Abstract
To examine the associations of cognitive and emotional facets (measured by life satisfaction [LS], positive affect [PA], negative affect [NA], and affect balance [AB]) of subjective well-being (SWB) with exceptional longevity (EL), we conducted a population-based study with 463 EL individuals (95+, EL group) recruited from a longevity town of Rugao, China (N = 755, with a response rate of 71.6 %), and 926 elderly individuals (60–69, elderly/control group). The population-based controls were sampled from the resident registry according to the gender ratio of the EL group. We found that the EL group had significantly higher levels of LS (30.74 vs. 28.93), PA (3.91 vs. 3.67), and AB (7.89 vs. 7.40) and a lower level of NA (1.02 vs. 1.27) than the elderly group. Multivariate logistic regression analysis revealed that higher levels of LS, PA, AB, and NA were significantly associated with EL, with odds ratios (ORs) of 1.98 (95 % CI, 1.36–2.89), 2.35 (95 % CI, 1.59–3.48), 2.56 (95 % CI, 1.75–3.75), and 0.50 (95 % CI, 0.33–0.74), respectively. Stratification analysis showed that the associations were significant in the healthy subsample, with the following ORs: LS = 2.31, PA = 2.53, AB = 3.05, and NA = 0.39. In conclusion, SWB, with high cognitive and emotional facets, was associated with EL in the healthy Rugao population. The findings imply that interventions that aim to improve elderly individuals’ SWB may promote their quality of life and, ultimately, EL.
Keywords: Exceptional longevity, Subjective well-being, Life satisfaction, Positive affect, Negative affect, Affect balance
Introduction
Longevity has become a public health issue due to increases in life expectancy over the past several decades. In spite of the ongoing progress in improving the health of older adults, only an extremely small proportion of individuals in a population can survive to an advanced old age. Therefore, exceptional longevity (EL, 95+) individuals have been suggested to represent a highly select group. EL implies exceptional survival with intact health or function and has been considered to be a rare but important phenotype (Newman and Murabito 2013). EL is an ideal model to address factors that are related to life span and age-related diseases and, therefore, targeted prevention and health promotion. In the past few decades, an increasing number of studies have attempted to identify environmental and genetic factors related to EL and estimate their contributions. For instance, research has found that approximately 25–32 % of the variance in longevity is explained by genetic factors and that several polymorphisms are associated with EL (Christensen et al. 2006; Hjelmborg et al. 2006). Although the related factors and mechanisms of EL remain largely unknown, these studies have provided insight for further research opportunities. Among the suggested factors of EL, psychosocial well-being, an important determinant in maintaining a high quality of life, has received limited attention. Using a model of aging focusing on psychosocial aspects (including happiness), Cho et al. compared octogenarians and centenarians and found that a higher percentage of octogenarians displayed successful aging (Cho et al. 2012). This study provided direction for the exploration of the psychosocial factors that are related to EL. However, our understanding of the associations between multiple psychosocial factors and EL as well as the strength of these associations remains limited.
Subjective well-being (SWB), a new psychosocial factor, was proposed by Diener (2000) as “people’s cognitive and affective evaluations of their lives.” To date, SWB has been widely examined and generally regarded as an important indicator of mental health. According to the conceptualization, SWB consists of cognitive and emotional facets (Diener 2000). More specifically, the cognitive facets include two components, life satisfaction (LS, global judgments of life) and satisfaction with important domains. These components are proposed to stably reflect individuals’ evaluations of life circumstances over time. The emotional facets include two components, positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA). They are assumed to be much more reactive to daily uplifts and hassles (Chow et al. 2005; Larsen and Prizmic 2008). Affect balance (AB) is considered to be a balance between PA and NA and a valid measure of a sense of well-being (Schuur and Kruijtbosch 1995). Whereas PA and NA are distinct orthogonal dimensions (Bradburn 1969; Baker et al. 1992), AB represents a comprehensive understanding of emotional facets. In sum, these components constitute an important and effective index system in evaluating the relationship between SWB and health outcomes or EL.
In recent years, SWB has been linked to mortality or survival (Sadler et al. 2011; Wiest et al. 2011; Xu and Roberts 2010). For instance, the Alameda County Study found that SWB and its various positive components, including domain life satisfaction (DLS), global life satisfaction (GLS), and PA, predicted lowered risk of mortality over 28 years (1965–1993) (Xu and Roberts 2010). In addition, in the population-based Longitudinal Study of Aging Danish Twins (LSADT), SWB was found associated with increased survival time independent of familial factors of genes and shared environment (Sadler et al. 2011). Such literature is accumulating (Chida and Steptoe 2008; Howell et al. 2007; Iwasa et al. 2006; Levy et al. 2002; Li 2005; Pressman and Cohen 2005).
