Abstract
The claustrum is a subcortical nucleus present in all placental mammals. Many anatomical studies have shown that its inputs are predominantly from the cerebral cortex and its outputs are back to the cortex. This connectivity thus suggests that the claustrum serves to amplify or facilitate information processing in the cerebral cortex. The size and the complexity of the cerebral cortex varies dramatically across species. Some species have lissencephalic brains, with few cortical areas, while others have a greatly expanded cortex and many cortical areas. This evolutionary diversity in the cerebral cortex raises several questions about the claustrum. Does its volume expand in coordination with the expansion of cortex and does it acquire new functions related to the new cortical functions? Here we survey the organization of the claustrum in animals with large brains, including great apes and cetaceans. Our data suggest that the claustrum is not always a continuous structure. In monkeys and gorillas there are a few isolated islands of cells near the main body of the nucleus. In cetaceans, however, there are many isolated cell islands. These data suggest constraints on the possible function of the claustrum. Some authors propose that the claustrum has a more global role in perception or consciousness that requires intraclaustral integration of information. These theories postulate mechanisms like gap junctions between claustral cells or a “syncytium” to mediate intraclaustral processing. The presence of discontinuities in the structure of the claustrum, present but minimal in some primates, but dramatically clear in cetaceans, argues against the proposed mechanisms of intraclaustral processing of information. The best interpretation of function, then, is that each functional subdivision of the claustrum simply contributes to the function of its cortical partner.
Keywords: gorilla, whale, dolphin, calcium-binding proteins, visual cortex
The claustrum is a subcortical nucleus described in all placental mammals. Its structure, physiology, connections and neurochemistry have been studied in multiple species (references in Sherk, 1986; Buchanan and Johnson, 2011; Baizer, 2014). Classification schemes vary, but the claustrum is typically divided into two subdivisions, a dorsal and a ventral part, sometimes called the endopiriform nucleus (summary in Baizer, 2014). This paper will focus on the dorsal claustrum. Many studies show that the dorsal claustrum is functionally linked to the cerebral cortex, but the fundamental question of the function of the claustrum is still unanswered. We will consider first what is known about the organization and function of the claustrum from anatomical and electrophysiological studies in the cat and the macaque monkey. We will then ask what the comparative literature on the claustrum suggests about its function. We will supplement existing studies with data on the size and shape of the claustrum in a great ape, the western lowland gorilla, and from two cetaceans, the bottlenose dolphin and the humpback whale. Finally, we will summarize the implications of the comparative neuroanatomy of the claustrum for hypotheses about its function.
The dorsal claustrum in the cat and the macaque monkey
Many anatomical studies in cat and monkey have shown that the cerebral cortex projects to the claustrum and the claustrum projects back to the cerebral cortex (Druga, 1966, 1968, 1971; Künzle, 1975; Riche and Lanoir, 1978; Turner et al., 1980; LeVay and Sherk, 1981a; Macchi et al., 1981, 1983; Mizuno et al., 1981; Pearson et al., 1982; Bullier et al., 1984; Ungerleider et al., 1984; Weber and Yin, 1984; Kennedy and Bullier, 1985; Guldin et al., 1986; LeVay, 1986; McCourt et al., 1986; Perkel et al., 1986; Sloniewski et al., 1986; Hinova-Palova et al., 1988; Minciacchi et al., 1991, 1995; Boussaoud et al., 1992; Clascá et al., 1992; Morecraft et al., 1992; Baizer et al., 1993, 1997; Tokuno and Tanji, 1993; Tanne-Gariepy et al., 2002; Miyashita et al., 2005; Smith and Alloway, 2010). While there have been suggestions of subcortical connections of the claustrum, data from different studies are inconsistent and contradictory. For example, LeVay and Sherk (1981a), performed extensive experiments using both retrograde and anterograde tracers in the cat, and concluded that “no subcortical projections from the claustrum could be identified”. In contrast, Amaral and Cowan (1980) reported a projection from the claustrum to the hippocampus in the macaque monkey. Similarly, Arikuni and Kubota (1985) reported a projection from claustrum to the caudate nucleus in the macaque monkey, while Saint-Cyr et al. (1990) did not find such a projection. Furthermore, in the most detailed anatomical study of the claustrum LeVay and Sherk (1981a) found that a large injection of retrograde tracer in visual cortex labeled 87% of the cells in the visual claustrum. The unlabeled cells were argued, on the basis of cell size, to represent local interneurons (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a). The weight of the evidence, at present, argues that the major, and possibly only, target of claustral projections is the cortex, and that the major input to the claustrum is from the cortex.
