Abstract
Disordered gambling and alcohol dependence are influenced by unique and shared genetic factors. Although the evidence is mixed, some research has linked COMT rs4680 (or COMT Val158Met) to the development of gambling or drinking problems; however, no molecular genetic study has jointly examined gambling and drinking problems. Furthermore, the majority of past studies examined gambling or drinking problems using a case-control design. The purpose of the current study was to examine associations of COMT rs4680 with dimensionally and categorically measured gambling and drinking problems in a nonclinical sample (139 Caucasian adults). The current study found that COMT rs4680 was related to both dimensionally and categorically measured gambling and drinking problems. It appears that the COMT Met/Met genotype may be a genetic risk factor that contributes to the development of both gambling and drinking problems.
Keywords: alcohol use, COMT gene, gambling, South Oaks Gambling Screen, Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test
Introduction
The lifetime prevalence of pathological gambling in adults is only about 1%; however, approximately twice as many adults (2%) have experienced problem gambling (i.e., subclinical pathological gambling), resulting in a lifetime prevalence of disordered gambling (i.e., problem or pathological gambling) of about 3% (Kessler et al. 2008). Similarly, the lifetime prevalence of alcohol dependence and alcohol abuse in adults is about 4% and 5%, respectively, resulting in a lifetime prevalence of alcohol use disorder of about 9% (Hasin et al. 2007). Gambling and drinking problems have been categorized even further according to level of problems, including at-risk gambling (Bonke and Borregaard 2009) and hazardous (or high-risk) drinking (Reid et al. 1999), which are less severe than disordered gambling and alcohol use disorder, respectively. In conjunction with this information, recent taxometric analyses have provided evidence supportive of the value of measuring gambling and drinking problems from both a categorical and dimensional perspective (Braverman et al. 2011; Green et al. 2011; Kincaid et al. 2013).
Importantly, twin studies have revealed that disordered gambling and alcohol dependence are moderately heritable and are modestly influenced by shared genetic factors (Agrawal et al. 2012; Lobo and Kennedy 2009). Given that disordered gambling and alcohol dependence are genetically influenced and related, it is important to identify specific genetic risk factors that may contribute to the development of both gambling and drinking problems. One single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) of interest in regard to gambling and drinking problems is COMT rs4680. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) is an enzyme responsible for the inactivation of dopamine (DA) and other catecholamines (Mannisto and Kaakkola 1999). COMT rs4680 is commonly known as COMT Val158Met because it involves a G → A (guanine-to-adenine) substitution at codon 158 of the COMT gene that causes an amino acid change of valine (Val) to methionine (Met) in COMT, resulting in reduced enzyme activity (Chen et al. 2004; Mannisto and Kaakkola 1999). Genotypic variation in COMT rs4680 has been shown to affect COMT activity in the brain, which presumably leads to variation in DA neurotransmission and may result in behavioral differences (Chen et al. 2004). Because DA is known to play a prominent role in reward and addiction (Berridge 2007; Wise 2004), it is reasonable to suspect that COMT rs4680 may be associated with both gambling and drinking problems.
Indeed, some studies have revealed a relationship between COMT rs4680 and pathological gambling (Comings et al. 2001) or alcohol dependence (Tiihonen 1999; Wang et al. 2001). Although recent genome-wide association studies have not linked COMT rs4680 to disordered gambling or alcohol dependence, individual SNPs rarely have reached genome-wide significance in relation to addictive behavior, and the contribution of any particular SNP to the development of addictive behavior is likely to be small (Bierut et al. 2010; Lind et al. 2013; Olfson and Bierut 2012). In addition, disordered gambling and alcohol dependence are not completely heritable (Agrawal et al. 2012), meaning that they tend to develop when genetically predisposed individuals are exposed to certain environmental influences. Thus, it is also important to see if COMT rs4680 and other SNPs are related to variability in gambling and drinking problems in non-disordered individuals. Notably, one study has associated the COMT Met/Met genotype with greater alcohol use in social drinkers (Kauhanen et al. 2000), although no such study has been conducted in relation to gambling.