However, less is known regarding the association of SWB with EL, partially due to limited sources of data on EL individuals. In the relevant studies, longevity has been indicated by risk of mortality or survival time (Iwasa et al. 2006; Sadler et al. 2011; Wiest et al. 2011; Xu and Roberts 2010), as mortality is considered to be the ultimate result of survival or health. However, few of the subjects who were defined in these mortality or survival studies survived to an advanced old age (e.g., 95+). Therefore, an examination of EL, rather than the surrogate endpoint mortality or survival time, is particularly needed. In addition, most of previous studies on mortality or survival have used only a single item or a few items of established standardized scales to define components of SWB, such as LS (Li 2005; Sadler et al. 2011) and PA (Levy et al. 2002; Xu and Roberts 2010), or even SWB itself (Li 2005). Furthermore, most of these studies have excluded NA and AB (Iwasa et al. 2006). According to the broader conceptualization of SWB (Diener 2000), NA is an important construct that functions as a relatively independent dimension of emotional well-being (Larsen and Prizmic 2008). As previously mentioned, AB reflects an emotional state rather than one facet. Hence, we argue that all components of the index system are imperative for a full understanding of the relationship between SWB and EL.
In summary, existing studies have focused on establishing the relationship between SWB and mortality/survival. However, despite the simple measurement of SWB, little is known about the effects of SWB components on EL. Consequently, we performed a well-established population-based association study in a longevity town of Rugao, China. The strengths of the current study included the ascertainment of a reasonably large sample size of EL individuals; and randomly recruited population-based controls who shared homogenous environmental exposure with EL individuals. These study design features allowed us to examine the associations of cognitive (LS) and emotional facets (PA, NA, and AB) of SWB with EL.
Methods
Design and study population
The data from the Rugao longevity cohort were collected between December 24, 2007 and February 29, 2008 in Rugao, Jiangsu Province, China. It is a well-designed population-based case–control study that has been described in detail elsewhere (Cai et al. 2009). The aim of the cohort study is to examine possible genetic and environmental factors that influence EL. Notably, Rugao has been termed as a “longevity town” in China since ancient times. The strong survival advantage of the EL subjects at Rugao (less than 1/10,000 Rugao people survive to 100 years, while the average life span in Rugao is 75.58 years) is crucial to our study approach (Cai et al. 2009).
The current study subjects were sampled from the Rugao longevity cohort. According to a strict four-step verification process, the number of persons aged 95+ years was 705 (149 males and 556 females), including 102 centenarians (18 males and 84 females), in Rugao. Of these individuals, 463 were recruited into the Rugao longevity cohort, with a response rate of 71.6 % (Cai et al. 2009). Of these subjects, 446 (EL group; 346 female, 77.6 %; mean ± SD age, 97.4 ± 2.08 years; range, 95–107 years) responded to all covariates and no less than two thirds of the items of the LS, PA, or NA subscale and, thus, were included in the current study.
It is pivotal to ensure a representative sample of the general population as a control in population-based studies. With the support of the local government and local inhabitants, we obtained the current control group from the original population. In short, twofold control subjects who were aged 60–69 years were systematically randomly recruited from the resident registry at the local government offices of Rugao according to the gender ratio of the EL group. Of these individuals, nine were excluded due to missing covariates data, resulting in a final population-based control of 917 subjects (elderly group; 712 females, 77.6 %; mean ± SD age, 64.8 ± 2.93 years; range, 60–69 years).
Procedures
All subjects were contacted at home and examined by physicians who were previously trained to administer a structured questionnaire. The questionnaire includes demographic characteristics, histories of chronic disease (tuberculosis, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, stroke, coronary heart disease, and malignant tumor), daily activity histories, mental health appraisals, etc. All interviews were tape-recorded, and 5 % of the recorded interviews were evaluated for interviewing quality. Approximately 3–5 % of the subjects were re-contacted by phone to evaluate the interviewers’ work. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant or a member of his/her immediate family. The Human Ethnics Committee of Fudan University School of Life Sciences approved the research (Cai et al. 2009).
Measures
Subjective well-being (SWB) and covariates assessments
We used cognitive and emotional facets to operationalize SWB by means of the following two scales: the Life Satisfaction Index A (LSIA) and Bradburn’s Affect Balance Scale (ABS). More specifically, the cognitive facets were measured by the LS component, and the emotional facets were measured by the PA, NA, and AB components. All covariates in the current models were collected from the questionnaire. Demographic variables (gender, education levels, marital status), physical health status (histories of chronic diseases, the Katz Index of ADLs), and perceived overall health status (fair, good, excellent) were previously shown to be associated with longevity and psychological factors such as SWB and depression (Engstrom et al. 1999; González Gutiérrez et al. 2005; Xu and Roberts 2010).
The Life Satisfaction Index A (LSIA)
The LSIA scale, developed by Neugarten et al. (1961), includes 12 positively and 8 negatively loaded items with the response alternatives of “Agree” “?” or “Disagree”. For positively worded items, “Agree” and “Disagree” responses are scored as 2 and 0 points, respectively. For negatively worded items, “Agree” and “Disagree” responses are scored as 0 and 2 points, respectively. For all items, “?” responses are scored as 1 point. Total scores are obtained by summing all points for the 20 items. The scores range from 0 to 40, with higher scores indicating higher LS. The LSIA scale has been widely used and shown to be a reliable scale (Bienenfeld et al. 1997). An average reliability of 0.79 was found by Wallace and Wheeler (2002), and the reliability was unrelated to sample characteristics such as sample size, age, and sex. In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha for the LSIA was 0.792, indicating an adequate internal consistency. The LSIA correlated 0.39 with clinical ratings using Life Satisfaction Ratings (LSR), which includes the following attitudes: zest (as opposed to apathy), resolution and fortitude, congruence among desired and achieved goals, a positive self-concept, and mood tone (Schiaffino 2003).