The connections between cortex and claustrum are topographic, with different cortical functional regions connected to dedicated claustrum territories (reviews in Sherk, 1986; Baizer, 2014). The cerebral cortex is characterized by both cytoarchitectural and neurochemical subdivisions, many of which correlate with functional regions (examples and references in Von Bonin and Bailey, 1947; Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Geyer et al., 1996). However, cytoarchitectural and neurochemical analysis of the claustrum shows structural uniformity with no evidence of structurally- defined subdivisions (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a; Reynhout and Baizer, 1999; Baizer, 2001; Rahman and Baizer, 2007).
The claustrum of the cat extends over about 11 mm rostrocaudally (Snider and Niemer, 1961). Figure 1 illustrates the claustrum of the cat on three sections showing its very different configuration at different rostro-caudal levels. Figure 1A shows a section at about the center of the claustrum, the locus of the large dorsal enlargement in which the visual region is found (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a,b). Figure 1B shows that neurons in the triangular part of the cat claustrum have round or oval somata. Figures 1C,D illustrate the shape of the claustrum at its caudal and rostral limits.
Anatomical data suggest that there is also a visual region of the macaque monkey claustrum that may contain more than one map of the visual field (Baizer et al., 1993, 1997). Figures 2A–C shows three sections at different rostral-levels of the macaque monkey claustrum. As in the cat, the shape of the claustrum changes with rostro-caudal level. Figure 3 shows the neurons in the claustrum of the macaque monkey at different dorso-ventral levels. Figures 3C,D show that in the long, thin ascending stem of the claustrum many neurons have cell bodies elongated parallel to the long axis of the claustrum (Figure 3D), whereas in wider regions neurons have oval/polymorphic somata (Figure 3B). These differences in soma shape are presumably mirrored by differences in the shapes and extents of dendritic arbors. Further, the long dorsal stem of the claustrum can be discontinuous, with cell sparse regions (Figure 3C, large arrowhead). Interestingly, there is a major difference in the organization of the claustrum between macaque monkey and cat. In both species, the claustrum consists of a long thin stem with an enlargement but this enlargement is dorsally in the cat and ventrally the monkey. In both cat and macaque monkey, the visual claustrum is found within the enlarged region (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a,b; Baizer et al., 1993, 1997).
Cellular organization of the claustrum
Studies in several species identify a population of projection neurons in the claustrum; these neurons are also the targets of descending projections from the neocortex (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a). In addition, there are populations of local interneurons. These were first identified by Golgi impregnation studies (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a; Mamos et al., 1986). Later studies showed that different populations of interneurons are immunoreactive for different calcium-binding proteins (Reynhout and Baizer, 1999; Rahman and Baizer, 2007; Baizer, 2014). Figures 4A–C shows neurons immunoreactive for calbindin (CB; A), calretinin (CR, B) and parvalbumin (PV, C) in the claustrum of the cat. Figures 4D–F show neurons in the claustrum of the macaque monkey immunoreactive for CB (D), CR (E) and PV (F). These interneurons provide a substrate for local information processing within the claustrum.
Theories of the function of the claustrum
There are two very different views of the function of the claustrum. One is that the claustrum is divided into independent functional zones defined by cortical connections and that each zone then influences only its cortical partner. This view derives from anatomical and physiological studies of the visual claustrum of the cat (Olson and Graybiel, 1980; LeVay and Sherk, 1981a,b; Macchi et al., 1981; Sherk and Levay, 1981; Boyapati and Henry, 1985; LeVay, 1986; Updyke, 1993; Minciacchi et al., 1995; Pérez-Cerdá et al., 1996). There is a visuotopic map of the contralateral visual hemifield in the part of the claustrum connected with visual cortex (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a,b; Sherk and Levay, 1981). Neurons in this region have exclusively visual responses and receptive field properties similar to those of cells in V1 (LeVay and Sherk, 1981b; Sherk and Levay, 1981). By this view, it is not possible to define a single function for the claustrum, each functional subdivision would affect the function of its cortical counterpart, which could be sensory or motor or cognitive or affective. Other authors, however, have proposed a more integrative and global role for the claustrum, envisioning that it mediates consciousness (Crick and Koch, 2005) or perceptual integration across sensory modalities (Smythies et al., 2012). These ideas require the integration of information across different functional subdivisions of the claustrum. Anatomical studies did not find evidence for projections among subdivisions of the claustrum (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a), leading to the suggestion of novel mechanisms for intraclaustral information processing. Interneurons in the claustrum are critical for these mechanisms, which include gap junctions among interneurons (Crick and Koch, 2005), and/or dendrodendritic chemical synapses among claustral cells (Crick and Koch, 2005). A related proposal is that the interneurons of the claustrum form an “interactive gap-junction syncytium” (Smythies et al., 2014). These mechanisms require that the claustrum is a continuous structure with neurons in close proximity to each other, that “a densely packed amorphous syncytium [that] constitutes the interior of the claustrum (Smythies et al., 2014). This idea of the interior structure of the claustrum also implies a uniformity of arrangements of dendritic trees and spacing of neurons throughout the claustrum. Examination of the fine structure of the claustrum should allow some evaluation of the plausibility of these mechanisms.