Molecular genetic studies thus far have not jointly examined gambling and drinking problems. Furthermore, most of those studies have searched for genetic associations with disordered gambling or drinking using a case-control design. The purpose of the current study was to examine associations of COMT rs4680 with dimensionally and categorically measured gambling and drinking problems in a nonclinical sample.
Methods
Participants and Procedures
This study included 139 (77 male and 62 female) healthy Caucasian volunteers between the ages of 21 and 55 (M = 25.95; SD = 7.45) enrolled in a larger project examining the effects of alcohol on self-aggressive behavior. The project was approved by The University of Southern Mississippi Human Subjects Protection Review Committee. Written informed consent was obtained prior to participation.
Participants were recruited from the university and community through fliers, university-based e-mail announcements, and newspaper and online advertisements requesting volunteers for a paid study ($10 per hour) on “the effects of alcohol on motor skills.” Potential participants were screened by telephone interview and were excluded if they reported that they had previously participated in alcohol-related research, had not experienced alcohol intoxication during the past few years, were currently taking medication with which alcohol should not be consumed, had ever experienced a significant medical problem that was directly attributed to alcohol use, had been diagnosed with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, had experienced a depressive or anxiety disorder in the past 6 months, were currently engaged in psychological treatment, or had a significant medical condition. During the phone screen, a cut-off score of 9 on the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) was used to exclude probable problem drinkers (62% sensitivity; Kokotailo et al. 2004). In order to further limit the presence of problem drinking, non-excluded individuals scoring 7 or higher on the AUDIT (73% sensitivity; Kokotailo et al. 2004) were also administered the Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (SMAST) and were excluded if they displayed an unweighted score of 3 or higher (94% sensitivity; Selzer et al. 1975). Because the larger project involved administering alcohol to participants based on weight, participants with a BMI greater than or equal to 35 were excluded for safety reasons. All gambling- and alcohol-related instruments were administered as online questionnaires on a lab computer.
Blood samples were obtained from each participant using an automatic fingerstick lancet device to puncture the index finger and 3MM chromatography paper (Whatman, Inc., Florham Park, NJ) to collect three small blots of blood. A total of 222 participants were genotype tested as part of the larger project. One participant was excluded from the larger project because of difficulty in genotyping across multiple polymorphisms, and two participants were excluded because of only having genotyping data. Other polymorphisms genotyped as part of the larger project included ADH1B rs1229984 (ADH1B*2), ADH1B rs2066702 (ADH1B*3), ADH1C rs698 (ADH1C*2), ALDH1A1 rs6151031 (ALDH1A1*2), ALDH1A1*3, ANKK1 rs1800497 (DRD2/ANKK1Taq 1A), GABRA2 rs279871, SNCA rs356195, and 5-HTTLPR. These other SNPs were genotyped primarily to examine associations with alcohol- and aggression-related measures (not with gambling-related measures). Although ANKK1 rs1800497 was another candidate for examining joint associations between gambling and drinking problems, there were less than 5 individuals with the rare variant of this polymorphism; therefore, we chose not to include ANKK1 rs1800497 in the current study. In order to limit the potentially confounding effects of population stratification, only participants who self-identified as “Caucasian” were retained in the current study. Of the remaining 145 participants, 6 were excluded because of incomplete self-report data.
Measures
South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS)
The SOGS is a 20-item measure that was designed to screen for lifetime pathological gambling (Lesieur and Blume 1993). Higher SOGS scores are indicative of greater lifetime levels of gambling problems. A score of 5 or higher on the SOGS has been used to classify individuals as probable pathological gamblers (Lesieur and Blume 1993); scores of 3–4 have been used to classify individuals as problem gamblers (Lesieur and Blume 1993); and SOGS scores of 1–2 have been used to classify individuals as at-risk gamblers (Bonke and Borregaard 2009). In addition to analyzing dimensional SOGS scores, we analyzed SOGS scores that had been dichotomized in a manner that is most sensitive to the detection of gambling problems: The absence of any response suggestive of disordered gambling was coded as 0 (non-DG-risk participants), and the presence of any response suggestive of disordered gambling was coded as 1 (at-least-at-risk gamblers).
Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST)
The MAST is a 24-item measure that was developed to screen for alcohol abuse/dependence, or problem drinking (Selzer et al. 1975). Higher MAST scores are indicative of greater drinking problems. If specificity is valued over sensitivity, then a score of 7 or higher on the MAST (analogous to an unweighted score of 3 or higher on the SMAST) is recommended as being indicative of problem drinking (Selzer et al. 1975). Because during the phone screen we excluded some problem drinkers, we used a more sensitive cut-off score on the MAST to create a categorical problem-drinking variable: If sensitivity is valued over specificity, then a cut-off score of 5 on the MAST (analogous to an unweighted cut-off score of 2 on the SMAST) is recommended as being indicative of problem drinking (Selzer et al. 1975), which in the current study we used to classify participants as mildly probable problem drinkers and probable non-problem drinkers. A cut-off score of 5 on the MAST has yielded a sensitivity of 97% and a specificity of 75% for current or recent problem drinking (Selzer et al. 1975).
Genotyping
Dried blood samples were analyzed at the Indiana Alcohol Research Center. DNA was isolated using the HotSHOT method (Truett et al. 2000), in which TaqMan probes are used for allelic discrimination (Applied BioSystems, Inc., Foster City, CA). The allelic discrimination assay is a multiplexed, end-point assay. Each assay mix contains two different TaqMan probes labeled with VIC or FAM fluorescent reporter dye which bind preferentially to one of the alleles. The genotype of each sample is determined by the fluorescence levels of the reporter dyes and is clustered on a graph with other samples of the same genotype. Each reaction contains 5 ul of 2X TaqMan Universal PCR Mastermix, No AmpErase UNG, 3.75 ul of water, 0.25ul of 40X Assay Mix, and 1ul of DNA sample. Eight or eleven controls are included on each 96-well plate: 2 no template controls, 2 or 3 heterozygous samples, and 2 or 3 of each of the homozygous samples. Because genotyping is done by endpoint reading, thermocycling is carried out in MJ Research PTC-200 thermocyclers. The PCR products are then analyzed in an ABI PRISM® 7300 Sequence Detection System (SDS) instrument. SDS Software 1.3.1 converts the raw data to pure dye components and plots the results of the allelic discrimination on a scatter plot of Allele X versus Allele Y; each genotype appears on the graph as a cluster of points.
Data Analysis
Alpha was set at .05 for all analyses unless otherwise noted. For continuous variables, Levene’s test was used to test for violations of homogeneity of variance. As recommended by Keppel (1991, p. 127) for a similar test of equality of variances (the Brown-Forsythe test), α for Levene’s test was set at .25 in order to increase the power of the test. Because heteroscedasticity was found for both continuous variables, the Welch test was used to test for genotypic group differences in continuous outcomes, and the Games-Howell test was used for multiple comparisons. Two-tailed Fisher’s exact tests were used in evaluating relationships between categorical variables and allelic groups (e.g., Met/Met and non-Met/Met groups). Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium was tested using the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium calculator located at http://www.oege.org/software/hwe-mr-calc.shtml (Rodriguez et al. 2009).
Results
Correlations and Participant Classification
SOGS score was not significantly correlated with MAST score (r = .155, p = .068), and SOGS dichotomized score was not significantly correlated with MAST dichotomized score (rφ = .151, p = .076). In regard to participant classification, 2.9%, 3.6%, and 25.9% of participants were classified as probable pathological gamblers, problem gamblers, and at-risk gamblers, respectively, for a total of 32.4% of participants classified as at-least-at-risk gamblers according to the SOGS (M = 1.96, SD = 1.46 vs. M = 0.00, SD = 0.00 for non-DG-risk participants). According to the MAST, 20.1% of participants were classified as mildly probable problem drinkers (M = 6.75, SD = 2.10 vs. M = 1.32, SD = 1.40 for probable non-problem drinkers).
