively, in discrete and rhythmic coordination patterns. We hypothesize that rhythmic (more robust) coordination might be preserved while anticipatory motor control needed for discrete tasks might be impaired. These high-order motor planning deficits observed in patients with subcortical subacute and chronic stroke are ameliorated by transfer of information from the unaffected hemisphere (at least in right hemiparesis) (Raghavan et al., 2006).

The precision grip impairments described for various stroke patients are of importance because there is an impact of unimanual dysfunction on bimanual tasks, and therefore on activities of daily living (Müller and Dichgans, 1994). Excessive GF and deficits in the temporal coupling of the forces have been observed in the paretic hand of stroke patients (cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar lesions). On the basis of one of the largest studies of precision grip control after stroke, the excessive GF was attributed to impaired sensory feedback (Blennerhassett et al., 2007). The lack of temporal coupling was linked to deficits in developing internal models. Temporal coupling impairments could also be due to disturbed feedback transmission – this implies that an inability to update the sensorimotor memory. Predictive and reactive nature of deficits in precision grip have also been assessed further using the self or examiner brisk loading bimanual system described for children with HCP in order to test both of them separately. Deficits highlighted in the paretic hand were similar to those observed on the more affected hand of children with HCP. In predictive conditions, while an anticipatory control was present, temporal deficits were shown after brisk load increase. In reactive conditions, a slight increase was observed in the reflex latency. The less-affected hand did not differ from age- and sex-matched healthy controls (Dispa et al., 2014).

Previous studies including analysis of precision grip in the less-affected hand in patients with unilateral middle cerebral artery stroke (Quaney et al., 2005) showed an increase GF at lift-off across dynamic and static portions of the grip–lift task. In patients with subcortical lesion (subacute phase), the ipsilesional hand was also affected with a slowing of movements and poor coordination of grip and lift forces (Nowak et al., 2007).

Both in stroke patients and children with HCP, major precision grip impairments are observed in the paretic hand. Many of the deficits highlighted in the prehension of children with HCP and stroke patients present similarities: excessive GF, lack of GF and LF coupling, negative LF at the onset of the movement, release impairments, and a decreased ability to adapt to different loads (Table 1). These impairments are likely due to impairments in both predictive and reactive process.

Implications for Rehabilitation

Various implications for rehabilitation could arise from precision grip deficits described in both congenital and acquired hemiparesis. First, as a consequence of deficits observed in the integration of sensory feedback in the anticipatory control of precision grip, one could suggest training of sensory abilities, as these are needed for precision grip tasks and are traditionally considered as a prerequisite for the performance of prehension tasks (Moberg, 1958; Jones, 1996; Gordon et al., 1999). The idea that recovery of the sensory system is needed and/or useful for motor recovery is widespread (Peurala et al., 2002; Smania et al., 2003; Blennerhassett et al., 2007; Conforto et al., 2007; Shirahashi et al., 2007), and partly forms the basis of some treatment approaches such as neurodevelopmental treatment (NDT). A recent paper on tactile spatial resolution in acute stroke patients even suggests that the link between sensory and motor modalities is such that pure motor stroke patients temporarily develop a hypersensibility on the paretic hand to compensate for the absence of movement (Doh et al., 2008). This implies a strong link between sensory and motor functions. Therefore, one could focus rehabilitation mainly on sensory modalities with the expectation of enhancing motor function (Bumin and Kayihan, 2001). This could be especially true at the beginning of rehabilitation where the motor deficit is greatest, and there is typically a lack of active movement. However, this strong link between sensory and motor function can be questioned as a result of the lack of correlation between sensory dysfunction and motor deficits (Fugl-Meyer et al., 1975; Bleyenheuft and Thonnard, 2011). The correlation between sensory modalities and independence in everyday life activities has been described only as weak to moderate in stroke patients (Carey and Matyas, 2011). Still in stroke patients, a recent single case report shows no effect of a change in proprioceptive abilities on motor recovery (Helliwell, 2009). Regarding children with HCP, Cooper et al. (1995) stated that “The extent of sensory loss did not mirror the severity of motor deficit.” Moreover, a study that included a large sample of children with cerebral palsy demonstrated that neither tactile pressure detection nor proprioception was related to manual ability (Arnould et al., 2007).

Many attempts have been made to increase sensory performance of stroke patients through long-term rehabilitation (Dannenbaum and Dykes, 1988; Carey et al., 1993; Yekutiel and Guttman, 1993; Carey and Matyas, 2005; Voller et al., 2006; Sullivan and Hedman, 2007), or transiently via local anesthesia to the intact hand (Doh et al., 2008) or transcranial direct current stimulation (Ragert et al., 2008). A recent review suggested that while there is a larger number of studies describing efficacy of passive sensory training than active sensory training (Schabrun and Hillier, 2009), there is “insufficient evidence to support or refute their effectiveness in improving sensory impairment, upper-limb function” (Doyle et al., 2010). However, a critical appraisal for this review added some relevant approaches to “active” sensory retraining (Carey et al., 2010) and a recent study showed the effectiveness of neurorehabilitation on sensation (Carey et al., 2011). Sensory cueing has also been recently described as useful for improving voluntary arm use (Fong et al., 2011).