Another implication of the more global theories of claustral function is that the claustrum should increase in importance over evolution. This hypothesis could be examined by quantitative comparative analysis of the relative sizes of claustrum and the cerebral cortex.
Evolution, the claustrum, and the cortex
Because of the anatomical interdependence of cerebral cortex and claustrum, phylogenetic variation in the organization of the claustrum must be considered in the context of the organization of the cortex. The configuration of the cerebral cortex varies widely among mammals (images of the brains of many different species are shown at http://www.brainmuseum.org). There are many species-specific functional specializations of cortical areas. For example, the maps of the body in sensory and motor cortex adapt to reflect functional specializations in different species, e.g., a representation of the trunk in the somatosensory and motor cortex of the elephant or of high frequency sounds in the auditory cortex of bats (representative studies Edamatsu and Suga, 1993; Fitzpatrick et al., 1993, 1998; Esser et al., 1997; Xiao and Suga, 2004). The size and the complexity of the cerebral cortex change dramatically over evolution. Many rodents are lissencephalic with a relatively small number of cortical areas. By contrast, many anthropoid primates, including humans, have a greatly expanded cerebral cortex with considerable gyrification and an increased number of cortical areas (Agulhon et al., 1998).
Species differences in cortical organization are mirrored in differences in the size and shape of the claustrum. Buchanan and Johnson (2011) illustrate the shape and size of both cortex and claustrum in 26 different species. Kowiański et al. (1999) illustrated the shape of the claustrum in different species. They recognized five different morphological types; the shape of the dorsal claustrum in 8 different species is illustrated in Figure 5 (modified from Kowiański et al., 1999). It is important to note, however, that while all of these drawings show the claustrum as a continuous structure, examination of Nissl sections from the macaque monkey at higher magnification (as in Figure 3C) shows discontinuities. Figures 6A,B supplement the drawings of the human claustrum with photomicrographs of the human claustrum (at arrows) in Nissl-stained coronal sections of the human brain. The image in A shows a hint of a ventral enlargement as seen in macaque monkey; the image in B supports dorsal-ventral symmetry. A ventral enlargement is present in small-brain lissencephalic New World anthropoid primates such as owl monkeys, squirrel monkeys, titi monkeys, tamarins and marmosets, although it is not as pronounced in the strepsirrhine primates, the lemurs and lorises. However, while illustrations can show species variability in the form of the claustrum, they cannot relate the differences in the shape and size of the claustrum to differences in the numbers or organization or specializations of cortical areas.
In addition to illustrating the species differences in the appearance of the claustrum, Kowiański et al. (1999) asked a critical question: do the claustrum and the cerebral cortex expand in parallel? They calculated the ratio of the volume of the claustrum to the volume of the cerebral hemispheres in several species; the data showed that the volume of the claustrum does not increase proportionately at the same rate to the volume of the cerebral cortex (Kowiański et al., 1999). Of the species they examined, the ratio of the volume of the claustrum to the volume of the isocortex was highest in mice (6.5%) and lowest in humans (0.45%). These data suggest that as the cerebral cortex expands the relative size of the claustrum actually decreases.
Both the qualitative and quantitative analyses of the claustrum in human raise questions about the similarity of its organization to that of other species. The shape of the claustrum in the human is strikingly different from that of the rodents, the cat and most primate species. Does the claustrum in human connect only with the “older” cortical structures, i.e., the primary sensory and motor areas or are there parts of the claustrum that are more related to association cortical areas and their functions? Is the claustrum in humans structurally uniform as it is in animals? In humans, there is lateralization of both language and handedness and this lateralization is also mirrored by structural asymmetries (Steinmetz et al., 1989; Jäncke et al., 1994; Steinmetz, 1996; Westbury et al., 1999; Pujol et al., 2002). Might this lateralization be reflected in left-right asymmetries in the claustrum? Another characteristic of cerebral cortex in humans, great apes and other primates with gyrified brains is individual variability in the pattern of cortical sulci and gyri (Steinmetz, 1996; Westbury et al., 1999). Is this individual variability mirrored in individual differences in claustrum morphology?
There are thus many questions about the organization and function of the claustrum in humans. How may these questions be addressed? Direct studies of connections and function using invasive techniques cannot be undertaken in humans or other great apes. However, descriptive anatomy based on postmortem tissue, as well as imaging studies, are powerful techniques that can be used in human.
There are studies of the cell types in the human claustrum (Braak and Braak, 1982; Spahn and Braak, 1985; Hinova-Palova et al., 2013). The anatomical data in general support the idea of the presence of both projection neurons and interneurons, and overall structural uniformity within the claustrum.