Genotype Frequencies and Group Demographics
The COMT genotypic frequency distribution did not deviate significantly from expected Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (χ2= .83, p = .36). Genotypic groups did not differ significantly in respect to gender, age, years of education, or marital status (all ps > .05). Demographics for COMT genotypic groups are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
Variable | GG | AG | AA |
---|---|---|---|
Gender (Male/Female) | 14/11 | 35/39 | 28/12 |
Age | 25.08 (7.61) | 25.97 (7.12) | 26.46 (8.09) |
Years of Education | 16.68 (1.46) | 16.44 (2.14) | 16.74 (2.23) |
Marital Status (Married/Unmarried) | 5/20 | 12/60 | 6/32 |
Note: For marital status, 4 participants were missing data.
Group Comparisons
SOGS
Differences in SOGS scores between COMT genotypic groups were significant, Welch statistic (2, 62.03) = 3.82, p = .027, η2 = .075. The post hoc Games-Howell test revealed that Met/Met participants (M = 1.15, SD = 1.75) displayed significantly higher SOGS scores (p = .020, d = .60) than Val/Val participants (M = .28, SD = .74).
In regard to the relationship between COMT group membership and SOGS cut-off score category, a Fisher’s exact test revealed a significant relationship between COMT allelic group and at-least-at-risk gambling (p = .009, ϕ = .239, RR = 1.98), such that Met homozygotes were at greater risk of being at-least-at-risk gamblers in comparison to Val carriers. COMT genotypic group scores and categorical results are shown in Tables 2 and 3, respectively.
Table 2.
Measure | GG | AG | AA |
---|---|---|---|
(n = 25) | (n = 74) | (n = 40) | |
SOGS | .28 (.74)* | .47 (.93) | 1.15 (1.75)* |
MAST | 1.36 (2.02)** | 2.35 (2.77) | 3.18 (2.72)** |
SOGS—South Oaks Gambling Screen; MAST—Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test
p < .05
p < .01
Table 3.
Measure | |||
---|---|---|---|
SOGS** | |||
Non-DG-Risk | At-Least-At-Risk | ||
Non-Met/Met | 74 (74.7) | 25 (25.3) | |
Met/Met | 20 (50.0) | 20 (50.0) | |
MAST* | |||
PNPD | MPPD | ||
Non-Met/Met | 84 (84.8) | 15 (15.2) | |
Met/Met | 27 (67.5) | 13 (32.5) |
SOGS—South Oaks Gambling Screen; MAST—Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test; DG—Disordered Gambling; PNPD—Probable Non-Problem Drinker; MPPD—Mildly Probable Problem Drinker
p < .05
p < .01
MAST
Differences in MAST scores between COMT genotypic groups were significant, Welch statistic (2, 67.80) = 4.75, p = .012, η2 = .051. The post hoc Games-Howell test revealed that Met/Met participants (M = 3.18, SD = 2.72) displayed significantly higher MAST scores (p = .009, d = .73) than Val/Val participants (M = 1.36, SD = 2.02).
In regard to the relationship between COMT group membership and MAST cut-off score category, a Fisher’s exact test revealed a significant relationship between COMT allelic group and mildly probable problem drinking (p = .034, ϕ = .196, RR = 2.15), such that Met homozygotes were at greater risk of being mildly probable problem drinkers in comparison to Val carriers.
Discussion
In the current study, COMT rs4680 was related to both dimensionally and categorically measured gambling and drinking problems. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first molecular genetic study to jointly examine gambling and drinking problems.
COMT Met homozygotes reported greater levels of gambling problems than Val homozygotes and were about twice as likely to be at-least-at-risk gamblers in comparison to Val carriers (small-to-medium effect sizes; Cohen 1988/2009). In contrast with current results, a recent genome-wide association study did not find an association between COMT rs4680 and disordered gambling (Lind et al., 2013); however, that study examined a quantitative factor score derived from four measures of gambling involvement and two measures of gambling problems (one of which was the SOGS), whereas the current study focused on gambling problems as measured by the SOGS, which may account for this discrepancy.