Motor recovery is linked to cortical reorganization (Rehme et al., 2011a,b). Many strategies have also been developed to increase the motor performance of precision grip in stroke patients: Quaney et al. (2010) have recently shown interesting results on visuomotor training and suggested also to use retraining of the ipsilesional hand to improve function of contralesional hand. Strength training in the paretic hand improves both strength and function (Harris and Eng, 2010). This type of treatment based on force training is congruent with studies reporting that grip strength of the paretic hand best predicts upper-limb performance (Boissy et al., 1999; Harris and Eng, 2007). Recently, transcranial direct continuous stimulation (dual-tDCS) applied simultaneously over the ipsilesional (anodal) and contralateral (cathodal) primary motor cortices for a short duration (20 min) has been demonstrated as transiently improving precision grip in patients with stroke (Lefebvre et al., 2014). Functional electrical stimulation therapy is described as efficacious for rehabilitation of reach and grasp function (Thrasher et al., 2008) Constraint-induced movement therapy (CIMT) is also designed to increase hand motor function (see Eliasson and Gordon, 2008; Sawaki et al., 2008; Gordon and Friel, 2009; Sakzewski et al., 2009). Inhibitory repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS – 1 Hz) over the unaffected primary motor cortex improves the timing of grasping and lifting with the affected hand (Dafotakis et al., 2008). Mirror therapy (Michielsen et al., 2011) and mental training have a positive effect on the hand function (Müller et al., 2007; Nilsen et al., 2010; Prasad et al., 2010; Ietswaart et al., 2011). Robot-assisted therapies also have potential for prehension recovery (Ziherl et al., 2010; Merians et al., 2011). The monitoring of recovery and treatment strategies after stroke through motion analysis has been reviewed (Nowak, 2008). The nature of hand motor impairment and its treatment after stroke has also been reviewed (Raghavan, 2007), as well as treatment’s intensity (Cooke et al., 2010). Whatever the strategy used to re-train precision grip, a large cohort study shows that the likelihood to regain some dexterity is strongly linked to the presence of early voluntary extension of the fingers and abduction of the shoulder (Nijland et al., 2010; Kong et al., 2011).

In children with HCP, many types of training have provided evidence of increasing precision grip performance: short term training effects were reported (Wenzelburger et al., 2005), training based on visual feedback (Valvano and Newell, 1998), and intensive training with CIMT (Charles et al., 2001).

In stroke patients, like in children with HCP, the ipsilesional hand has only subtle deficits (Gordon et al., 1999; Quaney et al., 2005; Nowak et al., 2007). Therefore, the performance of this less-affected hand is of interest for rehabilitation purposes. As the patients are able to correctly program precision tasks with the non-involved hand, this ability could be used either to transfer intact sensory information from the non-involved to the involved hand, or to construct a template for the appropriate motor command. The use of both hands to establish appropriate motor commands bilaterally has been developed with success in neurorehabilitation both in children with HCP (Hung et al., 2004; Gordon et al., 2007, 2008) and acquired hemiparesis (Brogårdh and Lexell, 2010; Hayner et al., 2010). In contrast, while the transfer of information from the non-involved to the involved hand has been clearly demonstrated (Raghavan et al., 2006; Steenbergen et al., 2008), likely involving the corpus callosum, neurorehabilitation based on the alternate use of both hands (kinematic mirroring), starting with the non-paretic hand has not yet been tested. While a new neurorehabilitation strategy based on this principle could be interesting if it enables acquisition of a valid motor plan that could be transferred from the non-paretic to the paretic hand, the effectiveness of this scheme could be questioned because of inter-hemispheric influences (Rouiller et al., 1994; Di Lazzaro et al., 1999; Mochizuki et al., 2004; Westlake and Nagarajan, 2011). Through transcallosal interactions, each primary motor cortex when stimulated has an inhibitory action on the opposite motor cortex. Future studies based on kinematic mirroring should provide interesting clues on the possibility of transfer from the less-affected to the paretic hand (Ward and Cohen, 2004; Duque et al., 2005).

Limitations

The discussion of similarities in hand impairments of children with HCP and adult acquired lesion would have benefited from investigations both in adults with HCP and in children with later acquired lesion (3 years and older). While several studies are available for adults with HCP, especially in the field of functioning and quality of life (Roebroeck et al., 2009; Mesterman et al., 2010), none specifically focus on their hand function and precision grip. However, the stability of gross motor function classification (GMFCS) after the age of 12 years (McCormick et al., 2007; Hanna et al., 2009) and the absence of decrease in functional performance with age in longitudinal studies (Eliasson et al., 2006; Blank and Kluger, 2009) suggest that the deficit relative to healthy peers probably remains stable when children with HCP become adults. For children with later acquired brain injury, no information is available on hand function. Therefore, it would be interesting in future studies to specifically focus on hand function in children with acquired brain injury after the age of 3 years as well as in adults with HCP.