There are also imaging studies of the human claustrum using a variety of techniques. One study suggests that the claustrum in humans, as in other species, is interconnected with many regions of the cerebral cortex (Milardi et al., 2013). That study also showed both individual variability and left-right differences in claustral volumes, in keeping with findings for the human cerebral cortex. There were also sex differences in claustral volume, with males having a larger volume than females. This may reflect overall sex differences in brain size (Leonard et al., 2008; Luders et al., 2009) (One caution about the results of this study: a pathway between claustrum and basal ganglia was also found, such a pathway was not described in the most detailed experimental study of the connections of the claustrum (LeVay and Sherk, 1981a)).
There are many imaging studies that suggest a role of the claustrum in rather diverse functions. At this point, these studies must be interpreted with caution, as the imaging resolution may not yet be sufficient to distinguish activation of the claustrum, which in humans is very thin, from that in surrounding brain regions like the cerebral cortex and the putamen. Imaging resolution is certainly not yet able to resolve activation differences within different functional zones of the claustrum itself. For example, Wegiel et al. (2014, p. 227) list 10 functions in which the claustrum has been implicated including “experiential dread” and “suppression of natural urges”. In addition to the studies listed by Wegiel et al. (2014) there is a PET imaging study that suggested a role for the claustrum in sexual function (Redouté et al., 2000; Figure 3), an fMRI study linked the claustrum to ADHD (Wang et al., 2013), another to aesthetic judgment (Ishizu and Zeki, 2013) and yet another suggested pathology of the claustrum in bipolar disorder (Selvaraj et al., 2012). Increased resolution of imaging techniques may clarify the role of the claustrum in these, and other, as yet unstudied, functions.
The claustrum in the gorilla
The drawings in Figure 5 suggest that the human claustrum has more dorsal-ventral symmetry than seen in other species. In order to see if this is unique to humans or is a general feature of the brains of great apes, we examined the claustrum in the gorilla. Figure 7F shows a lateral view of the gorilla brain; its rostro-caudal extent is about 130 mm. There is a more complex pattern of sulci and gyri than seen in the macaque monkey. Examination of cresyl violet-stained sections showed that the claustrum was present over about 33 mm in the rostro-caudal direction. Figures 7A–E show five outline drawings of the claustrum at different rostro-caudal levels. The claustrum is very elongated in the dorso-ventral direction, extending as far as 20 mm (Figure 7D); it is also very thin. Comparison of the claustrum in the gorilla (Figure 7) and the human (Figures 5, 6) shows that the claustrum in the gorilla is much longer and narrower than in the human. Strikingly, at some levels, the claustrum is not a continuous structure; Figure 7A shows an isolated cluster of neurons in the dorsal part of the claustrum. Figures 8A,C show lower magnification photomicrographs of the claustrum on two different sections, one at a more dorsal level and the other at a more ventral level. Again the isolated clusters of cells are apparent (Figure 8A). The shapes of the somata also vary with location in the claustrum. Figures 8B,D shows higher magnification images showing the numbers, shape, and density of stained neurons. Figure 8B shows somata that are elongated roughly parallel to the dorso-ventral axis. Figure 8D shows larger stained somata that are polygonal with no preferred orientation. These differences in soma shape probably correlate with differences in the shape and extent of the dendritic trees, differences not considered in the “gap junction syncytium” hypothesis. Since the brains for the cat and monkey were not prepared in the same way (celloidin vs. frozen sections) meaningful comparisons of neuronal size or neuronal packing density among these species cannot be made.
The claustrum in cetaceans
The drawings in Figure 5 do not include all of the forms that the claustrum can take. In the elephant, the claustrum is elongated toward the frontal pole of the cortex and appears as cell islands just under the cortex (see Figure 2 in Hakeem et al., 2009). Cell islands are also found in cetaceans, animals with well developed and highly folded cerebral cortices. Figure 9A shows the highly folded cortex in the brain of the bottlenose dolphin; arrows indicate the locations of cell islands comprising the claustrum. Figure 9B shows claustral islands at higher magnification and Figure 9C shows the cellular organization of one such island. It is not clear from the anatomy, of course, if the islands represent fragmented functional subdivisions (as would be the case, for example, if the the visual claustrum of the cat were broken up into isolated cell clusters) or if each island is a self-contained unit. Figure 10A shows the highly folded cerebral cortex of the whale on a parasagittal section; the arrows show the very scattered islands of claustral cells. Figures 1B,C show these cell clusters in more detail.
Conclusions: what is the function of the claustrum?