Although our results differ from a prior study, they may be explained by the tonic-phasic DA hypothesis (Bilder et al. 2004). According to this hypothesis, subcortical tonic (baseline) DA levels are modulated by and correspond with cortical DA concentrations, and subcortical phasic DA bursts (in response to behaviorally relevant stimuli) are downregulated by tonic stimulation of presynaptic DA autoreceptors. Previous research suggests that the COMT Met allele is associated with higher DA neurotransmission cortically, which according to the tonic-phasic DA hypothesis is expected to result in higher tonic DA levels and lower phasic DA release subcortically relative to the Val allele (Bilder et al. 2004). Consistent with current results interpreted in light of the tonic-phasic DA hypothesis, it appears that elevated tonic DA levels and tonic stimulation of DA receptors in subcortical regions of the brain (particularly in the striatum and mesolimbic pathway) heighten aspects of behavioral impulsivity and contribute to a greater preference for gambling-like rewards in rats and greater susceptibility to pathological gambling in chronically exposed humans (Johnson et al. 2012; Voon et al. 2010; Weintraub et al. 2010). In addition, persistently elevated and reduced tonic DA levels in transgenic mice have been shown to increase and decrease motivation for rewards, respectively (Berridge 2007). These findings suggest that the Met/Met genotype may be associated with greater behavioral impulsivity and motivation for rewards compared to other groups. Although a few studies have associated the Val/Val genotype (Boettiger et al. 2007) or Val allele (Gianotti et al. 2012) with greater delay discounting, Met homozygotes have displayed worse Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) performance (suggestive of greater attentiveness to wins than losses) and greater reward-seeking behavior than Val homozygotes (Lancaster et al. 2012; van den Bos et al. 2009). In summary, the Met/Met genotype may be associated with higher tonic DA levels subcortically resulting in greater reward-seeking and impulsive behavior and heightened risk for the development of gambling problems relative to other groups.
One line of evidence that may seem contrary to our proposed explanation of current results is prior research indicating that there is an association between gambling problems and greater phasic DA release (Joutsa et al. 2012; Linnet et al. 2010; Steeves et al. 2009), which according to the tonic-phasic DA hypothesis is expected to be associated with the Val allele. However, such results possibly can be explained with the incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson and Berridge 2008). According to this theory, the repeated intermittent use of certain drugs, such as amphetamine, by susceptible individuals causes the DA system to become increasingly responsive (sensitized) to those drugs and associated stimuli (drug cues), leading to the intensification of drug craving and ultimately to addiction. In support of the notion that chronic gambling has amphetamine-like effects on the brain, amphetamine has been shown to prime motivation to gamble in problem gamblers (Zack and Poulos 2004), and rats chronically exposed to gambling-like rewards have displayed sensitization to the locomotor effects of amphetamine (Singer et al. 2012). In short, non-disordered individuals who are vulnerable to the development of disordered gambling (e.g., Met homozygotes) may tend to display higher tonic DA levels and lower phasic DA release subcortically (consistent with the tonic-phasic DA hypothesis), whereas individuals who have developed disordered gambling (regardless of COMT genotype) may tend to display greater phasic DA release in response to gambling-related stimuli (consistent with the incentive sensitization theory of addiction). Therefore, studies showing a relationship between gambling problems and greater phasic DA activity may not be incompatible with our interpretation of current results, as those studies focused on individuals with more chronic exposure to gambling. Of course, more research is needed to provide support for this distinction. If this distinction is valid, however, then this may explain why greater phasic DA release has been associated with better IGT performance in healthy controls but with worse IGT performance in pathological gamblers (Linnet et al. 2011).
Similar to what was found with gambling problems, COMT Met homozygotes reported greater levels of drinking problems than Val homozygotes and were about twice as likely to be mildly probable problem drinkers in comparison to Val carriers (small-to-medium effect sizes; Cohen 1988/2009). Consistent with current results, some past studies have associated the COMT Met/Met genotype with greater alcohol use in social drinkers (Kauhanen et al. 2000) and with early-onset (Wang et al. 2001) and late-onset alcohol dependence (Tiihonen 1999). In addition, the Met/Met genotype has been associated with greater impulsive and reward-seeking behavior (Lancaster et al. 2012; van den Bos et al. 2009), and the mesolimbic DA system, which is presumably modulated indirectly by COMT activity, appears to mediate motivation for reward (Berridge 2007). Although other past studies have reported conflicting results (Bierut et al. 2010; Foroud et al. 2007; Olfson and Bierut 2012; Schellekens et al. 2012), those studies attempted to relate COMT rs4680 to a diagnosis of alcohol dependence, whereas the current study used more sensitive categorical and dimensional measures of drinking problems. Thus, it is possible that the effect of the Met/Met genotype on drinking behavior is more easily detected in a nonclinical sample using more sensitive measures of alcohol abuse.