Bradburn’s Affect Balance Scale (ABS)
The Bradburn’s Affect Balance Scale (Bradburn 1969) is based on the definition of “happiness” as a preponderance of PA over NA. It consists of five positive items and five negative items. All items are formulated as questions about the subject’s feelings during the last few weeks, and answers are provided using a dichotomous “yes” or “no” scale. Positive and negative questions are summed separately, with a score of 1 for a “yes” response and a score of 0 for a “no” response. Thus, the score on each subscale (PA and NA) ranges from 0 to 5. In addition, a general AB score is computed as PA minus NA plus a constant of 5 (in order to avoid negative values), resulting in a range from 0 to 10. ABS has satisfactory strong reliability. With a sample of 200 over a 3-day period, Bradburn reported the test-retest reliability of PA, NA, and AB to be 0.83, 0.81, and 0.76, respectively. The internal consistency reliabilities of PA range from 0.55 to 0.73 and those of NA range from 0.61 to 0.73 (Schiaffino 2003). In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.595 for PA and 0.692 for NA. The ABS instrument also has convergent validity. Bradburn (1969) showed that PA correlated with single-item indicators of happiness from 0.34 to 0.38 and with corresponding NA values from −0.33 to −0.38 (Schiaffino 2003). PA and NA are distinct orthogonal dimensions, as has been widely replicated (Baker et al. 1992). Factor analyses have indicated that PA relates more to situational factors and NA relates more to dispositional factors (Baker et al. 1992).
The Katz Index of Activities of Daily Living (ADL)
The ADL scale, which was modified from the original scale used by Lawton and Brody (1969), comprises one instrumental and one physical dimension and has been widely used to assess the functional status of older adults in research. Specifically, the Katz Index of ADLs was used to represent the physical health status of the current study sample. The Katz Index is based on the six daily tasks of bathing, dressing, indoor transferring, going to the toilet and cleaning oneself afterwards, eating, and continence (Katz et al. 1963). Each task has the following three response alternatives: strongly dependent, somewhat dependent, and strongly independent, with a score of 1, 2, and 3 points, respectively. Based on the total summed scores, a nominal variable with two categories, ADL independent (total score ≤9) and ADL dependent (total score >9), is constructed.
Missing data treatment
In the current study, subjects who missed any of the covariates and subjects who answered less than two thirds of the items of a SWB component (LS, PA, or NA) were excluded from the analysis. The missing value of each item of SWB was imputed with the subject’s mean component value. The AB score was calculated by subtracting the NA score from the PA score and then adding a constant of 5, as previously mentioned. A similar strategy for missing data was used by Xu and Roberts (2010).
Statistical analyses
To compare the covariates of the EL and elderly groups, Student’s t test was used for continuous variables and the Pearson chi-square test was employed for dichotomous variables. In the empirical analysis, LS was partitioned into two categories (higher group and lower group) using the median of 31 as a cutoff point. Similarly, the following variables were dichotomized according to their median values: PA (median = 4), NA (median = 1), and AB (median = 8). To estimate the relative probability of EL as a function of SWB between the EL and elderly groups after adjusting for the covariates, odds ratios (ORs) and 95 % confidence intervals (CI) were derived from multivariate logistic regression models in SPSS, version 19.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). In the current study, four models were performed. Crude ORs were calculated in model 1. Model 2 added demographic variables, e.g., gender, education levels, and marital status, to model 1. Physical health status, including histories of chronic diseases and the Katz Index of ADLs, was added in model 3, and model 4 added perceived overall health status. Furthermore, in order to estimate the effects of a one standard deviation (SD) increase in the SWB components on EL probability, LS, PA, NA, and AB were standardized (Z score, with a mean of 0 and SD of 1) and then entered into the multivariable models. The ORs of these SWB components for EL were obtained in the same manner as described above.
Stratification analyses for gender, education levels, and physical health status were performed, as many studies have reported that these variables account for effects of SWB on other health outcomes. For physical health status, subjects were classified as healthy if they had no chronic disease and were ADL independent and as nonhealthy otherwise (Krijthe et al. 2011; Xu and Roberts 2010). LS, PA, NA, and AB were analyzed as dichotomous and continuous (Z scores) variables, and the ORs were calculated.
Results
As expected, LS was positively correlated with PA (correlation coefficient = 0.433) and AB (correlation coefficient = 0.525), and PA was negatively correlated with NA (correlation coefficient = −0.353). AB was correlated with the other three components (correlation coefficient [LS, PA, and NA] = 0.525, 0.741, and −0.715, respectively). As the descriptive statistics in Table 1 show, the EL group had significantly higher levels of LS (30.74 vs. 28.93, p < 0.001), PA (3.91 vs. 3.67, p < 0.001), and AB (7.89 vs. 7.40, p < 0.001) and a lower level of NA (1.02 vs. 1.27, p < 0.001) than the elderly group.
Table 1.