The comparative data summarized here argue against the hypothesis that the claustrum has an important role in global perceptual integration and/or consciousness. First, examination of the shape of the claustrum in different species shows considerable variability, with several species having very long, thin claustral “stems” that would seem to impose constraints on the proposed mechanisms of intraclaustral processing. These notions of intraclaustral processing also require that the claustrum is a continuous structure, and that clearly is not the case in a number of species. We have shown sections in both macaque monkey and gorilla in which the claustrum is characterized by separated cell clusters. Claustral fragmentation is seen even more dramatically in the bottlenose dolphin and humpback whale. Another argument against a major integrative role for the claustrum, however, comes from the quantitative analysis showing its decreased relative size (as indicated by the ratio of volumes) as cerebral cortex expands (Kowiański et al., 1999). Rather than suggesting a global role for the claustrum, the comparative data reinforce the early view that each functional division of the claustrum is independent from the others, and works with the cortical area with which it is interconnected. By this view, the function of each subdivision would be to enhance or modulate the function of its cortical partner. Thus, the anatomical data do not support a more global role of the claustrum. The fact that expansion of the claustrum does not parallel cortical expansion suggests that the function of amplifying or adjusting cortical excitability was of greater importance in lissencephalic animals, and that the importance of claustrum for cortical operations has decreased as cortical complexity has increased.
Materials and methods
Tissue and histology
Cat, macaque monkey
We photographed archival celloidin-embedded cresyl violet stained sections of the cat and macaque monkey claustrum. These slides had been prepared in the laboratory of Dr. Mitchell Glickstein, then at Brown University. We also photographed archival immunostained sections that had been prepared in the course of earlier studies (Reynhout and Baizer, 1999; Rahman and Baizer, 2007).
Gorillas
We obtained the brains of two western lowland gorillas from the Buffalo Zoo. Gorilla 1 was a female, age at death 6 years, cause of death unknown. Gorilla 2 was a male, age at death 20 years; the cause of death was cancer. The brains were removed and stored in 10% formalin. No data on the postmortem intervals of this tissue were available. The brains were cryoprotected in 15% then 30% sucrose in 10% formalin. The brain of Gorilla 1 was blocked to include the claustrum; the brain of Gorilla 2 was divided into four equal size blocks. Frozen sections 40 μm thick were cut in the coronal plane on an American Optical sliding microtome fitted with a custom-built copper freezing platform. All sections were collected and stored in large plastic compartment boxes, 5 sections/compartment in 5% formalin in a cold room. Series of sections 2 mm apart were mounted onto gelled slides (Brain Research Laboratories, Newton, MA) and stained for Nissl substance with cresyl violet (following the protocol of LaBossiere and Glickstein, 1976).
Cetaceans
We photographed archival celloidin-embedded cresyl-violet stained sections of a bottlenose dolphin brain that had been used in earlier projects (Hof et al., 2005). We also photographed slides of the brain of a stranded humpback whale; the cerebral cortex of that animal was described in an earlier report (Hof and Van der Gucht, 2007).
Outline drawings
The outlines of the gorilla claustrum in Figure 7 were drawn from sections using MDplot software (Accustage, Shoreview, MN) with stage encoders mounted on a Leitz Dialux 20 microscope. These drawings were saved as .bmp files using the screen capture function of the software and then assembled into figures with Adobe Photoshop (Adobe, San Jose, CA).
Photography
Images (1600 × 1200 pixels) were captured with a SPOT Insight Color Mosaic camera (Diagnostic Imaging, Sterling Heights, MI) mounted on the Leitz microscope. We used Adobe Photoshop to adjust the brightness and contrast of images and to assemble and label the figures.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Supported in part by grants from the James S. McDonnell Foundation to Patrick R. Hof and Chet C. Sherwood and by the Department of Physiology and Biophysics, University at Buffalo (Joan S. Baizer). Kathryn Callan provided technical support in the analysis of the gorilla brain.