The tonic-phasic DA hypothesis may also be relevant to understanding the relationship between COMT rs4680 and drinking problems. Consistent with this hypothesis, greater alcohol-induced phasic DA release in social drinkers has been associated with a lower frequency of high-volume drinking episodes (Urban et al. 2010), and chronically elevated tonic DA levels in transgenic mice, which are accompanied by reduced phasic DA responses to electric stimulation, have been shown to increase motivation for rewards (Berridge 2007; Cagniard et al. 2006). In addition, abstinent alcohol-dependent individuals have displayed reduced baseline DA receptor binding and blunted psychostimulant-induced DA release in the striatum relative to control participants (Diana 2011), and an opposite set of findings in schizophrenia has been interpreted as evidence of greater phasic DA release (Abi-Dargham et al. 2000), which is apparently related to the increased attribution of salience to external and internal stimuli that culminates in psychosis (Kapur 2003). Perhaps individuals with lower subcortical phasic DA release (e.g., Met homozygotes) tend to attribute less salience to stimuli (particularly reward-related stimuli) and consequently tend to seek out more immediately rewarding stimuli such as drugs, which strongly activate and sensitize the DA system (Berridge 2007). However, the Met/Met genotype thus far has not been associated with the use of other substances (Bousman et al. 2010; Vandenbergh et al. 1997), although the Met allele has been associated with heaviness of smoking (Munafo et al. 2011).
Strengths of this study include the use of a nonclinical sample and the assessment of dimensionally and categorically measured gambling and drinking problems. However, this study also has limitations worth noting, one of which is the inclusion of only Caucasian individuals. Although this practice was employed in order to limit the potentially confounding effects of population stratification, it limits the ability to generalize results beyond that of Caucasians. Another limitation is that none of the instruments administered in the current study allowed us to diagnose pathological gambling or alcohol abuse/dependence, which in regard to a diagnosis of pathological gambling would have required a much larger sample given the low prevalence of the disorder. Finally, we cannot be certain that COMT associations with gambling and drinking problems are indicative of an influence of COMT on gambling and drinking problems. It is possible that COMT rs4680 was jointly associated with gambling and drinking problems by chance. However, this possibility is somewhat offset by the lack of significant correlations between gambling- and drinking-related variables.
In conclusion, it appears that the COMT Met/Met genotype may be a genetic risk factor that contributes to the development of both gambling and drinking problems. In addition to attempting to replicate current results, future studies should continue to search for associations between COMT rs4680 and reward-seeking behavior, which may mediate the relationship between COMT rs4680 and gambling/drinking problems.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by funding from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism to Mitchell E. Berman (Grant Number: AA14025).