EL group (95+, n = 446) | Elderly group (60–69, n = 917) | p value | |
---|---|---|---|
SWB (mean ± SD) | |||
LS | 30.74 ± 4.93 | 28.93 ± 6.64 | <0.001 |
PA | 3.91 ± 1.17 | 3.67 ± 1.33 | <0.001 |
NA | 1.02 ± 1.23 | 1.27 ± 1.42 | <0.001 |
AB | 7.89 ± 1.80 | 7.40 ± 2.13 | <0.001 |
Covariates | |||
Demographics | |||
Gender (female, n [%]) | 346 [77.6] | 712 [77.6] | N.S. |
Education levels (illiterate, n [%]) | 365 [81.8] | 499 [54.4] | <0.001 |
Marital status (currently married, n [%]) | 22 [4.9] | 740 [80.7] | <0.001 |
Physical health status | |||
History of chronic diseases (none, n [%])a | 372 [83.4] | 759 [82.8] | N.S. |
ADL (independent, n [%])b | 320 [71.7] | 898 [97.9] | <0.001 |
Psychological health status | |||
Perceived overall health status (n [%]) | |||
Excellent | 87 [19.5] | 185 [20.2] | |
Good | 249 [55.8] | 465 [50.7] | |
Poor | 110 [24.7] | 267 [29.1] | |
p value for trend | N.S. |
The range of scores was as follows: LS, 8–40; PA, 0–5; NA, 0–5; AB, 0–10
SWB subject well-being, LS life satisfaction, PA positive affect, NA negative affect, AB affect balance, N.S. nonsignificant
aNone: subjects had no history of several types of chronic diseases (tuberculosis, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, stroke, coronary heart disease, and malignant tumor)
bADL independent (total score of six daily tasks ≤9), ADL dependent (total score of six daily tasks >9)
Associations between SWB components and EL
ORs for EL in relation to the components of SWB for the full sample are presented in Table 2. In the unadjusted model, significant associations between EL and LS (model 1, OR = 1.57, 95 % CI = 1.25–1.98, p < 0.001), PA (model 1, OR = 1.54, 95 % CI = 1.21–1.97, p < 0.01), NA (model 1, OR = 0.70, 95 % CI = 0.54–0.90, p < 0.01), and AB (model 1, OR = 1.72, 95 % CI = 1.35–2.18, p < 0.001) were observed. Compared with the individuals who reported a lower LS score, the individuals who reported higher LS, PA, AB, and NA had a 1.57-, 1.54-, and 1.72-fold increased probability of EL and a 0.30-fold decreased probability of EL, respectively. The significant associations remained when the demographic variables (gender, education levels, marital status) were included. After physical health status was controlled in model 3, stronger associations were observed (LS, OR = 2.22, 95 % CI = 1.54–3.19, p < 0.001; PA, OR = 2.52, 95 % CI = 1.71–3.71, p < 0.001; NA, OR = 0.47, 95 % CI = 0.31–0.69, p < 0.001; AB, OR = 2.79, 95 % CI = 1.92–4.05, p < 0.001). In the final model, which included perceived overall health status, the significant associations between LS, PA, NA, AB, and EL remained, although they were slightly weaker than those in model 3.
Table 2.
SWB | Model 1a | Model 2b | Model 3c | Model 4d | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95 % CI | OR | 95 % CI | OR | 95 % CI | OR | 95 % CI | |||||
Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | |||||
LS | 1.57*** | 1.25 | 1.98 | 1.68** | 1.20 | 2.37 | 2.22*** | 1.54 | 3.19 | 1.98*** | 1.36 | 2.89 |
PA | 1.54** | 1.21 | 1.97 | 1.99*** | 1.39 | 2.85 | 2.52*** | 1.71 | 3.71 | 2.35*** | 1.59 | 3.48 |
NA | 0.70** | 0.54 | 0.90 | 0.56** | 0.39 | 0.81 | 0.47*** | 0.31 | 0.69 | 0.50** | 0.33 | 0.74 |
AB | 1.72*** | 1.35 | 2.18 | 2.33*** | 1.64 | 3.31 | 2.79*** | 1.92 | 4.05 | 2.56*** | 1.75 | 3.75 |
SWB subject well-being, LS life satisfaction, PA positive affect, NA negative affect, AB affect balance, OR odds ratio, CI confidence interval
**p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
aModel 1: unadjusted
bModel 2: add demographic variables (gender, education levels, marital status)
cModel 3: add physical health status (histories of chronic diseases, the Katz Index of ADLs) to model 2
dModel 4: add perceived overall health status to model 3
Stratification analysis
Potential confounders may influence the associations between the components of SWB and EL; therefore, we performed a stratification analysis. We found that the elevated EL probability with higher LS, PA, and AB and decreased EL probability with higher NA were more pronounced in the healthy subsample (EL group, N = 266; elderly group, N = 749), with the following ORs: LS = 2.12 (95 % CI = 1.58–2.85, p < 0.001), PA = 2.08 (95 % CI = 1.51–2.88, p < 0.001), AB = 2.22 (95 % CI = 1.63–3.02, p < 0.001), and NA = 0.52 (95 % CI = 0.37–0.73) (see Table 3). However, these associations were nonsignificant in the nonhealthy subsample (p > 0.05). The significant associations in the healthy subsample remained after gender, education levels, marital status (model 2), and perceived overall health status (model 3) were included (see Table 3). After standardizing (Z score) all components of SWB, we found that with 1 SD increase in the components (LS, PA, AB, NA), the EL probability increased by 37, 22, and 30 % and decreased by 18 %, respectively, for the full sample (data not shown).