References
- Agulhon C., Charnay Y., Vallet P., Abitbol M., Kobetz A., Bertrand D., et al. (1998). Distribution of mRNA for the alpha4 subunit of the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor in the human fetal brain. Mol. Brain Res. 58, 123–131 10.1016/s0169-328x(98)00113-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amaral D. G., Cowan W. M. (1980). Subcortical afferents to the hippocampal formation in the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 189, 573–591 10.1002/cne.901890402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arikuni T., Kubota K. (1985). Claustral and amygdaloid afferents to the head of the caudate nucleus in macaque monkeys. Neurosci. Res. 2, 239–254 10.1016/0168-0102(85)90003-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baizer J. S. (2014). “The neurochemical organization of the claustrum,” in The Claustrum. Structural, Functional and Clinical Neuroscience, eds Smythies J. R., Edelstein L. R., Ramachandran V. S. (Amsterdam: Elsevier; ), 85–118 [Google Scholar]
- Baizer J. S. (2001). Serotonergic innervation of the primate claustrum. Brain Res. Bull. 55, 431–434 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00535-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baizer J. S., Desimone R., Ungerleider L. G. (1993). Comparison of subcortical connections of inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in monkeys. Vis. Neurosci. 10, 59–72 10.1017/s0952523800003229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baizer J. S., Lock T. M., Youakim M. (1997). Projections from the claustrum to the prelunate gyrus in the monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 113, 564–568 10.1007/pl00005607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boussaoud D., Desimone R., Ungerleider L. G. (1992). Subcortical connections of visual areas MST and FST in macaques. Vis. Neurosci. 9, 291–302 10.1017/s0952523800010701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyapati J., Henry G. H. (1985). The character and influence of the claustral pathway to the striate cortex of the cat. Exp. Brain Res. 61, 141–152 10.1007/bf00235629 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braak H., Braak E. (1982). Neuronal types in the claustrum of man. Anat. Embryol. 163, 447–460 10.1007/bf00305558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchanan K. J., Johnson J. I. (2011). Diversity of spatial relationships of the claustrum and insula in branches of the mammalian radiation. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1225(Suppl. 1), E30–E63 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06022.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullier J., Kennedy H., Salinger W. (1984). Bifurcation of subcortical afferents to visual areas 17, 18 and 19 in the cat cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 228, 309–328 10.1002/cne.902280303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clascá F., Avendaño C., Román-Guindo A., Llamas A., Reinoso-Suárez F. (1992). Innervation from the claustrum of the frontal association and motor areas: axonal transport studies in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 326, 402–422 10.1002/cne.903260307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick F. C., Koch C. (2005). What is the function of the claustrum? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360, 1271–1279 10.1098/rstb.2005.1661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Druga R. (1966). Cortico-claustral connections. I. Fronto-claustral connections. Folia Morphol. (Praha) 14, 391–399 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Druga R. (1968). Cortico-claustral connections. II. Connections from the parietal, temporal and occipital cortex to the claustrum. Folia Morphol. (Praha) 16, 142–149 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Druga R. (1971). Projection of prepyriform cortex into claustrum. Folia Morphol. (Praha) 19, 405–410 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edamatsu H., Suga N. (1993). Differences in response properties of neurons between two delay-tuned areas in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 1700–1712 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esser K. H., Condon C. J., Suga N., Kanwal J. S. (1997). Syntax processing by auditory cortical neurons in the FM-FM area of the mustached bat Pteronotus parnellii. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 94, 14019–14024 10.1073/pnas.94.25.14019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felleman D. J., Van Essen D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 1, 1–47 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzpatrick D. C., Kanwal J. S., Butman J. A., Suga N. (1993). Combination-sensitive neurons in the primary auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J. Neurosci. 13, 931–940 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzpatrick D. C., Olsen J. F., Suga N. (1998). Connections among functional areas in the mustached bat auditory cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 391, 366–396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer S., Ledberg A., Schleicher A., Kinomura S., Schormann T., Burgel U., et al. (1996). Two different areas within the primary motor cortex of man. Nature 382, 805–807 10.1038/382805a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guldin W. O., Markowitsch H. J., Lampe R., Irle E. (1986). Cortical projections originating from the cat’s insular area and remarks on claustrocortical connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 243, 468–487 10.1002/cne.902430404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hakeem A. Y., Sherwood C. C., Bonar C. J., Butti C., Hof P. R., Allman J. M. (2009). Von Economo neurons in the elephant brain. Anat. Rec. 292, 242–248 10.1002/ar.20829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinova-Palova D. V., Edelstein L., Landzhov B. V., Braak E., Malinova L. G., Minkov M., et al. (2013). Parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the human claustrum. Brain Struct. Funct. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1007/s00429-013-0603-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinova-Palova D. V., Paloff A. M., Usunoff K. G., Dimova R. N., Yossifov T. Y., Ivanov D. P. (1988). Reciprocal connections between the claustrum and the auditory cortical fields in the cat. An experimental study using light- and electron microscopic anterograde degeneration methods and the horseradish peroxidase retrograde axonal transport. J. Hirnforsch. 