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
- Abi-Dargham A, Rodenhiser J, Printz D, Zea-Ponce Y, Gil R, Kegeles LS, et al. Increased baseline occupancy of D2 receptors by dopamine in schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2000;97:8104–8109. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.14.8104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal A, Verweij KJH, Gillespie NA, Heath AC, Lessov-Schlagger CN, Martin NG, et al. The genetics of addiction—a translational perspective. Translational Psychiatry. 2012;2:e140. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:391–431. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0578-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bierut LJ, Agrawal A, Bucholz KK, Doheny KF, Laurie C, Pugh E, et al. A genome-wide association study of alcohol dependence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:5082–5087. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911109107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bilder RM, Volavka J, Lachman HM, Grace AA. The catechol-O-methyltransferase polymorphism: Relations to the tonic-phasic dopamine hypothesis and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1943–1961. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boettiger CA, Mitchell JM, Tavares VC, Robertson M, Joslyn G, D’Esposito M, et al. Immediate reward bias in humans: Fronto-parietal networks and a role for the catechol-O-methyltransferase 158Val/Val genotype. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:14383–14391. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2551-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonke J, Borregaard K. The prevalence of problematic gambling behaviour: A Scandinavian comparison. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health. 2009;37:654–660. doi: 10.1177/1403494809106545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bousman CA, Glatt SJ, Cherner M, Atkinson JH, Grant I, Tsuang MT, et al. Preliminary evidence of ethnic divergence in associations of putative genetic variants for methamphetamine dependence. Psychiatry Research. 2010;178:295–298. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.07.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braverman J, LaBrie RA, Shaffer HJ. A taxometric analysis of actual internet sports gambling behavior. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23:234–244. doi: 10.1037/a0021404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cagniard B, Balsam PD, Brunner D, Zhuang X. Mice with chronically elevated dopamine exhibit enhanced motivation, but not learning, for a food reward. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1362–1370. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, et al. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): Effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. The American Journal of Human Genetics. 2004;75:807–821. doi: 10.1086/425589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2009. (Original work published 1988) [Google Scholar]
- Comings DE, Gade-Andavolu R, Gonzalez N, Wu S, Muhleman D, Chen C, et al. The additive effect of neurotransmitter genes in pathological gambling. Clinical Genetics. 2001;60:107–116. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0004.2001.600204.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diana M. The dopamine hypothesis of drug addiction and its potential therapeutic value. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2011;2 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foroud T, Wetherill LF, Dick DM, Hesselbrock V, Nurnberger JI, Kramer J, et al. Lack of association of alcohol dependence and habitual smoking with catechol-O-methyltransferase. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2007;31:1773–1779. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00505.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gianotti LRR, Figner B, Ebstein RP, Knoch D. Why some people discount more than others: Baseline activation in the dorsal PFC mediates the link between COMT genotype and impatient choice. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2012;6 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green BA, Ahmed AO, Marcus DK, Walters GD. The latent structure of alcohol use pathology in an epidemiological sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2011;45:225–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasin DS, Stinson FS, Ogburn E, Grant BF. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence in the United States: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:830–842. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson PS, Madden GJ, Stein JS. Effects of acute pramipexole on male rats’ preference for gambling-like rewards II. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2012;20:167–172. doi: 10.1037/a0027117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joutsa J, Johansson J, Niemela S, Ollikainen A, Hirvonen MM, Piepponen P, et al. Mesolimbic dopamine release is linked to symptom severity in pathological gambling. NeuroImage. 2012;60:1992–1999. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:13–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kauhanen J, Hallikainen T, Tuomainen TP, Koulu M, Karvonen MK, Salonen JT, et al. Association between the functional polymorphism of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene and alcohol consumption among social drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24:135–139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keppel G. Design and analysis: A researcher’s handbook. 3. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Hwang I, LaBrie R, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Winters KC, et al. The prevalence and correlates of DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:1351–1360. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kincaid H, Daniels R, Dellis A, Hofmeyr A, Rousseau J, Sharp C, et al. A taxometric analysis of problem gambling data from a South African national urban sample. Journal of Gambling Studies. 2013;29:377–392. doi: 10.1007/s10899-012-9316-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kokotailo PK, Egan J, Gangnon R, Brown D, Mundt M, Fleming M. Validity of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test in college students. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2004;28:914–920. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000128239.87611.f5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster TM, Linden DE, Heerey EA. COMT val158met predicts reward responsiveness in humans. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00838.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesieur HR, Blume SB. Revising the South Oaks Gambling Screen in different settings. Journal of Gambling Studies. 1993;9:213–223. [Google Scholar]