Conclusion

In this review, we have documented impairments in predictive motor control of patients with congenital and acquired hemiparesis. The ability to use such information in an anticipatory manner (i.e., based on memory from prior manipulations) to scale their forces was present in both children and adults with hemiparesis. Patients with acquired or congenital hemiparesis had the ability – often with many trials for new objects/conditions – to predicatively scale their forces to different weights or surface friction. This highlights their remaining possibility to use predictive control, even if it is impaired in the paretic hand. In both congenital and acquired hemiparesis, it is suggested that the ipsilesional hand may aid the subsequent control of the contralesional hand. Indeed, even if it has subtle deficits, this hand has proven in both groups of patients an intact ability to aid in anticipatory control.

We initially aimed to determine whether the neonatal or adult occurrence of the lesion influences the type and importance of hand deficits. We hypothesized that the comparison of hand deficits in early lesions in children and acquired lesion in adults should help to direct neurorehabilitation of hand deficits to each pathology more specifically. Surprisingly, except for the more focused studies on different locations of stroke in adults (cerebellar, capsular stroke, etc.), we observed very few differences between the results of studies in children with HCP and stroke patients. The motor control deficits measured with precision grip were similar. Therefore, we suggest that while the therapeutic approach should be adapted to the age and the level of understanding of each patient, the underlying strategies of neurorehabilitation could be similar in adult stroke patients and children with HCP.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Professors P. Lefèvre and J. L. Thonnard for their help with the design of the model presented in Figure 1.