29, 255–278 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hof P. R., Chanis R., Marino L. (2005). Cortical complexity in cetacean brains. Anat. Rec. 287, 1142–1152 10.1002/ar.a.20258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hof P. R., Van der Gucht E. (2007). Structure of the cerebral cortex of the humpback whale, Megaptera novaeangliae (Cetacea, Mysticeti, Balaenopteridae). Anat. Rec. 290, 1–31 10.1002/ar.20407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ishizu T., Zeki S. (2013). The brain’s specialized systems for aesthetic and perceptual judgment. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 1413–1420 10.1111/ejn.12135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jäncke L., Schlaug G., Huang Y., Steinmetz H. (1994). Asymmetry of the planum parietale. Neuroreport 5, 1161–1163 10.1097/00001756-199405000-00035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy H., Bullier J. (1985). A double-labeling investigation of the afferent connectivity to cortical areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 5, 2815–2830 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kowiański P., Dziewiątkowski J., Kowiański J., Moryś J. (1999). Comparative anatomy of the claustrum in selected species: a morphometric analysis. Brain. Behav. Evol. 53, 44–54 10.1159/000006581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Künzle H. (1975). Bilateral projections from precentral motor cortex to the putamen and other parts of the basal ganglia. An autoradiographic study in Macaca fascicularis. Brain Res. 88, 195–209 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90384-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBossiere E., Glickstein M. (1976). Histological Processing for the Neural Sciences. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. [Google Scholar]
- Leonard C. M., Towler S., Welcome S., Halderman L. K., Otto R., Eckert M. A., et al. (2008). Size matters: cerebral volume influences sex differences in neuroanatomy. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2920–2931 10.1093/cercor/bhn052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeVay S. (1986). Synaptic organization of claustral and geniculate afferents to the visual cortex of the cat. J. Neurosci. 6, 3564–3575 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeVay S., Sherk H. (1981a). The visual claustrum of the cat. I. Structure and connections. J. Neurosci. 1, 956–980 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeVay S., Sherk H. (1981b). The visual claustrum of the cat. II. The visual field map. J. Neurosci. 1, 981–992 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luders E., Gaser C., Narr K. L., Toga A. W. (2009). Why sex matters: brain size independent differences in gray matter distributions between men and women. J. Neurosci. 29, 14265–14270 10.1523/jneurosci.2261-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macchi G., Bentivoglio M., Minciacchi D., Molinari M. (1981). The organization of the claustroneocortical projections in the cat studied by means of the HRP retrograde axonal transport. J. Comp. Neurol. 195, 681–695 10.1002/cne.901950411 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macchi G., Bentivoglio M., Minciacchi D., Molinari M. (1983). Claustroneocortical projections studied in the cat by means of multiple retrograde fluorescent tracing. J. Comp. Neurol. 215, 121–134 10.1002/cne.902150202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mamos L., Narkiewicz O., Morys J. (1986). Neurons of the claustrum in the cat; a Golgi study. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars) 46, 171–178 10.1007/bf00239531 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCourt M. E., Boyapati J., Henry G. H. (1986). Layering in lamina 6 of cat striate cortex. Brain Res. 364, 181–185 10.1016/0006-8993(86)91001-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milardi D., Bramanti P., Milazzo C., Finocchio G., Arrigo A., Santoro G., et al. (2013). Cortical and subcortical connections of the human claustrum revealed in vivo by constrained spherical deconvolution tractography. Cereb. Cortex [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1093/cercor/bht231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minciacchi D., Granato A., Barbaresi P. (1991). Organization of claustro-cortical projections to the primary somatosensory area of primates. Brain Res. 553, 309–312 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90840-r [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minciacchi D., Granato A., Antonini A., Tassinari G., Santarelli M., Zanolli L., et al. (1995). Mapping subcortical extrarelay afferents onto primary somatosensory and visual areas in cats. J. Comp. Neurol. 362, 46–70 10.1002/cne.903620104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyashita T., Nishimura-Akiyoshi S., Itohara S., Rockland K. S. (2005). Strong expression of NETRIN-G2 in the monkey claustrum. Neuroscience 136, 487–496 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizuno N., Uchida K., Nomura S., Nakamura Y., Sugimoto T., Uemura-Sumi M. (1981). Extrageniculate projections to the visual cortex in the macaque monkey: an HRP study. Brain Res. 212, 454–459 10.1016/0006-8993(81)90477-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morecraft R. J., Geula C., Mesulam M. M. (1992). Cytoarchitecture and neural afferents of orbitofrontal cortex in the brain of the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 323, 341–358 10.1002/cne.903230304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olson C. R., Graybiel A. M. (1980). Sensory maps in the claustrum of the cat. Nature 288, 479–481 10.1038/288479a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson R. C., Brodal P., Gatter K. C., Powell T. P. (1982). The organization of the connections between the cortex and the claustrum in the monkey. Brain Res. 234, 435–441 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90883-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Cerdá F., Martinez-Millán L., Matute C. (1996). Anatomical evidence for glutamate and/or aspartate as neurotransmitters in the geniculo-, claustro- and cortico-cortical pathways to the cat striate cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 373, 422–432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkel D. J., Bullier J., Kennedy H. (1986). Topography of the afferent connectivity of area 17 in the macaque monkey: a double-labelling study. J. Comp. Neurol. 253, 374–402 10.1002/cne.902530307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pujol J., López-Sala A., Deus J., Cardoner N., Sebastián-Gallés N., Conesa G., et al. (2002). The lateral asymmetry of the human brain studied by volumetric magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 17, 670–679 10.1006/nimg.2002.1203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rahman F. E., Baizer J. S. (2007). Neurochemically defined cell types in the claustrum of the cat. Brain Res. 1159, 94–111 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.05.