- Lind PA, Zhu G, Montgomery GW, Madden PAF, Heath AC, Martin NG, et al. Genome-wide association study of a quantitative disordered gambling trait. Addiction Biology. 2013;18:511–522. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00463.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linnet J, Moller A, Peterson E, Gjedde A, Doudet D. Inverse association between dopaminergic neurotransmission and Iowa Gambling Task performance in pathological gamblers and healthy controls. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2011;52:28–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00837.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linnet J, Peterson E, Doudet DJ, Gjedde A, Moller A. Dopamine release in ventral striatum of pathological gamblers losing money. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2010;122:326–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobo DSS, Kennedy JL. Genetic aspects of pathological gambling: A complex disorder with shared genetic vulnerabilities. Addiction. 2009;104:1454–1465. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02671.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mannisto PT, Kaakkola S. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): Biochemistry, molecular biology, pharmacology, and clinical efficacy of the new selective COMT inhibitors. Pharmacological Reviews. 1999;51:593–628. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munafo MR, Freathy RM, Ring SM, St Pourcain B, Smith GD. Association of COMT Val108/158Met genotype and cigarette smoking in pregnant women. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2011;13:55–63. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntq209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olfson E, Bierut LJ. Convergence of genome-wide association and candidate gene studies for alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2012;36:2086–2094. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01843.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid MC, Fiellin DA, O’Connor PG. Hazardous and harmful alcohol consumption in primary care. Archives of Internal Medicine. 1999;159:1681–1689. doi: 10.1001/archinte.159.15.1681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2008;363:3137–3146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez S, Gaunt TR, Day INM. Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium testing of biological ascertainment for Mendelian randomization studies. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;169:505–514. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schellekens AFA, Franke B, Ellenbrock B, Cools A, de Jong CAJ, Buitelaar JK, et al. Reduced dopamine receptor sensitivity as an intermediate phenotype in alcohol dependence and the role of the COMT Val158Met and DRD2 Taq1A genotypes. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69:339–348. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selzer ML, Vinokur A, van Rooijen L. A self-administered Short Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (SMAST) Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1975;36:117–126. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1975.36.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer BF, Scott-Railton J, Vezina P. Unpredictable saccharin reinforcement enhances locomotor responding to amphetamine. Behavioural Brain Research. 2012;226:340–344. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steeves TDL, Miyasaki J, Zurowski M, Lang AE, Pellecchia G, van Eimeren T, et al. Increased striatal dopamine release in Parkinsonian patients with pathological gambling: A [11C] raclopride PET study. Brain. 2009;132:1376–1385. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiihonen J, Hallikainen T, Lachman H, Saito T, Volavka J, Kauhanen J, et al. Association between the functional variant of the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene and type 1 alcoholism. Molecular Psychiatry. 1999;4:286–289. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Truett GE, Heeger P, Mynatt RL, Truett AA, Walker JA, Warman ML. Preparation of PCR-quality mouse genomic DNA with hot sodium hydroxide and tris (HotSHOT) BioTechniques. 2000;29:52–54. doi: 10.2144/00291bm09. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urban NBL, Kegeles LS, Slifstein M, Xu X, Martinez D, Sakr E, et al. Sex differences in striatal dopamine release in young adults after oral alcohol challenge: A positron emission tomography imaging study with [11C]raclopride. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68:689–696. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandenbergh DJ, Rodriguez LA, Miller IT, Uhl GR, Lachman HM. High-activity catechol-O-methyltransferase allele is more prevalent in polysubstance abusers. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 1997;74:439–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Bos R, Homberg J, Gijsbers E, den Heijer E, Cuppen E. The effect of COMT Val158 Met genotype on decision-making and preliminary findings on its interaction with the 5-HTTLPR in healthy females. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:493–498. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V, Reynolds B, Brezing C, Gallea C, Skaljic M, Ekanayake V, et al. Impulsive choice and response dopamine agonist-related impulse control behaviors. Psychopharmacology. 2010;207:645–659. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1697-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang T, Franke P, Neidt H, Cichon S, Knapp M, Lichtermann D, et al. Association study of the low-activity allele of catechol-O-methyltransferase and alcoholism using a family-based approach. Molecular Psychiatry. 2001;6:109–111. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub D, Koester J, Potenza MN, Siderowf AD, Stacy M, Voon V, et al. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: A cross-sectional study of 3090 patients. Archives of Neurology. 2010;67:589–595. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:483–494. doi: 10.1038/nrn1406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zack M, Poulos CX. Amphetamine primes motivation to gamble and gambling-related semantic networks in problem gamblers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:195–207. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]