References

  1. Arnould C., Penta M., Thonnard J. L. (2007). Hand impairments and their relationship with manual ability in children with cerebral palsy. J. Rehabil. Med. 39, 708–714 10.2340/16501977-0111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Blank R., Kluger G. (2009). Changes in elementary finger-hand functions over time in preschool children with spastic cerebral palsy. Neurosci. Lett. 455, 30–35 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.03.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blennerhassett J. M., Carey L. M., Matyas T. A. (2006). Grip force regulation during pinch grip lifts under somatosensory guidance: comparison between people with stroke and healthy controls. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 87, 418–429 10.1016/j.apmr.2005.11.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blennerhassett J. M., Carey L. M., Matyas T. A. (2008). Clinical measures of handgrip limitation relate to impaired pinch grip force control after stroke. J. Hand Ther. 21, 245–252 10.1197/j.jht.2007.10.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blennerhassett J. M., Matyas T. A., Carey L. M. (2007). Impaired discrimination of surface friction contributes to pinch grip deficit after stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 21, 263–272 10.1177/1545968306295560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bleyenheuft Y., Gordon A. M. (2013). Precision grip control, sensory impairments and their interactions in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 3014–3028 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bleyenheuft Y., Lefèvre P., Thonnard J. L. (2009). Predictive mechanisms control grip force after impact in self-triggered perturbations. J. Mot. Behav. 41, 411–417 10.3200/35-08-084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bleyenheuft Y., Thonnard J. L. (2010a). Grip control in children before, during, and after impulsive loading. J. Mot. Behav. 42, 169–177 10.1080/00222891003765843 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bleyenheuft Y., Thonnard J. L. (2010b). Predictive and reactive control of precision grip in children with congenital hemiplegia. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 24, 318–327 10.1177/1545968309353327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bleyenheuft Y., Thonnard J. L. (2011). Tactile spatial resolution in unilateral brain lesions and its correlation with digital dexterity. J. Rehabil. Med. 43, 251–256 10.2340/16501977-0651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Boissy P., Bourbonnais D., Carlotti M. M., Gravel D., Arsenault B. A. (1999). Maximal grip force in chronic stroke subjects and its relationship to global upper extremity function. Clin. Rehabil. 13, 354–362 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brogårdh C., Lexell J. (2010). A 1-year follow-up after shortened constraint-induced movement therapy with and without mitt poststroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 91, 460–464 10.1016/j.apmr.2009.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bumin G., Kayihan H. (2001). Effectiveness of two different sensory-integration programmes for children with spastic diplegic cerebral palsy. Disabil. Rehabil. 23, 394–399 10.1080/09638280010008843 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carey L., Macdonell R., Matyas T. A. (2011). SENSe: study of the effectiveness of neurorehabilitation on sensation: a randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 25, 304–313 10.1177/1545968310397705 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Carey L. M., Blennerhassett J., Matyas T. (2010). Evidence for the retraining of sensation after stroke remains limited. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 57, 200–202 10.1111/j.1440-1630.2010.00867.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carey L. M., Matyas T. A. (2005). Training of somatosensory discrimination after stroke: facilitation of stimulus generalization. Am. J. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 84, 428–442 10.1097/01.PHM.0000159971.12096.7F [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carey L. M., Matyas T. A. (2011). Frequency of discriminative sensory loss in the hand after stroke in a rehabilitation setting. J. Rehabil. Med. 43, 257–263 10.2340/16501977-0662 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carey L. M., Matyas T. A., Oke L. E. (1993). Sensory loss in stroke patients: effective training of tactile and proprioceptive discrimination. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 74, 602–611 10.1016/0003-9993(93)90158-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014a). CDC 24/7 Saving Lives, Protecting People [Internet, last review/update 7 May 2014]. Cerebral Palsy [updated 20 March 2014]. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/cp/
  20. Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014b). CDC 24/7 Saving Lives, Protecting People [Internet, last review/update 7 May 2014]. Prevalence of Stroke – United States, 2006–2010 [May 25, 2012/61(20); 379–382]. Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm6120a5.htm
  21. Charles J., Lavinder G., Gordon A. M. (2001). Effects of constraint-induced therapy on hand function in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 13, 68–76 10.1097/00001577-200107000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Conforto A. B., Cohen L. G., dos Santos R. L., Scaff M., Marie S. K. (2007). Effects of somatosensory stimulation on motor function in chronic cortico-subcortical strokes. J. Neurol. 254, 333–339 10.1007/s00415-006-0364-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cooke E. V., Mares K., Clark A., Tallis R. C., Pomeroy V. M. (2010). The effects of increased dose of exercise-based therapies to enhance motor recovery after stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Med. 8:60. 10.1186/1741-7015-8-60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cooper J., Majnemer A., Rosenblatt B., Birnbaum R. (1995). The determination of sensory deficits in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. J. Child Neurol. 10, 300–309 10.1177/088307389501000412 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dafotakis M., Grefkes C., Eickhoff S. B., Karbe H., Fink G. R., Nowak D. A. (2008). Effects of rTMS on grip force control following subcortical stroke. Exp. Neurol. 211, 407–412 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.02.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dannenbaum R. M., Dykes R. W. (1988). Sensory loss in the hand after sensory stroke: therapeutic rationale. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 69, 833–839 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Di Lazzaro V., Rothwell J. C., Oliviero A., Profice P., Insola A., Mazzone P., et al. (1999). Intracortical origin of the short latency facilitation produced by pairs of threshold magnetic stimuli applied to human motor cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 129, 494–499 10.1007/s002210050919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dispa D., Thonnard J. L., Bleyenheuft Y. (2014). Impaired predictive and reactive control of precision grip in chronic stroke patients. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 37, 130–137 10.1097/MRR.0000000000000045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Doh W. Y., Kang S. Y., Sohn Y. H. (2008). Enhanced spatial discrimination in paretic hands. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 1153–1157 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Doyle S., Bennett S., Fasoli S. E., McKenna K. T. (2010). Interventions for sensory impairment in the upper limb after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 16, CD006331. 10.1002/14651858.CD006331.pub2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Duff S. V., Gordon A. M. (2003). Learning of grasp control in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 45, 746–757 10.1017/S0012162203001397 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Duque J., Hummel F., Celnik P., Murase N., Mazzocchio R., Cohen L. G. (2005). Transcallosal inhibition in chronic subcortical stroke. Neuroimage 28, 940–946 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Duque J., Thonnard J.-L., Vandermeeren Y., Sebire G., Cosnard G., Olivier E. (2003). Correlation between impaired dexterity and corticospinal tract dysgenesis in congenital hemiplegia. Brain 126, 732–747 10.1093/brain/awg069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Eliasson A., Forssberg H., Ikuta K., Apel L., Westling G., Johansson R. (1995). Development of human precision grip. V. Anticipatory and triggered grip actions during sudden loading. Exp. Brain Res. 106, 425–433 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Eliasson A.-C., Forssberg H., Hung Y.-C., Gordon A. M. (2006). Development of hand function and precision grip control in individuals with cerebral palsy: a 13-year follow-up study. Pediatrics 118, e1226–e1236 10.1542/peds.2005-2768 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Eliasson A. C., Gordon A. M. (2000). Impaired force coordination during object release in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 42, 228–234 10.1017/S0012162200000396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Eliasson A. C., Gordon A. M. (2008). “Constraint-induced movement therapy for children with hemiplegia,” in Improving Hand Function in Children with Cerebral Palsy: Theory, Evidence and Intervention. Clinics in Developmental Medicine, eds Eliasson A. C., Burtner P. (London: Mac Keith Press; ), 308–319 [Google Scholar]
  38. Eliasson A. C., Gordon A. M., Forssberg H. (1991). Basic co-ordination of manipulative forces of children with cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 33, 661–670 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1991.tb14943.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Eliasson A. C., Gordon A. M., Forssberg H. (1992). Impaired anticipatory control of isometric forces during grasping by children with cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 34, 216–225 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1992.tb14994.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Eliasson A. C., Gordon A. M., Forssberg H. (1995a). Tactile control of isometric fingertip forces during grasping in children with cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 37, 72–84 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1995.tb11933.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Eyre J. A., Smith M., Dabydeen L., Clowry G. J., Petacchi E., Battini R., et al. (2007). Is hemiplegic cerebral palsy equivalent to amblyopia of the corticospinal system? Ann. Neurol. 62, 493–503 10.1002/ana.21108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Fong K. N., Lo P. C., Yu Y. S., Cheuk C. K., Tsang T. H., Po A. S., et al. (2011). Effects of sensory cueing on voluntary arm use for patients with chronic stroke: a preliminary study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 92, 15–23 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.09.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Forssberg H., Eliasson A. C., Kinoshita H., Johansson R. S., Westling G. (1991). Development of human precision grip. I: basic coordination of forces. Exp. Brain Res. 85, 451–457 10.1007/BF00229422 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Forssberg H., Eliasson A. C., Kinoshita H., Johansson R. S., Westling G. (1992). Development of human precision grip. II. Anticipatory control of isometric forces targeted for object’s weight. Exp. Brain Res. 90, 393–398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Forssberg H., Eliasson A. C., Kinoshita H., Westling G., Johansson R. S. (1995). Development of human precision grip. IV. Tactile adaptation of isometric finger forces to the frictional condition. Exp. Brain Res. 104, 323–330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Forssberg H., Eliasson A. C., Redon-Zouitenn C., Mercuri E., Dubowitz L. (1999). Impaired grip-lift synergy in children with unilateral brain lesions. Brain 122, 1157–1168 10.1093/brain/122.6.1157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fugl-Meyer A. R., Jääskö L., Leyman I., Olsson S., Steglind S. (1975). The post-stroke hemiplegic patient 1. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med. 7, 13–31 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Gordon A. M., Bleyenheuft Y., Steenbergen B. (2013). Pathophysiology of impaired hand function in children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 55(Suppl. 4), 32–37 10.1111/dmcn.12304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gordon A. M., Charles J., Duff S. V. (1999). Fingertip forces during object manipulation in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. II: bilateral coordination. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 41, 176–185 10.1017/S0012162299000365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Gordon A. M., Charles J., Steenbergen B. (2006). Fingertip force planning during grasp is disrupted by impaired sensorimotor integration in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Pediatr. Res. 60, 587–591 10.1203/01.pdr.0000242370.41469.74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Gordon A. M., Chinnan A., Gill S., Petra E., Hung Y. C., Charles J. (2008). Both constraint-induced movement therapy and bimanual training lead to improved performance of upper extremity function in children with hemiplegia. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 50, 957–958 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03166.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Gordon A. M., Duff S. V. (1999). Fingertip forces during object manipulation in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. I: anticipatory scaling. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 41, 166–175 10.1017/S0012162299000365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gordon A. M., Forssberg H., Johansson R. S., Eliasson A. C., Westling G. (1992). Development of human precision grip. III. Integration of visual size cues during the programming of isometric forces. Exp. Brain Res. 90, 399–403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gordon A. M., Friel K. (2009). “Intensive training of upper extremity function in children with cerebral palsy,” in Sensorimotor Control of Grasping: Physiology and Pathophysiology, eds Hermsdoerfer J., Nowak D. A. (Cambridge University Press; ), 438–457 [Google Scholar]