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redouté J., Stoléru S., Grégoire M. C., Costes N., Cinotti L., Lavenne F., et al. (2000). Brain processing of visual sexual stimuli in human males. Hum. Brain Mapp. 11, 162–177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynhout K., Baizer J. S. (1999). Immunoreactivity for calcium-binding proteins in the claustrum of the monkey. Anat. Embryol. (Berl) 199, 75–83 10.1007/s004290050211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riche D., Lanoir J. (1978). Some claustro-cortical connections in the cat and baboon as studied by retrograde horseradish peroxidase transport. J. Comp. Neurol. 177, 435–444 10.1002/cne.901770306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saint-Cyr J. A., Ungerleider L. G., Desimone R. (1990). Organization of visual cortical inputs to the striatum and subsequent outputs to the pallido-nigral complex in the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 298, 129–156 10.1002/cne.902980202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selvaraj S., Arnone D., Job D., Stanfield A., Farrow T. F., Nugent A. C., et al. (2012). Grey matter differences in bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Bipolar Disord. 14, 135–145 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01000.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherk H. (1986). “The claustrum and the cerebral cortex,” in Cerebral Cortex, eds Jones E. G., Peters A. (New York: Plenum; ), 467–499 [Google Scholar]
- Sherk H., Levay S. (1981). The visual claustrum of the cat. III. Receptive field properties. J. Neurosci. 1, 993–1002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sloniewski P., Usunoff K. G., Pilgrim C. (1986). Retrograde transport of fluorescent tracers reveals extensive ipsi- and contralateral claustrocortical connections in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 246, 467–477 10.1002/cne.902460405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith J. B., Alloway K. D. (2010). Functional specificity of claustrum connections in the rat: interhemispheric communication between specific parts of motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 16832–16844 10.1523/jneurosci.4438-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smythies J. R., Edelstein L. R., Ramachandran V. S. (2012). Hypotheses relating to the function of the claustrum. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:53 10.3389/fnint.2012.00053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smythies J., Edelstein L., Ramachandran V. (2014). Hypotheses relating to the function of the claustrum II: does the claustrum use frequency codes? Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:7 10.3389/fnint.2014.00007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snider R. S., Niemer W. T. (1961). A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Cat Brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press [Google Scholar]
- Spahn B., Braak H. (1985). Percentage of projection neurons and various types of interneurons in the human claustrum. Acta Anat. (Basel) 122, 245–248 10.1159/000146023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinmetz H. (1996). Structure, functional and cerebral asymmetry: in vivo morphometry of the planum temporale. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 20, 587–591 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00071-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinmetz H., Rademacher J., Huang Y. X., Hefter H., Zilles K., Thron A., et al. (1989). Cerebral asymmetry: MR planimetry of the human planum temporale. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 13, 996–1005 10.1097/00004728-198911000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanne-Gariepy J., Boussaoud D., Rouiller E. M. (2002). Projections of the claustrum to the primary motor, premotor and prefrontal cortices in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 454, 140–157 10.1002/cne.10425 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tokuno H., Tanji J. (1993). Input organization of distal and proximal forelimb areas in the monkey primary motor cortex: a retrograde double labeling study. J. Comp. Neurol. 333, 199–209 10.1002/cne.903330206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner B. H., Mishkin M., Knapp M. (1980). Organization of the amygdalopetal projections from modality-specific cortical association areas in the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 191, 515–543 10.1002/cne.901910402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ungerleider L. G., Desimone R., Galkin T. W., Mishkin M. (1984). Subcortical projections of area MT in the macaque. J. Comp. Neurol. 223, 368–386 10.1002/cne.902230304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Updyke B. V. (1993). Organization of visual corticostriatal projections in the cat, with observations on visual projections to claustrum and amygdala. J. Comp. Neurol. 327, 159–193 10.1002/cne.903270202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von Bonin G., Bailey P. (1947). The Neocortex of Macaca Mulatta. Urbana, IL: The University of Illinois Press [Google Scholar]
- Wang X., Jiao Y., Tang T., Wang H., Lu Z. (2013). Altered regional homogeneity patterns in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Eur. J. Radiol. 82, 1552–1557 10.1016/j.ejrad.2013.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber J. T., Yin T. C. (1984). Subcortical projections of the inferior parietal cortex (area 7) in the stump-tailed monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 224, 206–230 10.1002/cne.902240204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wegiel J., Morys J., Kowianski P., Ma S., Kuchna I., Nowici K., et al. (2014). “Delayed development of the claustrum in autism,” in The Claustrum. Structural, Functional and Clinical Neuroscience, eds Smythies J. R., Edelstein L. R., Ramachandran V. S. (Amsterdam: Elsevier; ), 225–235 [Google Scholar]
- Westbury C. F., Zatorre R. J., Evans A. C. (1999). Quantifying variability in the planum temporale: a probability map. Cereb. Cortex 9, 392–405 10.1093/cercor/9.4.392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao Z., Suga N. (2004). Reorganization of the auditory cortex specialized for echo-delay processing in the mustached bat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 1769–1774 10.1073/pnas.0307296101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]