Table 3.
Model 1a | Model 2b | Model 3c | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95 % CI | OR | 95 % CI | OR | 95 % CI | ||||
SWB | Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | |||
LS | |||||||||
Healthyd | 2.12*** | 1.58 | 2.85 | 2.51*** | 1.65 | 3.83 | 2.31*** | 1.50 | 3.55 |
Nonhealthy | 1.29 | 0.84 | 1.97 | 1.13 | 0.57 | 2.21 | 0.87 | 0.41 | 1.83 |
PA | |||||||||
Healthyd | 2.08*** | 1.51 | 2.88 | 2.67*** | 1.70 | 4.18 | 2.53*** | 1.61 | 3.98 |
Nonhealthy | 1.18 | 0.77 | 1.81 | 1.73 | 0.87 | 3.45 | 1.40 | 0.67 | 2.91 |
NA | |||||||||
Healthyd | 0.52*** | 0.37 | 0.73 | 0.36*** | 0.23 | 0.58 | 0.39*** | 0.25 | 0.63 |
Nonhealthy | 0.95 | 0.61 | 1.48 | 1.20 | 0.58 | 2.46 | 1.37 | 0.64 | 2.96 |
AB | |||||||||
Healthyd | 2.22*** | 1.63 | 3.02 | 3.27*** | 2.12 | 5.05 | 3.05*** | 1.96 | 4.74 |
Nonhealthy | 1.30 | 0.85 | 1.99 | 1.43 | 0.73 | 2.82 | 1.14 | 0.55 | 2.34 |
SWB subject well-being, LS life satisfaction, PA positive affect, NA negative affect, AB affect balance, OR odds ratio, CI confidence interval
***p < 0.001
aModel 1: unadjusted
bModel 2: add demographic variables (gender, education levels, marital status)
cModel 3: add perceived overall health status to model 2
dThe number of healthy individuals in the EL group is 266 and that in the elderly group is 749
Discussion
To our knowledge, the current study is the first population-based association study to comprehensively explore the associations between the cognitive and emotional facets of SWB and EL in a longevity town. We found that higher LS, PA, and AB and lower NA were significantly associated with EL. In addition, the associations were especially salient among healthy subjects, indicating that physical health status is a mediator of the relationship. The current findings suggest that SWB, with high cognitive and emotional facets, plays an important role in the healthy Rugao population.
Psychosocial factors have been linked to EL for inextricably influencing functioning, health, and quality of life among older adults (Lawton 1983). However, our understanding of the relationship between psychosocial indicators of well-being and EL remains limited due to few sources of data on EL individuals and objective measurements of psychosocial exposures. The conceptualization of SWB provides an effective index system (LS, PA, NA, and AB) to examine the associations of psychosocial factors with EL. Generally, LS is proposed to reflect one’s cognitive assessments of the extent to which one’s life matches one’s expectations (Okun and Stock 1987; Schimmack and Oishi 2005). In the current study, we found that LS (cognitive facets of SWB) had a favorable effect on exceptional survival, and the associations remained significant after controlling for potential confounders, such as gender, education levels, marital status, etc. Similarly, emotional facets of SWB (higher PA and lower NA) were also associated with EL. This study is the first to establish this striking relationship in a famous longevity town. SWB and its components have been widely studied and associated with a lower risk of mortality or prolonged survival time (Chida and Steptoe 2008; Collins et al. 2009; Howell et al. 2007; Lyyra et al. 2006; Maier and Smith 1999; Pressman and Cohen 2005; Sadler et al. 2011; Wiest et al. 2011; Xu and Roberts 2010). From a survival point of view, the present findings corroborate those of the previous studies on mortality. EL implies considerable long-term survival time. To some extent, the findings can be considered as an effective compensation of causal inference, although whether EL individuals maintain cognitive and emotional facets of SWB across their life span remains uncertain (Charles et al. 2001; Stacey and Gatz 1991; Stone et al. 2010).
As one of the few studies with measures of both PA and NA, the current study obtained an index of AB on the basis of the ABS scale, which was extensively validated to measure one’s emotional state, with good psychometrics. We are the first to suggest that AB might play an important role in EL among subjects in a famous longevity town, which is scarcely examined in epidemiologic investigations. PA was previously associated with a higher risk of mortality than was NA (Ostir et al. 2001). Therefore, at times, NA has been ignored as an exposure. The current study focused on AB as a whole to provide a valid measure of a sense of well-being (Schuur and Kruijtbosch 1995) without regard to the inherent limitation of AB in concept and content (Schiaffino 2003). AB may reflect emotion situations well, likely through the accumulation of other positive, health-producing attributes or experiences (Meeks et al. 2012). AB confers a mental health advantage over time and regulates a balance that appears to be an important aspect of successful adjustment in later life (Meeks et al. 2012). Considering the unfounded positivity ratio (Brown et al. 2013) and lack of direct evidence of the impact of mechanisms of AB on longevity, further research on this aspect is needed. In addition, future studies should examine whether AB is directly pertinent to interventions to promote resilience or help older adults adapt to various life adversities (Meeks et al. 2012).
The present study revealed that cognitive and emotional facets of SWB display compelling relationships with EL. The highly consistent results not only indicate the accuracy of component measurements, but also imply a considerable advantage of EL individuals over the elderly in Rugao. High SWB in EL individuals in Rugao may be attributed to two main factors, the social environment and family care-giving resources. Rugao has been termed as a “longevity town” in China since ancient times. The local government of Rugao has provided a list of preferential care schemes for long-lived individuals. For example, the government delivers a bottle of milk each day, provides an allowance and color television, performs door-to-door physical examinations free of charge, regularly visits the long-lived individuals to extend sincere greetings and sympathies, and celebrates their birthdays. In addition, the local government has devoted a substantial body of financial and material resources to construct entertainment facilities and organizations for EL individuals. This is interesting, as Diener and his colleagues argued that a good society is one offering the most SWB to the greatest number of its citizens (Diener 2012). On the other hand, our prior investigation (Cai et al. 2009) showed that approximately 94 % of the EL subjects in Rugao lived with their offspring of three or four generations, who provided sufficient financial and spiritual support. Filial respect, a traditional virtue, is particularly emphasized in Rugao. It may be that the SWB measures used in the current study reflect the harmonious social environment and high proximity of EL individuals to offspring in Rugao, which substantially promote the high SWB of EL individuals in the famous longevity town of China.
Then, what might be the mechanisms of the association between SWB and EL? A recent review by Diener and Chan (2011) suggested two potential pathways that link SWB and longevity. They argued that the broader conception of SWB emphasizes positive emotions and attitudes over negative feelings. In terms of indirect pathways, the positive feeling affects health and longevity through its relationship with protective psychosocial and behavioral factors such as greater social connectedness, perceived social support, optimism, preference for adaptive coping responses, and a greater probability of performing healthy behaviors (Diener and Chan 2011; Fredrickson et al. 2003; Grant et al. 2009; Lyubomirsky et al. 2005; Waugh and Fredrickson 2006). The broaden-and-build theory proposed by Fredrickson (2003), resting on a strong empirical foundation that was constructed across multiple laboratories (Cohn et al. 2009; Fredrickson and Branigan 2005; Fredrickson et al. 2008), emphasizes that positive emotions broaden an individual’s momentary mindset in ways that gradually reshape the individual (Garland et al. 2010). On the other hand, discussion has mainly focused on the direct pathway. Many studies have demonstrated that positive emotions/affect are associated with physiological indicators, especially those involved in cardiovascular health and immune functioning, which help interpret the effects of positive well-being on longevity (Howell et al. 2007; Marsland et al. 2006). For example, Steptoe et al. (2005) found that PA was related to reduced levels of cortisol, a key stress hormone linked to a range of risk factors for metabolic, cardiovascular, and immune diseases. In sum, further research is needed to verify whether the indirect cognitive-behavioral and direct physiological pathways underlie the association between SWB and EL.
In the current study, we employed stratification analysis to examine whether the associations of cognitive and emotional facets of SWB with EL were modified by potential confounders. We focused on physical health status due to its important effect on the relationship between psychosocial factors and health outcomes. We showed consistent associations between the components of SWB and EL in the healthy subsample but no significant associations in the nonhealthy subsample. When comparing the effect of SWB on EL in the full sample and the healthy subsample, the results indicated that physical health status mediates the effects of SWB on EL in the Rugao population. Previous studies on related health outcomes have paid substantial attention to physical health status. For instance, Xu and Roberts (2010) and their colleagues found a similar pattern that the associations between positive predictors (SWB, PF, GLS, DLS, and PA) and mortality were stronger in the “healthy” group than in the “not healthy” group. Krijthe et al. (2011) demonstrated that the association of PA with survival might be moderated by differences in health status among the oldest old. To a great extent, the current results corroborate prior studies. However, more importantly, the present study is the first to reveal that physical health status is a mediator in the relationship between various components of SWB and EL, although this relationship seems controversial for other health outcomes (Chida and Steptoe 2008).
The present study has several strengths. First, the reasonably large sample size of EL individuals and the population-based approach allowed us to obtain credible findings. It is still a challenge to ascertain EL individuals and match population controls. However, in the current study, all cases were recruited from Rugao City, a famous longevity town of China, with a high response rate. The sample frame of our control group was also obtained from the original population of the case group and was well matched in terms of gender ratio, thus minimizing the population stratification and false positive results. The study population was relatively homogeneous in regard to environmental exposures, minimizing the selection bias for case–control matching (Cai et al. 2009). Second, we used a multiple index system to assess the cognitive and emotional facets of SWB by means of established standardized scales. Despite the current well-designed research protocol, the findings should be interpreted with caution due to the protocol’s inherent limitations. First, the current design was not an experimental approach; therefore, no causal inferences can be made. Second, histories of chronic diseases were collected through self-report. However, this method is widely used. In fact, in most epidemiologic investigations, self-report is the only available measurement of chronic disease history. Compared with medical examinations, self-reports might underestimate some conditions (Goldman et al. 2003). The current study adopted the Katz Index of ADLs, which is a relatively objective measure of physical health status. Third, the present findings might not extend to other general populations due to the special culture of Rugao, as described above, and the small/moderate effect sizes (e.g., the Cohen’s d of LS was 0.30). However, the current study highlights the potential to increase life span through the promotion of SWB among the elderly who live in an environment with protective social support and harmonious social and family relationships. The findings may be instructional and meaningful in formulating health promotion policies and programs for other populations. Finally, future advanced interdisciplinary studies are needed to finely dissect the potential mechanisms of the association between SWB and EL to promote a greater understanding of the relationship.
In conclusion, the present study found that SWB, with high cognitive and emotional facets, was associated with EL in a longevity town in China. In addition, physical health status is more likely to be a mediator of this relationship in the Rugao population. The findings imply that interventions that aim to improve elderly individuals’ SWB may also promote their quality of life and, ultimately, EL.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by a grant from the National Science Fund for Distinguished Young Scholars (30625016), a grant from the Major Program of National Natural Science Foundation (30890034), and a grant from Shanghai Municipal Health Bureau Fund for Distinguished Young Scholars (2006Y22). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Conflict of interest
None.
References
- Baker LA, Cesa IL, Gatz M, Mellins C. Genetic and environmental influences on positive and negative affect: support for a two-factor theory. Psychol Aging. 1992;7(1):158–163. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.1.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bienenfeld D, Koenig HG, Larson DB, Sherrill KA. Psychosocial predictors of mental health in a population of elderly women. Test of an explanatory model. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1997;5(1):43–53. doi: 10.1097/00019442-199700510-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradburn NM. The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine; 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Brown NJ, Sokal AD, Friedman HL. The complex dynamics of wishful thinking: the critical positivity ratio. Am Psychol. 2013;68(9):801–813. doi: 10.1037/a0032850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai XY, Wang XF, Li SL, Qian J, Qian DG, Chen F, Yang YJ, Yuan ZY, Xu J, Bai Y, Yu SZ, Jin L. Association of mitochondrial DNA haplogroups with exceptional longevity in a Chinese population. PLoS One. 2009;4(7):e6423. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charles ST, Reynolds CA, Gatz M. Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80(1):136–151. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chida Y, Steptoe A. Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(7):741–756. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ba. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho J, Martin P, Poon LW. The older they are, the less successful they become? Findings from the Georgia Centenarian Study. J Aging Res. 2012;2012:695854. doi: 10.1155/2012/695854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chow SM, Ram N, Boker SM, Fujita F, Clore G. Emotion as a thermostat: representing emotion regulation using a damped oscillator model. Emotion. 2005;5(2):208–225. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen K, Johnson TE, Vaupel JW. The quest for genetic determinants of human longevity: challenges and insights. Nat Rev Genet. 2006;7(6):436–448. doi: 10.1038/nrg1871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohn MA, Fredrickson BL, Brown SL, Mikels JA, Conway AM. Happiness unpacked: positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion. 2009;9(3):361–368. doi: 10.1037/a0015952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins AL, Glei DA, Goldman N. The role of life satisfaction and depressive symptoms in all-cause mortality. Psychol Aging. 2009;24(3):696–702. doi: 10.1037/a0016777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E. Subjective well-being: the science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):34–43. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E. New findings and future directions for subjective well-being research. Am Psychol. 2012;67(8):590–597. doi: 10.1037/a0029541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, Chan MY. Happy people live longer: subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Appl Psychol: Health Well-Being. 2011;3(1):1–43. [Google Scholar]
- Engstrom G, Hedblad B, Janzon L. Subjective well-being associated with improved survival in smoking and hypertensive men. J Cardiovasc Risk. 1999;6(4):257–261. doi: 10.1177/204748739900600411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL. The value of positive emotions. Am Sci. 2003;91:330–335. doi: 10.1511/2003.4.330. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, Branigan C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cogn Emot. 2005;19(3):313–332. doi: 10.1080/02699930441000238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, Cohn MA, Coffey KA, Pek J, Finkel SM. Open hearts build lives: positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;95(5):1045–1062. doi: 10.1037/a0013262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, Tugade MM, Waugh CE, Larkin GR. What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84(2):365–376. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garland EL, Fredrickson B, Kring AM, Johnson DP, Meyer PS, Penn DL. Upward spirals of positive emotions counter downward spirals of negativity: insights from the broaden-and-build theory and affective neuroscience on the treatment of emotion dysfunctions and deficits in psychopathology. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(7):849–864. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman N, Lin IF, Weinstein M, Lin YH. Evaluating the quality of self-reports of hypertension and diabetes. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003;56(2):148–154. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(02)00580-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- González Gutiérrez JL, Jiménez BM, Hernández EG, Puente CP. Personality and subjective well-being: big five correlates and demographic variables. Personal Individ Differ. 2005;38(7):1561–1569. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grant N, Wardle J, Steptoe A. The relationship between life satisfaction and health behavior: a cross-cultural analysis of young adults. Int J Behav Med. 2009;16(3):259–268. doi: 10.1007/s12529-009-9032-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howell RT, Kern ML, Lyubomirsky S. Health benefits: meta-analytically determining the impact of well-being on objective health outcomes. Health Psychol Rev. 2007;1(1):83–136. doi: 10.1080/17437190701492486. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Iwasa H, Kawaai C, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Suzuki T. Subjective well-being as a predictor of all-cause mortality among middle-aged and elderly people living in an urban Japanese community: a seven-year prospective cohort study. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2006;6(4):216–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2006.00351.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Katz S, Ford AB, Moskowitz RW, Jackson BA, Jaffe MW. Studies of illness in the aged: the index of ADL: a standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA. 1963;185(12):914–919. doi: 10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krijthe BP, Walter S, Newson RS, Hofman A, Hunink MG, Tiemeier H. Is positive affect associated with survival? A population-based study of elderly persons. Am J Epidemiol. 2011;173(11):1298–1307. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwr012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsen RJ, Prizmic Z. Regulation of emotional well-being: overcoming the hedonic treadmill. In: Larsen MERJ, editor. The science of subjective well-being. New York: Guilford Press; 2008. pp. 258–289. [Google Scholar]
- Lawton MP. Environment and other determinants of well-being in older people. The Gerontologist. 1983;23(4):349–357. doi: 10.1093/geront/23.4.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Slade MD, Kunkel SR, Kasl SV. Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83(2):261–270. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Q (2005) Subjective well-being and mortality in Chinese oldest old. MPIDR Working Paper, WP-2005–011. Rostock: Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research
- Lyubomirsky S, King L, Diener E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol Bull. 2005;131(6):803–855. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyyra TM, Tormakangas TM, Read S, Rantanen T, Berg S. Satisfaction with present life predicts survival in octogenarians. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2006;61(6):P319–P326. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.6.P319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier H, Smith J. Psychological predictors of mortality in old age. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1999;54(1):P44–P54. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54B.1.P44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsland AL, Cohen S, Rabin BS, Manuck SB. Trait positive affect and antibody response to hepatitis B vaccination. Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20(3):261–269. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2005.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meeks S, Van Haitsma K, Kostiwa I, Murrell SA. Positivity and well-being among community-residing elders and nursing home residents: what is the optimal affect balance? J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2012;67(4):460–467. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neugarten BL, Havighurst RJ, Tobin SS. The measurement of life satisfaction. J Gerontol. 1961;16:134–143. doi: 10.1093/geronj/16.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman AB, Murabito JM. The epidemiology of longevity and exceptional survival. Epidemiol Rev. 2013 doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxs013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okun MA, Stock WA. The construct validity of subjective well-being measures: an assessment via quantitative research syntheses. J Community Psychol. 1987;15(4):481–492. doi: 10.1002/1520-6629(198710)15:4<481::AID-JCOP2290150406>3.0.CO;2-E. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ostir GV, Markides KS, Peek MK, Goodwin JS. The association between emotional well-being and the incidence of stroke in older adults. Psychosom Med. 2001;63(2):210–215. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200103000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull. 2005;131(6):925–971. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadler ME, Miller CJ, Christensen K, McGue M. Subjective wellbeing and longevity: a co-twin control study. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011;14(3):249–256. doi: 10.1375/twin.14.3.249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiaffino KM. Other measures of psychological well-being: the Affect Balance Scale (ABS), General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12), Life Satisfaction Index-A (LSI-A), Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS), and State-Trait Anxiety Index (STAI) Arthritis Rheum. 2003;49(S5):S165–S174. doi: 10.1002/art.11408. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schimmack U, Oishi S. The influence of chronically and temporarily accessible information on life satisfaction judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;89(3):395–406. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuur W, Kruijtbosch M. Measuring subjective well-being: unfolding the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. Soc Indic Res. 1995;36(1):49–74. doi: 10.1007/BF01079396. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stacey CA, Gatz M. Cross-sectional age differences and longitudinal change on the Bradburn Affect Balance Scale. J Gerontol. 1991;46(2):P76–P78. doi: 10.1093/geronj/46.2.P76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steptoe A, Wardle J, Marmot M. Positive affect and health-related neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, and inflammatory processes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102(18):6508–6512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409174102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone AA, Schwartz JE, Broderick JE, Deaton A. A snapshot of the age distribution of psychological well-being in the United States. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(22):9985–9990. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003744107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hjelmborg J vB, Iachine I, Skytthe A, Vaupel JW, McGue M, Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J, Pedersen NL, Christensen K. Genetic influence on human lifespan and longevity. Hum Genet. 2006;119(3):312–321. doi: 10.1007/s00439-006-0144-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace KA, Wheeler AJ. Reliability generalization of the Life Satisfaction Index. Educ Psychol Meas. 2002;62(4):674–684. doi: 10.1177/0013164402062004009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Waugh CE, Fredrickson BL. Nice to know you: positive emotions, self-other overlap, and complex understanding in the formation of a new relationship. J Posit Psychol. 2006;1(2):93–106. doi: 10.1080/17439760500510569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiest M, Schuz B, Webster N, Wurm S. Subjective well-being and mortality revisited: differential effects of cognitive and emotional facets of well-being on mortality. Health Psychol. 2011;30(6):728–735. doi: 10.1037/a0023839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Roberts RE. The power of positive emotions: it’s a matter of life or death—subjective well-being and longevity over 28 years in a general population. Health Psychol. 2010;29(1):9–19. doi: 10.1037/a0016767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]