  55. Gordon A. M., Lewis S. R., Eliasson A. C., Duff S. V. (2003). Object release under varying task constraints in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 45, 240–248 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2003.tb00338.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gordon A. M., Schneider J. A., Chinnan A., Charles J. R. (2007). Efficacy of a hand-arm bimanual intensive therapy (HABIT) in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a randomized control trial. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 830–838 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00830.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gordon A. M., Westling G., Cole K. J., Johansson R. S. (1993). Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 1789–1796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Hanna S. E., Rosenbaum P. L., Bartlett D. J., Palisano R. J., Walter S. D., Avery L., et al. (2009). Stability and decline in gross motor function among children and youth with cerebral palsy aged 2 to 21 years. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 51, 295–302 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03196.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Harris J. E., Eng J. J. (2007). Paretic upper-limb strength best explains arm activity in people with stroke. Phys. Ther. 87, 88–97 10.2522/ptj.20060065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Harris J. E., Eng J. J. (2010). Strength training improves upper-limb function in individuals with stroke: a meta-analysis. Stroke 41, 136–140 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.567438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hayner K., Gibson G., Giles G. M. (2010). Comparison of constraint-induced movement therapy and bilateral treatment of equal intensity in people with chronic upper-extremity dysfunction after cerebrovascular accident. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 64, 528–539 10.5014/ajot.2010.08027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Helliwell S. (2009). Does the use of a sensory re-education programme improve the somatosensory and motor function of the upper limb in subacute stroke? A single case experimental design. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 72, 551–558 10.4276/030802209X12601857794853 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hermsdörfer J., Hagl E., Nowak D. A. (2004). Deficits of anticipatory grip force control after damage to peripheral and central sensorimotor systems. Hum. Mov. Sci. 23, 643–662 10.1016/j.humov.2004.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hermsdörfer J., Hagl E., Nowak D. A., Marquardt C. (2003). Grip force control during object manipulation in cerebral stroke. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 915–929 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00042-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hung Y. C., Charles J., Gordon A. M. (2004). Bimanual coordination during a goal-directed task in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 46, 746–753 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2004.tb00994.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ietswaart M., Johnston M., Dijkerman H. C., Joice S., Scott C. L., MacWalter R. S., et al. (2011). Hamilton Mental practice with motor imagery in stroke recovery: randomized controlled trial of efficacy. Brain 134, 1373–1386 10.1093/brain/awr077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Islam M., Gordon A. M., Sköld A., Forssberg H., Eliasson A. C. (2011). Grip force coordination during bimanual tasks in unilateral cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 53, 920–926 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04040.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Johansson R. S. (1996). “Sensory control of dexterous manipulation in humans,” in Hand and Brain: The Neurophysiology and Psychology of Hand Movements, eds Wing A. M., Haggard P., Flanagan J. R. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 381–414 [Google Scholar]
  69. Johansson R. S. (2002). Dynamic use of tactile afferent signals in control of dexterous manipulation. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 508, 397–410 10.1007/978-1-4615-0713-0_45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Johansson R. S., Westling G. (1984). Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Exp. Brain Res. 56, 550–564 10.1007/BF00237997 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Johansson R. S., Westling G. (1987). Signals in tactile afferents from the fingers eliciting adaptive motor responses during precision grip. Exp. Brain Res. 66, 141–154 10.1007/BF00236210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Johansson R. S., Westling G. (1988a). Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip. Exp. Brain Res. 71, 59–71 10.1007/BF00247522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Jones L. (1996). “Propriception and its contribution to manual dexterity,” in Hand and Brain, eds Wing A. W., Haggard P., Flanagan J. R. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 349–362 [Google Scholar]
  74. Kong K. H., Chua K. S., Lee J. (2011). Recovery of upper limb dexterity in patients more than 1 year after stroke: frequency, clinical correlates and predictors. Neurorehabilitation 28, 105–111 10.3233/NRE-2011-0639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lanin N. A., Novak I., Cusick A. (2007). A systematic review of upper extremity casting for children and adults with central nervous system motor disorders. Clin. Rehabil. 21, 963–976 10.1177/0269215507079141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lefebvre S., Thonnard J. L., Laloux P., Peeters A., Jamart J., Vandermeeren Y. (2014). Single session of dual-tDCS transiently improves precision grip and dexterity of the paretic hand after stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 28, 100–110 10.1177/1545968313478485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mackenzie S. J., Getchell N., Modlesky C. M., Miller F., Jaric S. (2009). Using grasping tasks to evaluate hand force coordination in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 90, 1439–1442 10.1016/j.apmr.2009.02.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. McCormick A., Brien M., Plourde J., Wood E., Rosenbaum P., McLean J. (2007). Stability of the gross motor function classification system in adults with cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 265–269 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00265.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. McDonnell M. N., Hillier S. L., Ridding M. C., Miles T. S. (2006). Impairments in precision grip correlate with functional measures in adult hemiplegia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 1474–1480 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.02.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Merians A. S., Fluet G. G., Qiu Q., Saleh S., Lafond I., Davidow A., et al. (2011). Adamovich robotically facilitated virtual rehabilitation of arm transport integrated with finger movement in persons with hemiparesis. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 8, 27. 10.1186/1743-0003-8-27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Mesterman R., Leitner Y., Yifat R., Gilutz G., Levi-Hakeini O., Bitchonsky O., et al. (2010). Cerebral palsy – long-term medical, functional, educational, and psychosocial outcomes. J. Child Neurol. 25, 36–42 10.1177/0883073809336677 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Michielsen M. E., Selles R. W., van der Geest J. N., Eckhardt M., Yavuzer G., Stam H. J., et al. (2011). Motor recovery and cortical reorganization after mirror therapy in chronic stroke patients: a phase II randomized controlled trial. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 25, 223–233 10.1177/1545968310385127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Moberg E. (1958). Objective methods for determining the functional value of sensibility in the hand. J. Bone Joint Surg. Br. 40-B, 454–476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mochizuki H., Huang Y. Z., Rothwell J. C. (2004). Interhemispheric interaction between human dorsal premotor and contralateral primary motor cortex. J. Physiol. 15, 331–338 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.072843 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Montané E., Vallano A., Laporte J. R. (2004). Oral antispastic drugs in nonprogressive neurologic diseases: a systematic review. Neurology 63, 1357–1363 10.1212/01.WNL.0000141863.52691.44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Müller F., Dichgans J. (1994). Impairments of precision grip in two patients with acute unilateral cerebellar lesions: a simple parametric test for clinical use. Neuropsychologia 32, 265–269 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90012-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Müller K., Bütefisch C. M., Seitz R. J., Hömberg V. (2007). Mental practice improves hand function after hemiparetic stroke. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 501–511 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Murphy C. C., Yeargin-Allsopp M., Decouflé P., Drews C. D. (1993). Prevalence of cerebral palsy among ten-year-old children in metropolitan Atlanta, 1985 through 1987. J. Pediatr. 123, S13–S20 10.1016/S0022-3476(05)80892-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Mutsaarts M., Steenbergen B., Bekkering H. (2006). Anticipatory planning deficits and task context effects in hemiparetic cerebral palsy. Exp. Brain Res. 172, 151–162 10.1007/s00221-005-0327-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Naik S. K., Patten C., Lodha N., Coombes S. A., Cauraugh J. H. (2011). Force control deficits in chronic stroke: grip formation and release phases. Exp. Brain Res. 211, 1–15 10.1007/s00221-011-2637-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Newton J. M., Ward N. S., Parker G. J., Deichmann R., Alexander D. C., Friston K. J., et al. (2006). Non-invasive mapping of corticofugal fibers from multiple motor areas – relevance to stroke recovery. Brain 129, 1844–1858 10.1093/brain/awl106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Nijland R. H., van Wegen E. E., Harmeling-van der Wel B. C., Kwakkel G., EPOS Investigators (2010). Presence of finger extension and shoulder abduction within 72 hours after stroke predicts functional recovery: early prediction of functional outcome after stroke: the EPOS cohort study. Stroke 41, 745–750 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.572065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Nilsen D. M., Gillen G., Gordon A. M. (2010). The use of mental practice to improve upper limb recovery post-stroke: a systematic review. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 64, 695–708 10.5014/ajot.2010.09034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Nowak D. A. (2008). The impact of stroke on the performance of grasping: usefulness of kinetic and kinematic motion analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1439–1450 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Nowak D. A., Grefkes C., Dafotakis M., Küst J., Karbe H., Fink G. R. (2007). Dexterity is impaired at both hands following unilateral subcortical middle cerebral artery stroke. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3173–3184 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05551.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Nowak D. A., Hermsdörfer J., Topka H. (2003). Deficits of predictive grip force control during object manipulation in acute stroke. J. Neurol. 250, 850–860 10.1007/s00415-003-1095-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Peurala S. H., Pitkänen K., Sivenius J., Tarkka I. M. (2002). Cutaneous electrical stimulation may enhance sensorimotor recovery in chronic stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 16, 709–716 10.1191/0269215502cr543oa [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Prabhu S. B., Diermayr G., Gysin P., Gordon A. M. (2011). Coordination of fingertip forces in object transport during gait in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 53, 865–869 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04061.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Prasad G., Herman P., Coyle D., McDonough S., Crosbie J. (2010). Applying a brain-computer interface to support motor imagery practice in people with stroke for upper limb recovery: a feasibility study. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 7, 60. 10.1186/1743-0003-7-60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Quaney B. M., He J., Timberlake G., Dodd K., Carr C. (2010). Visuomotor training improves stroke-related ipsilesional upper extremity impairments. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 24, 52–61 10.1177/1545968309341646 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Quaney B. M., Perera S., Maletsky R., Luchies C. W., Nudo R. J. (2005). Impaired grip force modulation in the ipsilesional hand after unilateral middle cerebral artery stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 19, 338–349 10.1177/1545968305282269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Ragert P., Vandermeeren Y., Camus M., Cohen L. G. (2008). Improvement of spatial tactile acuity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 805–811 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Raghavan P. (2007). The nature of hand motor impairment after stroke and its treatment. Curr. Treat. Options Cardiovasc. Med. 9, 221–228 10.1007/s11936-007-0016-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Raghavan P., Krakauer J. W., Gordon A. M. (2006). Impaired anticipatory control of fingertip forces in patients with a pure motor or sensorimotor lacunar syndrome. Brain 129, 1415–1425 10.1093/brain/awl070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Raghavan P., Santello M., Gordon A. M., Krakauer J. W. (2010). Compensatory motor control after stroke: an alternative joint strategy for object-dependent shaping of hand posture. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 3034–3043 10.1152/jn.00936.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Rehme A. K., Fink G. R., von Cramon D. Y., Grefkes C. (2011a). The role of the contralesional motor cortex for motor recovery in the early days after stroke assessed with longitudinal FMRI. Cereb. Cortex 21, 756–768 10.1093/cercor/bhq140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Rehme A. K., Eickhoff S. B., Wang L. E., Fink G. R., Grefkes C. (2011b). Dynamic causal modeling of cortical activity from the acute to the chronic stage after stroke. Neuroimage 55, 1147–1158 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Roebroeck M. E., Jahnsen R., Carona C., Kent R. M., Chamberlain M. A. (2009). Adult outcomes and lifespan issues for people with childhood-onset physical disability. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 51, 670–678 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03322.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Rouiller E. M., Babalian A., Kazennikov O., Moret V., Yu X. H., Wiesendanger M. (1994). Transcallosal connections of the distal forelimb representations of the primary and supplementary motor cortical areas in macaque monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 102, 227–243 10.1007/BF00227511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Sakzewski L., Ziviani J., Boyd R. (2009). Systematic review and meta-analysis of therapeutic management of upper-limb dysfunction in children with congenital hemiplegia. Pediatrics 123, 1111–1122 10.1542/peds.2008-3335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Sawaki L., Butler A. J., Leng X., Wassenaar P. A., Mohammad Y. M., Blanton S., et al. (2008). Constraint-induced movement therapy results in increased motor map area in subjects 3 to 9 months after stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 22, 505–513 10.1177/1545968308317531 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Schabrun S. M., Hillier S. (2009). Evidence for the retraining of sensation after stroke: a systematic review. Clin. Rehabil. 23, 27–39 10.1177/0269215508098897 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Seo N. J., Fischer H. W., Bogey R. A., Rymer W. Z., Kamper D. G. (2011). Use of visual force feedback to improve digit force direction during pinch grip in persons with stroke: a pilot study. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 92, 24–30 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.08.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Seo N. J., Rymer W. Z., Kamper D. G. (2010). Altered digit force direction during pinch grip following stroke. Exp. Brain Res. 202, 891–901 10.1007/s00221-010-2193-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Shirahashi I., Matsumoto S., Shimodozono M., Etoh S., Kawahira K. (2007). Functional vibratory stimulation on the hand facilitates voluntary movements of a hemiplegic upper limb in a patient with stroke. Int. J. Rehabil. Res. 30, 227–230 10.1097/MRR.0b013e32829fa4b6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Smania N., Montagnana B., Faccioli S., Fiaschi A., Aglioti S. M. (2003). Rehabilitation of somatic sensation and related deficit of motor control in patients with pure sensory stroke. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 84, 1692–1702 10.1053/S0003-9993(03)00277-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Smits-Engelsman B. C., Klingels K., Feys H. (2011). Bimanual force coordination in children with spastic unilateral cerebral palsy. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 2011–2019 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Smits-Engelsman B. C., Rameckers E. A., Duysens J. (2004). Late developmental deficits in force control in children with hemiplegia. Neuroreport 15, 1931–1935 10.1097/00001756-200408260-00020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Stanley F., Blair E., Alberman E. (2000). Cerebral Palsies: Epidemiology and Causal Pathways. Clinics in Developmental Medicine No 151. London: Mc Keith Press [Google Scholar]
  120. Staudt M., Gerloff C., Grodd W., Holthausen H., Niemann G., Krageloh-Mann I. (2004). Reorganization in congenital hemiparesis acquired at different gestational ages. Ann. Neurol. 56, 854–863 10.1002/ana.20297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Steenbergen B., Charles J., Gordon A. M. (2008). Fingertip force control during bimanual object lifting in hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Exp. Brain Res. 186, 191–201 10.1007/s00221-007-1223-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Steenbergen B., Gordon A. M. (2006). Activity limitation in hemiplegic cerebral palsy: evidence for disorders in motor planning. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 48, 780–783 10.1017/S0012162206001666 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Steenbergen B., Hulstijn W., Lemmens I. H., Meulenbroek R. G. (1998). The timing of prehensile movements in subjects with cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 40, 108–114 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1998.tb15370.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Sullivan J. E., Hedman L. D. (2007). Effects of home-based sensory and motor amplitude electrical stimulation on arm dysfunction in chronic stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 21, 142–150 10.1177/0269215506071252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Takahashi C. D., Reinkensmeyer D. J. (2003). Hemiparetic stroke impairs anticipatory control of arm movement. Exp. Brain Res. 149, 131–140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Thrasher T. A., Zivanovic V., McIlroy W., Popovic M. R. (2008). Rehabilitation of reaching and grasping function in severe hemiplegic patients using functional electrical stimulation therapy. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 22, 706–714 10.1177/1545968308317436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Valvano J., Newell K. M. (1998). Practice of a precision isometric grip-force task by children with spastic cerebral palsy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 40, 464–473 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1998.tb15397.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. van Elk M., Crajé C., Beeren M. E. G. V., Steenbergen B., van Schie H. T., Bekkering H. (2010). Neural evidence for compromised motor imagery in right hemiparetic cerebral palsy. Front Neurol. 1:150. 10.3389/fneur.2010.00150 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Voller B., Flöel A., Werhahn K. J., Ravindran S., Wu C. W., Cohen L. G. (2006). Contralateral hand anesthesia transiently improves poststroke sensory deficits. Ann. Neurol. 59, 385–388 10.1002/ana.20689 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Ward N. S., Cohen L. G. (2004). Mechanisms underlying recovery of motor function after stroke. Arch. Neurol. 61, 1844–1848 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Wenzelburger R., Kopper F., Frenzel A., Stolze H., Klebe S., Brossmann A., et al. (2005). Hand coordination following capsular stroke. Brain 128(Pt 1), 64–74 10.1093/brain/awh317 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Westlake K. P., Nagarajan S. S. (2011). Functional connectivity in relation to motor performance and recovery after stroke. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:8. 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Westling G., Johansson R. S. (1987). Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision grip in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 66, 128–140 10.1007/BF00236209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Witney A. G., Wing A., Thonnard J. L., Smith A. M. (2004). The cutaneous contribution to adaptive precision grip. Trends Neurosci. 27, 637–643 10.1016/j.tins.2004.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Wolpert D. M., Flanagan J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Curr. Biol. 11, R729–R732 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Wolpert D. M., Ghahramani Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 3(Suppl.), 1212–1217 10.1038/81497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Wolpert D. M., Miall C., Kawato M. (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 338–347 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01221-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Yekutiel M., Guttman E. (1993). A controlled trial of the retraining of the sensory function of the hand in stroke patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 56, 241–244 10.1136/jnnp.56.3.241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Ziherl J., Novak D., Olenšek A., Mihelj M., Munih M. (2010). Evaluation of upper extremity robot-assistances in subacute and chronic stroke subjects. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 7, 52. 10.1186/1743-0003-7-52 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES