Abstract
Here we review the usefulness of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in modulating cortical networks in ways that might produce performance enhancements in healthy human subjects. To date over sixty studies have reported significant improvements in speed and accuracy in a variety of tasks involving perceptual, motor, and executive processing. Two basic categories of enhancement mechanisms are suggested by this literature: direct modulation of a cortical region or network that leads to more efficient processing, and addition-by-subtraction, which is disruption of processing which competes or distracts from task performance. Potential applications of TMS cognitive enhancement, including research into cortical function, rehabilitation therapy in neurological and psychiatric illness, and accelerated skill acquisition in healthy individuals are discussed, as are methods of optimizing the magnitude and duration of TMS-induced performance enhancement, such as improvement of targeting through further integration of brain imaging with TMS. One technique, combining multiple sessions of TMS with concurrent TMS/task performance to induce Hebbian-like learning, appears to be promising for prolonging enhancement effects. While further refinements in the application of TMS to cognitive enhancement can still be made, and questions remain regarding the mechanisms underlying the observed effects, this appears to be a fruitful area of investigation that may shed light on the basic mechanisms of cognitive function and their therapeutic modulation.
Keywords: Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, TMS, enhancement, facilitation, cognitive performance
Introduction
Cognitive enhancement can be defined as any augmentation of core information processing systems in the brain, including the mechanisms underlying perception, attention, conceptualization, memory, reasoning and motor performance (Sandburg and Bostrom, 2009). As Sandburg and Bostrom point out, physiological approaches towards cognitive enhancement have tended towards pharmaceutical research. However, this review will suggest that non-invasive brain stimulation, specifically transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), may be a promising alternative. TMS uses very brief high intensity magnetic fields to induce currents and thus depolarize neurons in small regions of cortex. The neural effects of TMS depend on the frequency of stimulation. When the frequency of TMS stimulation is 1Hz or greater, the stimulation is called repetitive TMS (rTMS). If rTMS is pulsed at a low frequency (about 1Hz), cortical excitability generally decreases, while higher frequency rTMS (usually between 5-20Hz) can increase cortical excitability (Pascual-Leone et al., 1994; Chen et al., 1997). This ability to up- or down-regulate cortical excitability, along with its high temporal resolution, suggests that TMS might be a useful tool to manipulate cortical networks in ways that could alter cognitive performance.
Reports of TMS acting to cause cognitive enhancement occurred soon after its introduction as a research tool, with studies observing speeded response in simple reaction time (RT) tasks (Pascual-Leone et al., 1992) and better memory recall (Pascual-Leone et al., 1993; Wassermann et al., 1996), although in the former case the speeded RTs were explained by a general psychological attention effect rather than a specific effect on the stimulated cortex (Tereo et al., 1997), and in the latter cases the effects did not reach statistical significance. Nonetheless, beginning in the late 1990's reports of statistically significant findings of TMS-induced performance enhancements have accumulated.
In the context of cognitive processing, initial reports of facilitated performance were somewhat surprising, as TMS was thought to be a disruptive agent, producing random firing of a population of neurons, generating neural noise that interfered with ongoing processing, thus producing a temporary virtual lesion. Some early studies reporting performance enhancement with TMS suggested a mechanism of “paradoxical” facilitation, in which TMS selectively disrupted the processing of distracting stimulus elements, allowing task-relevant processing occurring at separate locations to proceed more smoothly (e.g., Walsh et al., 1998). In other studies a paradoxical explanation seemed unlikely, as the areas stimulated were thought to be central to the relevant task processing (e.g., Boroojerdi et al., 2001; Grosbras and Paus, 2002). In these cases, TMS may have acted directly on targeted cortex to cause changes that facilitated, rather than disrupted, processing.
While the particular target of stimulation is of course central, whether TMS is disruptive or facilitatory may also depend on other stimulation parameters, such as the frequency, duration, and timing relative to a given task. For example, one form of working memory (WM) task is the delayed-match-to-sample, in which a set of stimulus items are encoded, followed by a delay period, and then a test item which is to be responded to as being a member of the encoded set or not. In an initial finding, a train of 5Hz rTMS applied to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the delay period was shown to increase errors in the task (Pascual-Leone and Hallet, 1994). A number of other studies have also demonstrated disruptive effects of TMS in delayed-match-to-sample tasks (Cattaneo et al., 2009a; Desmond et al., 2005; Feredoes et al., 2007; Hamidi et al., 2009a; Herwig et al., 2003; Koch et al., 2005; Mottaghy et al., 2002). When letter stimuli were used as the encoded items, 15Hz trains applied during the delay period to left premotor cortex (Herwig et al., 2003) and 10Hz trains to left temporo-parietal cortex (Feredoes et al., 2007) also decreased accuracy. On the other hand, 5Hz trains applied during the delay period to midline parietal cortex speeded RT without decreasing accuracy (Luber et al., 2007a). In addition, in the Luber et al. study, it was only 5Hz stimulation, and not 1Hz or 20Hz that resulted in performance enhancement. These studies suggest that processing essential to the WM task may occur (and be disrupted by rTMS) during the delay period in left premotor and temporo-parietal cortex, while task-related processing occurs in midline parietal cortex in the test phase of the task, with frequency-specific stimulation prior to that phase aiding processing. Task-phase sensitivity to disruption or enhancement by TMS was also demonstrated in Cattaneo et al. (2009a), where single pulses applied to occipital cortex in the test phase slowed RT, while those applied in the delay phase enhanced RT: TMS in the former condition presumably disrupted processing of the test stimulus, while in the latter condition prior TMS aided processing.
After a search of the literature, we found sixty-one instances of performance enhancement associated with TMS. These included reports of better perceptual discrimination and motor learning, faster eye movements, and speeded visual search and object identification, as well as superior performance on tasks involved in attention, memory, and language. Enhancement has been reported using various TMS paradigms, including single pulse, theta burst, paired pulse, and trains of rTMS at both low and high frequencies. These various forms of TMS are thought to affect cerebral cortex differently, some acutely disrupting processing with the addition of neural noise or briefly inhibiting or facilitating activity, and others modulating cortical excitability up or down for periods beyond the stimulation. As such this suggests that multiple mechanisms are involved with TMS enhancement effects, and our survey suggested that these potential mechanisms could be grouped into three classes: nonspecific effects of TMS, direct modulation of a cortical region or network that leads to more efficient processing, and disruption of competing or distracting processing (i.e., addition-by-subtraction). The next three sections discuss these classes. It should be pointed out that the expectation from its beginnings has been that TMS will cause a disruption in processing and performance, and in general the finding of an enhancement has usually been a surprise. The classifications and mechanisms offered in the next sections are an attempt to sort out possibilities behind TMS cognitive enhancement, acknowledging that explanations are still post hoc and in the suggestion, rather than prediction, phase.
Enhancement via nonspecific effects of TMS
Better performance in tasks need not be the result of direct influence on cortical processing. TMS also produces a number of superficial effects, including a clicking sound and mechanical vibrations passed from the coil to the scalp. These peripheral auditory and somatosensory sensations can cause a phenomenon called intersensory facilitation (IF: Tereo et al., 1997). Specifically, if the TMS pulse occurs closely in time with the onset of a stimulus to be responded to, speeded RT can result- a purely psychological effect unrelated to the effects of the magnetic field on cortex below the TMS coil. For example, IF was suggested as the cause of enhanced performance in one study of visual motion discrimination (Campana et al., 2003). Here rTMS was applied to primary visual cortex, left extrastriate cortex, right angular gyrus, and vertex, the last being a control site not expected to be affect task performance. At all sites including the vertex, RTs were decreased with TMS compared to no TMS, a non-specific effect attributed to IF.
Using offline rTMS separates the stimulation and performance of the task in time, removing distracting factors, but even here non-specific enhancements still may occur. An example can be found in the results of another study in which TMS sped RT in a picture-word matching task (Drager et al., 2004). Subjects received 10 minutes of 1Hz rTMS at five different scalp locations, and sham stimulation at one of them. In the block of trials immediately following the stimulation RT decreased in all cases, significantly so for three of the sites, as well as for sham. This non-specific effect of TMS was attributed by the authors to general arousal unrelated to the processing specific to the task.
Enhancement mechanisms involved with direct TMS to task-related cortex
This class of mechanism relies on direct interaction of TMS with neural activity in an area needed for task performance. Single TMS pulses occurring immediately before the onset of a stimulus to be responded to have produced performance enhancements (Topper et al., 1998; Grosbras and Paus, 2002; 2003), suggesting the pulse potentiates local neural activity for a brief period. Grosbras and Paus (2002; 2003) found that stimulation delivered 40 ms before the onset of a small target light increased its detectability. They noted that in animal studies direct electrical stimulation of neurons in the same visual area immediately preceding a target improved performance, and that some models of decision making posit that the amount of neural activity in the relevant network can determine whether a stimulus reaches awareness. They suggested that the additional neural activity caused by the TMS pulse may often have brought the neural response to the target stimulus above the threshold of awareness. Short trains of high frequency rTMS also appear to directly facilitate cortical processing. (Gagnon et al., 2011; Kohler et al., 2006; Luber et al., 2007a; Wipfli et al., 2001). A possible mechanism suggested by Berardelli et al. (1998) for this enhancement is post-tetanic facilitation, an increase in excitatory post-synaptic potentials due to trains of electrical stimulation found in animal studies (Iriki et al., 1989).
Another possible mechanism for enhancement due to trains of rTMS is based on neural dynamics. Converging evidence highlights the importance of oscillatory behavior in cortical integration (Crick and Koch, 1990; Schnitzler and Gross, 2005), and in memory, attention and perception (Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004; Freunberger et al., 2011; Fox and Schroeder, 2005; Lakatos et al., 2005). TMS can reset and drive this oscillatory behavior (Fuggetta et al., 2008; Van Der Werf and Paus, 2006; Thut et al., 2011), and could possibly be used to enhance oscillatory function (Hamidi et al., 2009b). For example, two second trains at individual alpha frequency (IAF) immediately preceding stimulus onset has also increased accuracy in a mental rotation task (Klimesch et al., 2003). Performance enhancements have been found to be frequency-specific (e.g., 10Hz but not 5 or 20Hz in Romei et al., 2010; IAF+1 but not IAF-3 or 20Hz in Klimesch et al., 2003; 5Hz but not 1 or 20Hz in Luber et al., 2007a), presumably reflecting entrainment of functionally-relevant oscillations. In one interesting application, theta (i.e., 5Hz) and beta (20Hz) frequency rTMS to the same region (right parietal cortex) had differential enhancing effects, depending on whether the subject was to attend global or local aspects of a visual stimulus (Romei et al., 2011). The theta rhythm may work to entrain neural networks across large regions of the brain in the service of memorial and attentional faculties (e.g., Sirotta et al., 2009), and in that regard it is interesting that a number of studies show cognitive enhancements involving executive functions with the use of 5Hz stimulation (Boroojerdi et al., 2001; Cooper et al., 2004; Kohler et al. 2006; Luber et al., 2007a; Romei et al., 2011; Yamanaka et al., 2010). In all, the use of rTMS to modulate neural systems via interaction with their functional oscillations may be quite a promising approach in generating cognitive enhancement.
An offline approach of repeated trains of high frequency stimulation for 10-20 minutes immediately preceding a task has also produced performance enhancements (Boyd et al., 2009; Hwang et al., 2010; Ragert et al., 2003; Tegenthoff et al., 2005). For example, twenty-five 10 s trains of 5Hz rTMS reduced subsequent tactile discrimination thresholds for up to two hours (Tegenthoff et al., 2005). The extension of effects beyond the end of multiple trains of stimulation suggests a mechanism based on long term potentiation (LTP), a temporary change in synaptic plasticity caused by electrical stimulation of hippocampal slices of animals (Bliss and Lomo, 1973; Bliss et al., 2003). LTP-like plasticity effects have been reported using trains of rTMS on motor cortex (Peineman et al., 2004; Touge et al., 2001) and somatosensory cortex (Ragert et al., 2004) by examining changes caused in motor evoked potentials (MEPs). Direct changes in motor cortex excitability ostensibly caused by rTMS-induced LTP have also been observed using topographic EEG (Esser et al., 2006). It should be noted that there is also some evidence that plasticity effects of rTMS may also have to do with modulation of cortical inhibitory systems (Funk and Benali, 2010).
The synaptic effects involved in LTP have long been thought to be related to processes of memory and learning, based on Hebbian notions of changes in synaptic strength via co-activation of input neurons, and such neural co-activation might be facilitated using TMS (Tegenthoff et al., 2005). For example, repeated co-stimulation of the median nerve in the forearm and motor cortex can potentiate subsequent MEPs evoked by TMS (Stefan et al., 2000; Wolters et al., 2003). In an interesting application of these ideas, Butefisch et al. (2004) found that repeated trials applying a TMS pulse to motor cortex simultaneously with a training movement of the thumb enhanced the execution of the movement for more than an hour, while TMS asynchronous to movement did not. Given the activation of Hebbian and LTP-like mechanisms, TMS has the potential to accelerate skill learning, with the key being to target a cortical region essential to the skill or to learning it, and to apply TMS in conjunction with exercise of the skill. The relative timing of TMS with skill performance may be somewhat fluid: as TMS can generate plasticity effects that last beyond the stimulation period, skill performance may occur before, during or after the stimulation period with efficacious results (Thickbroom, 2007). For example, in one study 5Hz rTMS was applied over a 15 min period to motor cortex, followed by a training session in a motor tracking task, resulting in improved motor performance lasting at least one day (Boyd et al., 2009).
Studies reporting performance enhancements with TMS targeting regions expected to be directly involved in a given task are listed in Table 1. TMS can facilitate task performance whether given as a single pulse, a short train of pulses, or offline using multiple trains. Two keys to enhancement appear to be that stimulation be done immediately before performance, potentiating target cortex for task-related processing, and with rTMS, that high frequency rTMS is used, with the particular frequencies chosen based on their potential for interaction with intrinsic functional oscillations. Further, the LTP-like effects of rTMS, and the potential of co-activating targeted cortex with TMS and task performance to result in Hebbian memory effects, suggest the possible use of TMS in enhancement of skill acquisition.
Table 1.
Studies reporting performance enhancements with TMS targeting regions expected to be directly involved in a given task.
TMS Dosage | On-Line / Off-Line | reference | task | performance effect |
---|---|---|---|---|
Single pulse | on-line | Butefisch et al., 2004 | motor learning | increased accuracy |
Cattaneo et al., 2010 | action discrimination | decreased RT | ||
Cattaneo et al., 2008; 2009b | letter discrimination | decreased RT | ||
Cattaneo et al., 2009a | nonverbal working memory | decreased RT | ||
Grosbras & Paus 2002; 2003 | backward masking | increased detection sensitivity | ||
Hannula et al., 2010 | tactile working memory | decresed RT | ||
Renzi et al., 2011 | numerical magnitude judgment | decresed RT | ||
Stoeckel et al., 2009 | homophone & synonym judgment | decreased RT | ||
Topper et al., 1998 | picture naming | decreased RT | ||
Van Ettinger-Veenstra et al.,2009 | visual discrimination | increased accuracy | ||
Paired pulse | on-line | Gagnon et al., 2011 | verbal & non-verbal recognition | decreased RT |
Sakai et al., 2002 | syntactic decisions about sentences | decreased RT | ||
Wipfli et al., 2001 | memory-guided eye movement | decreased RT | ||
High Frequency | on-line | Boroojerdi et al., 2001 | visual analogic reasoning | decreased RT |
Cappa et al., 2002 | picture naming | decreased RT | ||
Cohen-Kadosh et al., 2010 | numeral discrimination | decreased RT | ||
Cooper et al. 2004 | visual attention | increased accuracy | ||
Evers et al., 2001 | visual go-nogo | decreased RT | ||
Klimesch et al., 2003 | mental rotation | increased accuracy | ||
Kohler et al., 2006 | word recognition | increased accuracy | ||
Luber et al., 2007 | letter working memory | decreased RT | ||
Mottaghy et al., 1999 | picture naming | decreased RT | ||
Romei et al., 2011 | visual discrimination | decreased RT | ||
Schwarzkopf et al. 2011 | motion discrimination | increased accuracy | ||
Sparing et al. 2001 | picture naming | decreased RT | ||
Yamanaka et al., 2010 | spatial working memory | decreased RT | ||
Low Frequency | off-line | Waterston and Pack, 2010 | visual discrimination | increased accuracy |
High Frequency | off-line | Boyd et al., 2009 | motor tracking | decreased movement error |
Hwang et al., 2010 | continuous performance | increased accuracy | ||
Ragert et al., 2003 | tactile discrimination | lowered sensory threshold | ||
Tegenthoff et al., 2005 Cardenas-Morales, 2011 | tactile discrimination motor choice RT | lowered sensory threshold decreased RT |
An exception to the first key to TMS enhancement cited above (i.e., stimulate immediately prior to processing) can be found with TMS actually applied during cortical processing via a suggested effect called stochastic resonance (Miniussi et al., 2009). For example, three pulses of 20Hz rTMS were applied in a direction-of-motion discrimination task to area V5 immediately after the onset of a visual stimulus, presumably during the time when processing in V5 should be crucial to the task (Schwarzkopf et al., 2011). In conditions when the visual signal was strong (i.e., high coherence in the motion of dots in the same direction), such stimulation at or slightly below the subject's motor threshold lowered accuracy, a performance deficit caused by disruption of ongoing cortical processing important to the task typically seen with TMS. However, in the case of low visual signal coherence, very weak intensity (60% motor threshold) TMS significantly increased accuracy. The authors suggested that a small visual signal often doesn't produce enough neural activity to pass a decision threshold and so is missed; but the neural noise added by the weak TMS pulses, while not enough to overwhelm the visual signal, actually boosts the overall background activity the signal is riding on such that the additional activity created by the signal reaches above the decision threshold, resulting in a successful hit and improved accuracy. This example of stochastic resonance demonstrates the need to model the state of the neural system in interpreting or predicting performance enhancement due to brain stimulation (Silvanto et al., 2008a).
Enhancement via “addition-by-subtraction”
Another class of mechanism by which TMS might produce cognitive enhancement is through disruption of processing which competes or distracts from task performance. This type of mechanism can be thought of as addition-by-subtraction, and can be illustrated by a study of visual search (Walsh et al., 1998). Single pulse TMS applied during stimulus presentation to a superior occipital site resulted in an improvement in performance in a visual search task in certain conditions. The task involved searching for a target composed of a conjunction of features also present in a set of distracters (e.g., the target might be a red letter “T” among a set of red “L”s and green “T”s). When one of the features of the search was direction of motion, TMS given over the occipital site, identified as motion analysis area V5, delayed RT. On the other hand, TMS applied to the same site decreased RT when the conjunction target was based only on form and color and not motion. This suggests a competition among the various visual cortical areas which process different properties of incoming stimuli in parallel. In the case where information about the movement of the stimuli was irrelevant, disruption of competing but irrelevant movement information from a motion processing region may have decreased the total processing time necessary to evaluate stimulus information and make a decision. Most likely for very similar reasons, in a different study single TMS pulses to V5 increased RT in a motion discrimination task, but enhanced RT when the task was object discrimination, where the critical visual processing is in more ventral posterior cortex (Alford et al., 2007).
One Hz rTMS is thought to lower local cortical excitability, and trains of 1Hz rTMS of sufficient duration (10-20 minutes) have been described as producing temporary “lesions” in targeted cortex (Pascual-Leone et al., 1999). This makes the use of 1Hz stimulation an interesting candidate to produce performance enhancements via the addition-by-subtraction mechanism, and there are indeed a number of examples where this may have taken place. Once again using the example of visual search, in one variant form the distracters used can be modulated in their salience (i.e., in their ability to capture attention), for instance by making a distracter a bright color compared to the other distracters and the target in the search array. The presence of such a strong distracter slows RT. However, 10 min of 1Hz rTMS applied to right posterior parietal cortex, an area involved with directing attention to salient stimuli, removed this RT cost, enhancing RT in this situation (Hodsoll et al., 2009). Another use of 1Hz down-modulation is to produce behavioral enhancements through release from cross-hemispheric inhibition (Hilgetag et al., 2001; Thut et al., 2005, Kobayashi et al., 2010). Besides 1 Hz rTMS, continuous theta burst stimulation is also thought to down-regulate cortical excitability, and was used on visual area V5 in a visual search task to enhance target detectability by the same group that used single pulses to V5 to enhance visual search performance, providing further evidence for their suggested addition-by-subtraction mechanism (Kalla et al., 2009).
Enhancement effects due to higher frequencies of rTMS may also be explained by an addition-by-subtraction mechanism. For example, short trains of 12Hz stimulation to right parietal area improved accuracy of object identification, a performance facilitation the authors suggested may have resulted from disinhibition of ventral occipital object identification areas (Harris et al., 2008). Another example was reported by Hayward et al. (2004), in which short trains of 10Hz rTMS to anterior cingulate cortex led to the abolition of RT costs to incongruent stimuli in a Stroop task. This may have occurred through the disruption of conflict resolution processing in a situation where it was unnecessary, only slowing down performance.
In summary, the addition-by-subtraction mechanism underlying performance enhancement by TMS appears to function by disrupting or inhibiting an inessential or less essential but competing part of one or more functional brain networks involved in a task resulting in temporary network reorganization. This reorganization can occur on a dorsal/ventral (e.g, Alford et al., 2007; Harris et al., 2008), left/right (e.g., Hilgetag et al., 2001), and/or anterior/posterior (e.g., Snyder et al., 2006) axes. Addition-by-subtraction can occur by either disrupting ongoing processing with single pulse TMS or higher frequency rTMS, or by down-regulation in cortical excitability caused by longer trains of 1Hz rTMS. The studies in which TMS-caused enhancements appear to be related to this mechanism are listed in Table 2.
Table 2.
The studies in which TMS-caused enhancements appear to be related to the addition-by-subtraction mechanism
TMS Dosage | On-Line / Off-Line | reference | task | performance effect |
---|---|---|---|---|
Single pulse | on-line | Alford et al., 2007 | object categorization | decreased RT |
Ellison et al. 2003 | visual search | decreased RT | ||
Walsh et al., 1998 | visual search | decreased RT | ||
Koski et al., 2004 | visual detection | decreased RT | ||
Lou et al., 2010 | adjective judgment | decreased RT | ||
Muri et al., 1999 | directed eye movement | decreased RT | ||
Seyal et al., 1995 | somatosensory detection threshold | decreased detection threshold | ||
Low Frequency | on-line | Kirschen et al., 2006 | verbal working memory | decreased RT & higher accuracy |
off-line | Drager et al., 2004 | picture-word match | decreased RT | |
Galea et al., 2010 | visuo-motor sequence learning | decreased RT | ||
Kobayashi et al., 2010; 2004; 2009 | visuo-motor sequence learning | decreased execution time | ||
Hilgetag et al., 2001 | spatial attention | increased detection accuracy | ||
Thut et al., 2005 | spatial attention | increased detection accuracy and decreased RT | ||
Hodsoll et al., 2009 | visual search | decreased RT | ||
Oliveri et al., 2010 | visual search | decreased RT | ||
Knecht et al., 2002 | picture-word verification | decreased RT | ||
Schutter & van Honk, 2006 | sequential item memory | increased accuracy | ||
Snyder et al., 2006 | numerosity | increased accuracy | ||
Tadin et al., 2011 | motion discrimination | decreated detection threshold | ||
High Frequency | on-line | Sauseng et al., 2009 | working memory | increased capacity |
off-line | Harris et al., 2008 | object identification | i ncreased accuracy | |
Hayward et al. 2004 | Stroop | decreased RT | ||
Rushworth et al., 2001 | spatial attention | decreased RT | ||
Continuous Theta Burst | off-line | Kalla et al., 2009 | visual search | increased detection accuracy |
Potential uses of TMS-induced performance enhancement
Potential applications of TMS cognitive enhancement include research into cortical function, treatment of neurological and psychiatric illness, and skill acquisition in healthy individuals. Manipulation of enhancement effects adds to the experimental palette of brain stimulation techniques examining cortical processing. For example, 1Hz rTMS inhibition of V5 led to understanding of a spatial suppression effect in visual motion perception (Tadin et al., 2011). Right parietal stimulation inhibited suboptimal processing in visual search, shedding light on the cortical dynamics of learned strategy changes (Oliveri et al., 2010). Stimulation of motor cortex before practicing a skilled thumb movement provided evidence for a theory concerning the contribution of interhemispheric interactions facilitating motor control (Kobayashi et al., 2010). Instead of passively observing brain states with brain imaging and EEG alone, TMS provides the ability to directly intervene with ongoing processing to causally test hypotheses, and being able to enhance performance with TMS allows modulation in two directions, enabling clear double dissociations even in the same cortical region. For example, bursts of rTMS in theta and beta frequency ranges to parietal cortex produce opposite effects in processing local and global features of the same stimuli, demonstrating different functional roles in cortical oscillatory behavior (Romei et al., 2010.
TMS performance enhancement can be used to directly facilitate neuroplastic and therapeutic effects in recovery from stroke (Bashir et al., 2010), in TBI (Cicerone et al., 2006), and in Alzheimer's disease (Nardone et al., 2012). TMS has repeatedly demonstrated facilitatory effects in neurorehabilitation after stroke (Bashir et al., 2010; Khedr et al., 2005; Mansur et al., 2005; Takeuchi et al., 2005; Fregni et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2012). For example, investigators applied trains of 10Hz rTMS to patients with hemiparesis who alternately completed practice trials of a sequential finger motor task (Kim et al., 2006). Over the course of a session patients showed significantly improved movement accuracy and speed. In line with suggested enhancement mechanisms based on a Hebbian TMS/task performance confluence , a recent study that used 5Hz rTMS in conjunction with physical therapy across ten sessions resulted in better clinical improvement above physical therapy alone that were sustained over a 12 week period (Emara et al., 2010). Such studies provide strong evidence for the potential clinical usefulness of TMS in post-stroke recovery. Similar use of TMS has also been suggested in accelerating therapies in TBI (Cicerone et al., 2006). TMS may also directly act to improve memory and language function in elderly patients, sometimes for periods of at least eight weeks (Sole-Padulles et al. 2006; Cotelli et al., 2006; 2008; 2011). For example, Sole-Padulles et al. (2006) found that 5Hz rTMS applied to the prefrontal cortex significantly enhanced performance on a face-name memory task in 40 subjects with impaired memory. Subjects showed increased activity in the occipital and prefrontal regions, suggesting that rTMS aided the recruitment of a compensatory neural network that led to enhanced performance, and that rTMS may thus be of use in staving off cognitive decline in dementia.
Improvements in performance caused by TMS may also prove to be beneficial to otherwise healthy individuals. Application of brain stimulation techniques to aid human operators in performance of work was proposed in a recent review (McKinley et al, 2012) as an extension of “neuroergonomics,” a new field of research discussed by Parasuraman and Rizzo (2008). They suggested that brain stimulation-produced enhancements might serve to deal with the growing misalignment of human abilities with the increasing capabilities of modern technology. One means of using TMS (in this case, through addition-by-subtraction) to improve skills has been popularized by Snyder and colleagues (Snyder et al., 2006; 2009). Snyder et al. note that some brain-impaired individuals (savants) are capable of amazing feats of cognition in very restricted areas, that these skills are literal and non-symbolic, and that they may derive from more direct access to raw sensory experience. They suggest 1Hz rTMS applied to anterior temporal and prefrontal cortical regions may inhibit conceptual and symbolic thought, allowing this direct access to raw perception and evoking “savant-like” ability in otherwise normal individuals, and have reported some success in using TMS in this manner (e.g., in enhancing numerosity skill: Snyder et al., 2006). While our knowledge of which cortical areas may be most efficiently inhibited to produce savant-like skill may still need development, the concept itself appears promising. A simple example was reported by Oliveri et al. (2010), where targets and distracters in a visual search task were identical except that the target was rotated relative to the distracters. The stimuli were shaped like the letter “X,” but if identified as such by higher conceptual centers, targets and distracters were much harder to distinguish (when identified as “X's” they tend to be seen in the canonical upright form, blurring the distinction between target and distracter), whereas if the simple feature of orientation was used alone, as could be done in visual areas lower in the processing hierarchy (i.e., where “raw” perception occurs), the task was more easily accomplished. True to this conception, when 1Hz rTMS was used to inhibit conceptual activity in parietal cortex, subjects performed better. As knowledge of functional cortical networks grows, TMS may be targeted more efficiently to enhance cognitive skills, and, with development of TMS-assisted Hebbian-like learning, to possibly accelerate the learning process.
Refinement of TMS enhancement induction techniques
The potential applications of TMS cognitive enhancement are exciting, but they presently remain at the stage of promise. This is because the reported enhancement effects are in general weak in size and short-lived, lasting from a few seconds in the case of short stimulation trains to ten minutes to an hour with offline stimulation. However, TMS is still a relatively new technology, and there is much that can be done to optimize its use. TMS targeting can still be improved and more fully integrated into brain imaging. The parameter space for stimulus delivery is quite large (including frequency, train duration, intensity, number of trains, stimulus waveform and polarity) and has been little explored, and the knowledge of the interaction of TMS with neural systems that could be used to intelligently explore that space is still only rudimentary (Hoogendam et al., 2011).
The spatial resolution of TMS is not likely to improve dramatically given current coil design and materials generally available, but what can be improved in terms of spatial parameters is to make the targeting of the peak magnetic field as efficacious as possible. Great progress along these lines has been made: targeting technology has rapidly matured over the last decade, advancing from marking scalp sites using the International 10-20 System used in EEG, which can only target with a resolution of a few centimeters due to the variable nature of head and brain anatomy (Homan et al., 1988), to positioning according to individual fMRI brain activation. An advance occurred when frameless stereotaxic systems were developed for the co-registration of the TMS coil to individual structural high resolution MRI scans and permitted the targeting of individual sites of activation found by fMRI. The differences between TMS targeting strategies have been illustrated in a study by Sack et al. (2009), in which it was found that only five subjects were needed to observe a behavioral effect of TMS on a task when individual fMRIs were used for targeting, while double that number were required to see the same effect when structural MRIs or group-analyzed fMRI were used, and 47 subjects were needed when the 10/20 EEG system was used. Development of robotic coil positioning systems that will remove human error in positioning and repositioning coils, as well as subject movement (Mattheus et al., 2008) will lead to targeting the precise, individualized targets provided by imaging with millimeter resolution. Targeting can be further improved by using realistic head modeling to guide placement of the coil to individual brain anatomy, as well as modeling the interaction of electric fields with gyral anatomy (Silva et al., 2008). For example, the efficacy of TMS in stimulating motor cortex, as measured by MEPs in targeted muscles, is extremely sensitive to coil orientation (Balslev et al., 2007), and this is thought to be due to the direction of the current induced by the coil relative to the orientation of the stimulated cortical gyrus (Thielscher, 2011). Also, the development of concurrent fMRI/TMS has made clear that brain stimulation has effects beyond the targeted region (Reithler et al., 2011). These effects need to be taken into account in interpreting TMS enhancement. However, such interpretations are not always straightforward. For example, offline theta burst TMS applied to left motor cortex resulted in speedier right hand responses in a choice RT task, as well as increased fMRI activation of the left motor cortex (Cardenas-Morales et al., 2011). This follows the previously stated keys for TMS enhancement from direct stimulation- that high frequency be used immediately before testing. However, similar speeded response was also seen with the left (ipsilateral) hand, which is not so easily explained and suggests some sort of premotor or prefrontal involvement in the enhancement. One exciting development in using MRI to aid targeting involves using MRI tractography: TMS to a medial frontal gyrus location connected (as demonstrated with DTI) to a TMS hot spot for thumb sensation in somatosensory cortex enhanced RT in a tactile working memory task, while TMS to a nearby but unconnected site in the same gyrus did not (Hannula et al., 2010). Further integration of TMS with brain imaging in modeling the cortical target and in tracing the distributed effects of stimulation will continue to improve TMS targeting and thus, enhancement effects. TMS has special potential in this regard because it can be integrated into imaging in ways other types of noninvasive brain stimulation like tDCS cannot, as its high temporal resolution allows it to be interleaved with MRI for the generation of concurrent TMS/fMRI.
Beyond spatial targeting, the effects of other TMS parameters are only crudely understood, and greater exploration of the large parameter space involved with TMS could lead to improvements in enhancement techniques. For example. the exact shape and duration of a TMS waveform plays a key role in the degree of neural response (Peterchev et al., 2011; Kammer et al., 2001), and the optimal waveforms needed to stimulate the membranes of target neurons remain to be investigated. Also, the finding that at low stimulus intensity TMS to V5 facilitated motion discrimination performance while at high intensities it disrupted performance (Schwarzkopf et al. 2011) indicates that TMS intensity level can play a crucial role in determining whether enhancements occur.
There is also a great need for sophisticated modeling of the dynamic responses of large systems of neurons to brain stimulation. It has become increasingly clear that the prior state of a cortical region and its associated network connections strongly influences the effect of TMS (Silvanto et al., 2008a). Prior brain stimulation can change whether cortex is up or down regulated in excitability by a train of rTMS (Silvanto et al., 2008b) and whether enhancement or disruption effects are found (Siebner, 2004). Prior stimulation of a distant region such as cerebellum can abolish LTP-like effects in motor cortex (Hamada et al., 2012). Prior state of indigenous neural oscillations has been shown to effect response to TMS (Romei et al., 2008). Prior neural processing in the stimulated region also can affect response to TMS. In the enhancement literature reviewed here for example, visual adaptation to a letter stimulus determined what new stimuli would be responded to with enhanced or impaired performance (Cattaneo et al., 2008; 2009b). Prior knowledge of which of two visual search tasks is to be performed (and thus of prior modulation of attentional set via top-down processing) is necessary for facilitation of RT (Ellison et al., 2005). In all these ways the state of the cortical region being stimulated plays a large role in determining whether disruption or enhancements occur, and any ability to predict enhancements must rely on cortical modeling.
A promising start in dynamic modeling occurred with a simulation of the response of 30,000 motor cortex neurons within a thalamocortical system, including over five million intra- and inter-cortical layer synaptic connections, to single and paired TMS pulses (Esser et al., 2005). The output of the model system was able to reproduce the effects seen in electrophysiological response to TMS pulses in epidural electrodes of varying stimulation frequency, timing, and dose, as well as with pharmacological manipulations,. Much, however, remains to be done. For example, receptive field properties of neurons and their changes with prior and ongoing processing should be included in neural modeling. One very interesting approach to explaining some effects of TMS on performance suggests that TMS disrupts ongoing processing in a cortical region by reducing signal-to-noise ratio (Ruzzoli et al., 2010, 2011; Silvanto and Muggleton, 2008); that the relationship of signal level and TMS intensity can lead to facilitating phenomena such as stochastic resonance (Miniussi et al., 2010; Schwarzfopf et al., 2011); and that the TMS pulse more strongly affects neurons that are less active at the moments of stimulation (e.g., Ruzzoli et al., 2010). On this latter point, it is useful to note that while a TMS pulse immediately prior to the detection of a stimulus may temporarily raise the level of excitation in all neurons in a region and possibly produce enhancement of detection accuracy through a mechanism such as stochastic resonance (as may have happened in, for example, Grosbras and Paus, 2002; 2003), this would not be the case if the task was to make a discrimination using the properties of opposing receptor field types, as all neurons receive more or less the same amount of excitatory boost. However, one way to take advantage of the possibility that it is the less active neurons that respond most strongly to TMS is to change the activity level of neurons in a cortical area differentially according to their receptor field properties through techniques such as adaptation and priming (Silvanto et al., 2008a). Specific facilitations using these techniques in discrimination paradigms have been successfully induced for attributes such as numerical magnitude (Cohen-Kadosh et al, 2010; Renzi et al, 2011), actions (Cattaneo et al, 2010), and semantic categories (Cattaneo et al, 2011)
Another aspect of neural modeling that demands attention concerns the functionality of neural oscillations in controlling or modulating local processing in cortical circuitry and in coordinating distant regions, as well as the local and distributed oscillatory response to single TMS pulses and to trains of various frequencies of rTMS. The capacity of rTMS to reset (Van Der Werf and Paus, 2006), drive and modulate cortical oscillations (Thut et al., 2011) needs to be better understood, especially given the frequency specificity of TMS enhancement effects (Klimesch et al., 2003; Luber et al., 2007a; Romei et al., 2010), and the apparent greater efficacy of theta burst stimulation, with its more complex nesting of gamma and theta frequencies. In the latter case for example, just a little over three minutes of excitatory theta burst stimulation to motor cortex sped subsequent RT for 40 minutes after the end of stimulation (Cardenas-Morales et al., 2011), and twenty seconds of inhibitory theta burst produced accuracy increases for thirty minutes in a visual search task (Kalla et al., 2009). Overall, a great deal still needs to be learned about the interaction of TMS and cortex. It is not unreasonable to assume that as more is learned and better modeled, enhancement of performance by TMS can be better predicted and effects considerably amplified.
Another serious issue is the short duration of TMS performance enhancements. Enhancement effects of single pulse and brief rTMS trains do not appear to last more than a few seconds acutely. For example, 5Hz rTMS trains to parietal cortex facilitated RT when applied during the retention period between encoding and retrieval in a WM task, but no facilitation occurred in the non-TMS trials which alternated with ones in which rTMS was applied (Luber et al., 2007a). When explicitly tested, no spill-over or cumulative effects of 3 s trains of 10Hz rTMS were found in a visual recognition task (Hamidi et al., 2011). Plasticity effects due to long or multiple trains of rTMS generally last a few minutes to an hour. When measured, those producing cognitive enhancement reported similar durations in performance changes, from two minutes (Sparing et al., 1999; Mottaghy et al., 1999) to ten minutes (Kobayashi et al., 2010; Thut et al., 2005) to one or two hours (Butefisch et al., 2004, Tegenthoff et al., 2005), although using theta burst stimulation, Galea et al. (2010) reported effects lasting six hours.
The reasons for limited durations of TMS effects are not known, but some mechanisms are beginning to be understood. Cortical neural systems tend to be homeostatic (Jung et al., 2008), resisting changes caused by external disruptions such as TMS, generating reactions such as local habituation or adaptation (Chen et al., 1997) or systemic learning within the TMS context through conditioning (Luber et al., 2007b) or practice (Gagnon et al., 2011). Nonetheless, two manipulations may have the potential to increase the duration of TMS performance enhancement substantially. First, it has been reported that increasing the duration of 5Hz rTMS to motor cortex increases the subsequent duration of MEP modulation (Peinemann et al., 2004). Given that repeated sessions of rTMS have already been shown to cause long-lasting changes in mood (e.g., O'Reardon et al., 2007) and in recovery of motor function from stroke (Emara et al., 2010), it has been suggested that repeated sessions of rTMS may also prolong the duration of cognitive benefits as well (Thut and Pascual-Leone, 2009). The second manipulation, suggested by Ragert et al. (2003) and Thickbroom (2007), is that beneficial TMS effects might be prolonged through Hebbian-like learning, by co-activation of a targeted cortical region by rTMS and task performance. According to Thickbroom (2007), an rTMS session can be conceived as a four part procedure: an immediate pre-rTMS period, an application period, an immediate post-application period during which plasticity mechanisms are up-regulated, and a later post-application period when neural excitability has returned to baseline. Having a subject practice a given task associated with a target cortical region prior to stimulation might prime the target network. Performing the task during rTMS application in a time-locked fashion might result in confluent Hebbian activation, potentiating the synapses central to processing the task. Task performance immediately after rTMS application, while the targeted cortical region remains modulated by the stimulation, could continue this neural network training.
The possibility of prolonging TMS-induced cognitive enhancement was tested in a series of experiments based on the enhancement of WM with 5Hz rTMS we found in Luber et al. (2007a). We created a deficit in WM performance using sleep deprivation: sleep deprived individuals will typically exhibit response slowing and lapsing (missed responses) during the task (Lim and Dinges, 2008). We used fMRI-guided rTMS to find an appropriate cortical network (Habeck et al., 2004), test its amenability to enhancement with rTMS (Luber et al., 2008), and finally tested whether concurrent rTMS/task performance over multiple sessions would prolong the rTMS-caused enhancement (Luber et al., 2013).
Initially, fMRI was used to identify a cortical network activated by a WM task that was also sensitive to sleep deprivation, in the sense that network activation decreased as WM performance decreased pre- and post-sleep deprivation (Habeck et al., 2004). fMRI-guided rTMS was then used to remediate performance in sleep deprived individuals (Luber et al., 2008). In subjects who had experienced total sleep deprivation for two days, 5Hz rTMS was applied during the retention phase of the WM task. The fMRI network associated with sleep deprivation-induced performance impairments in this task was used for targeting. rTMS to a left lateral occipital site within the network resulted in a reduction of the sleep-induced RT deficit without a corresponding decrease in accuracy, while stimulation outside the network did not. In pre-post sleep deprivation fMRI scans, the degree of performance enhancement with rTMS correlated with the degree to which each individual failed to sustain activation of the fMRI network. These results demonstrated that rTMS had modulated a cortical network critical to the WM task in a way that improved its resilience to sleep deprivation.
We then implemented two potential manipulations to prolong beneficial cognitive rTMS effects by co-activating targeted cortex with rTMS while subjects performed the WM task, and did so repeatedly in four sessions over the course of two days of sleep deprivation (Luber et al., 2013). In testing WM performance at the end of the sleep deprivation period, eighteen hours after the last TMS session, sleep deprived subjects receiving active 5Hz rTMS performed the WM task similarly to non-sleep deprived individuals, while sleep deprived subjects receiving sham rTMS showed all the slowing and lapses typical of sleep deprived individuals. These results suggest that rTMS applied in multiple sessions coupled with co-activation with WM task performance affected neural circuitry involved in WM to prevent the full impact of sleep deprivation on processing involved with the task from occurring. Most important in the present context, a beneficial cognitive effect of rTMS was demonstrated a full eighteen hours after the last of four sessions of concurrent rTMS/task performance, demonstrating a prolongation of benefit an order of magnitude longer than is typically reported in studies of TMS performance enhancement. This result suggests that long lasting TMS cognitive enhancement and a technology of specific skill enhancement using brain stimulation may be possible.
Conclusion
Over sixty reports of TMS performance enhancement have accumulated over the last decade and a half, and many more are likely as the technology of TMS is refined, and as knowledge of cortical network dynamics builds. Increasing our understanding of enhancement mechanisms such as addition-by-subtraction, potentiating oscillatory behavior, and promotion of Hebbian-type learning may result in acute facilitation of skills needed to interact with ever more complex information technology as well as long-lasting therapeutic benefits for those with neurological and psychiatric illnesses and accelerated learning of useful skills in healthy individuals. TMS itself can be expensive, especially when used in conjunction with brain imaging, and may ultimately be supplanted for common use by cheaper alternatives such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), but, given its superior spatial and temporal resolution, its present impact on researching cognitive enhancement is clear.
Highlights.
We have found 61 reports in which TMS enhanced cognitive performance, rather than disrupting it
Mechanisms of TMS enhancement include nonspecific, direct and indirect classifications
Applications of TMS enhancement include research, therapy, and skill acquisition
A great deal can still be done to strengthen TMS enhancement effects
In particular, new methods may result in long lasting TMS cognitive enhancement
Acknowledgements
This review was funded in part by NIH grant K01 AG031912.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Alford JL, Van Donkelaar P, Dassonville P, Marrcco RT. Transcranial magnetic stimulation over MT/MST fails to impair judgments of implied motion. Cog., Affect., Behav. Neurosci. 2007;7(3):225–232. doi: 10.3758/cabn.7.3.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balslev D, Braet W, McCallister C, Miall RC. Inter-individual variability in optimal current direction for transcranial magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex. J. Neurosci. Meth. 2007;162:309–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bashir S, Mizrahi I, Weaver K, Fregni F, Pascual-Leone A. Assessment and modulation of neural plasticity in rehabilitation with transcranial magnetic stimulation. PM&R. 2010;2(suppl 2):S253–S268. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.10.015. 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berardelli A, Inghilleri M, Rothwell JC, et al. Facilitation of muscle evoked responses after repetitive cortical stimulation in man. Exp Brain Res. 1998;122:79–84. doi: 10.1007/s002210050493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss TV, Collingridge GL, Morris RG. Introduction. Longterm potentiation and structure of the issue. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;358:607–611. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2003.1282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232:331–356. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boroojerdi B, Phipps M, Kopylev L, Wharton CM, Cohen LG, Grafman J. Enhancing analogic reasoning with rTMS over the left prefrontal cortex. Neurology. 2001;56:526–528. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.4.526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyd LA, Linsdell MA. Excitatory repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to left dorsal premotor cortex enhances motor consolidation of new skills. BMC Neurosci. 2009;10(72):1471–2202. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butefisch CM, Khurana V, Kopylev L, Cohen LG. Enhancing encoding of a motor memory in the primary motor cortex by cortical stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;91:2110–2116. doi: 10.1152/jn.01038.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004;304:1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campana G, Cowey A, Walsh V. Priming of motion direction and area V5/MT: a test of perceptual memory. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;12:663–669. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.6.663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cappa SF, Sandrini M, Rossini PM, Sosta K, Miniussi C. The role of the left frontal lobe in action naming: rTMS evidence. Neurology. 2002;59(5):720–723. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.5.720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardenas-Morales M, Gron G, Kammer T. Exploring the after-effects of theta burst magnetic stimulation on the human motor cortex: A functional imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011;32:1948–1960. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattaneo L, Sandrini M, Schwarzbach J. State-dependent TMS reveals a hierarchical representation of observed acts in the temporal, parietal, and premotor cortices. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:2252–2258. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattaneo Z, Rota F, Vecchi T, Silvanto J. Using state-dependency of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to investigate letter selectivity in the left posterior parietal cortex: A comparison of TMS-priming and TMS-adaptation paradigms. Eur J. Neurosci. 2008;28:1924–1929. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06466.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattaneo Z, Vecchi T, Pascual-Leone A, Silvanto J. Contrasting early visual cortical activation states causally involved in visual imagery and short-term memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2009a;30:1393–1400. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06911.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattaneo Z, Rota F, Walsh V, Vecchi T, Silvanto J. TMS-adaptation reveals abstract letter selectivity in the left posterior parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2009b;19:2321–2325. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen R, Classen J, Gerloff C, Celnik P, Wassermann EM, Hallett M, Cohen LG. Depression of motor cortex excitability by low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology. 1997;48:1398–1403. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.5.1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicerone K, Levin H, Malec J, Stuss D, Whyte J. Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for executive function: Moving from bench to bedside in patients with traumatic brain injury. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006;18:1212–1222. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen-Kadosh R, Muggleton N, Silvanto J, Walsh V. Double dissociation of format-dependent and number-specific neurons in human parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:2166–2171. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotelli M, Manenti R, Cappa SF, Geroldi C, Zanetti O, Rossini PM, Miniussi C. Effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on action naming in patients with Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2006;63:1602–1604. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.11.1602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotelli M, Manenti R, Cappa SF, Zanetti O, Miniussi C. Transcranial magnetic stimulation improves naming in Alzheimer disease patients at different stages of cognitive decline. Eur. J. Neurol. 2008;15:1286–1292. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2008.02202.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotelli M, Calabria M, et al. Improved language performance in Alzheimer disease following brain stimulation. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2011;82:794–797. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.197848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick F, Koch C. Some reflections on visual awareness. Cold Spring Harbor Symp.Quant. Biol. 1990;55:953–962. doi: 10.1101/sqb.1990.055.01.089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ACG, Humphreys GW, Hulleman J, Praamstra P, Georgeson M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation to right parietal cortex modifies the attentional blink. Exp. Brain Res. 2004;155:24–29. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1697-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desmond JE, Chen SHA, Shieh PB. Cerebellar transcranial magnetic stimulation impairs verbal working memory. Ann. Neurol. 2005;58:550–553. doi: 10.1002/ana.20604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drager B, Breitenstein C, Helmke U, Kamping S, Knecht S. Specific and nonspecific effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on picture-word verification. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004;20:1681–1687. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03623.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellison A, Battelli L, Cowey A, Walsh V. The effect of expectation on facilitation of color/form conjunction tasks by TMS over area V5. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1794–1801. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00180-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emara TH, Moustafa RR, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at 1Hz and 5Hz produces sustained improvement in motor function and disability after ischaemic stroke. Eur. J. Neurol. 2010;17:1203–1209. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2010.03000.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esser SK, Hill SL, Tononi G. Modeling the effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on cortical circuits. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:622–639. doi: 10.1152/jn.01230.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esser SK, Huber R, Massimini M, Peterson MJ, Ferrarelli F, Tononi G. A direct demonstration of cortical LTP in humans: a combined TMS/EEG study. Brain Res Bull. 2006;69:86–94. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evers S, Bockermann I, Nyhuis PW. The impact of transcranial magnetic stimulation on cognitive processing: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport. 2001;12(13):2915–2918. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200109170-00032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feredoes E, Tononi G, Postle BR. The neural bases of the short-term storage of verbal information are anatomically variable across individuals. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:11003–11008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1573-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox JJ, Schroeder CE. The case for feedforward multisensory convergence during early cortical processing. Neuroreport. 2005;16:419–423. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200504040-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fregni F, Boggio PS, et al. A sham-controlled trial of a 5-day course of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the unaffected hemisphere in stroke patients. Stroke. 2006;37:2115–2122. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000231390.58967.6b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freunberger R, Werkle-Bergner M, Griesmayr B, Lindenberger U, Klimesch W. Brain oscillatory correlates of working memory constraints. Brain Res. 2011;1375:93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuggetta G, Pavone EF, Fiaschi A, Manganotti P. Acute modulation of cortical oscillatory activities during short trains of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex: A combined EEG and TMS study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2008;29:1–13. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Funke K, Benali A. Cortical cellular actions of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2010;28:399–417. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2010-0566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagnon G, Schneider C, Grondin S, Blanchet S. Enhancement of episodic memory in young and healthy adults: a paired-pulse TMS study on encoding and retrieval performance. Neurosci. Lett. 2011;488:138–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea JM, Albert NB, Ditye T, Miall RC. Disruption of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex facilitates the consolidation of procedural skills. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010;22:1158–1164. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosbras MH, Paus T. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field: effects on visual perception and attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002;14:1109–1120. doi: 10.1162/089892902320474553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosbras MH, Paus T. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field facilitates visual awareness. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003;18:3121–3126. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03055.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habeck C, Rakitin BC. An event-related fMRI study of the neurobehavioral impact of SD on performance of a delayed-match-to-sample task. Cogn. Brain Res. 2004;18:306–321. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamada M, Strigaro G, et al. Cerebellar modulation of human associative plasticity. J. Physiol. 2012;10:2365–2374. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.230540. 590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamidi M, Tononi G, Postle BR. Evaluating the role of prefrontal and parietal cortices in memory-guided response with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuropsychologia. 2009a;47:295–302. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamidi M, Slagter HA, Tononi G, Postle BR. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation affects behavior by biasing endogenous cortical oscillations. Front. Integrative Neurosci. 2009b;3 doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.014.2009. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.014.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamidi M, Johnson JS, Feredoes E, Postle BR. Does high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation produce residual and/or cumulative effects within an experimental session? Brain Topogr. 2011;23:355–367. doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0153-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannula H, Neuvonen T, et al. Increasing top-down suppression from prefrontal cortex facilitates tactile working memory. NeuroImage. 2010;49:1091–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris IM, Benito CT, Ruzzoli M, Miniussi C. Effects of right parietal transcranial magnetic stimulation on object identification and orientation judgments. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008;20:916–926. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayward G, Goodwin GM, Harmer CJ. The role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the counting Stroop task. Exp. Brain Res. 2004;154:355–358. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1665-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herwig U, Abler B, et al. Verbal storage in a premotor-parietal network: evidence from fMRI-guided magnetic stimulation. NeuroImage. 2003;20:1032–1041. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00368-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hilgetag CC, Hugo T, Pascual-Leone A. Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced ‘virtual lesons’ of human parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2001:953–957. doi: 10.1038/nn0901-953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodsoll J, Mevorach C, Humphreys GW. Driven to less distraction: rTMS of the right parietal cortex reduces attentional capture in visual search. Cereb. Cortex. 2009;19(1):106–114. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homan RW, Herman J, Purdy P. Cerebral location of international 10-20 system electrode placement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1987;66:376–382. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90206-9. 1987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoogendam JM, Ramakers GMJ, DiLazzaro D. Physiology of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human brain. Brain Stimul. 2011;3:95–118. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hwang JH, Kim SH, Park CS, Bang SA, Kim SE. Acute high-frequency rTMS of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and attentional control in healthy young men. Brain Res. 2010;1329:152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iriki A, Pavlides C, Keller A, Asanuma H. Long-term potentiation in the motor cortex. Science. 1989;245:1385–1387. doi: 10.1126/science.2551038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalla R, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V. Human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in visual search for conjunctions but not features: A theta TMS study. Cortex. 2009;45:1085–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kammer T, Beck S, Thielscher A, Laubis-Herrmann U, Topka H. Motor thresholds in humans: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study comparing different pulse waveforms, current directions and stimulator types. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001;112:250–258. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00513-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khedr EM, Ahmed MA, Fathy N, Rothwell JC. Therapeutic trial of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation after acute ischemic stroke. Neurology. 2005;65:466–468. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000173067.84247.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim YH, You SH, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation-induced corticomotor excitability and associated motor skill acquisition in chronic stroke. Stroke. 2006;37:1471–1476. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000221233.55497.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschen MP, Davis-Ratner MS, Jerde TE, Schraedley-Desmond P, Desmond JE. Enhancement of phonological memory following transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Behav. Neurol. 2006;17(3–4):187–194. doi: 10.1155/2006/469132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Gerloff C. Enhancing cognitive performance with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at human individual alpha frequency. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003;17:1129–1133. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02517.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knecht S, Floel A, et al. Degree of language lateralization determines susceptibility to unilateral brain lesions. Nature Neurosci. 2002;5:695–699. doi: 10.1038/nn868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi M, Hutchinson S, Theoret H, Schlaug G, Pascual-Leone A. Repetitive TMS of the motor cortex improves ipsiliateral sequential simple finger movements. Neurology. 2004;62:91–98. doi: 10.1212/wnl.62.1.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi M, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A. Suppression of ipsilateral motor cortex facilitates motor skill learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:833–836. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06628.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi M. Effect of slow repetitive TMS of the motor cortex on ipsilateral sequential simple finger movements and motor skill learning. Restorative Neurol. Neurosci. 2010;28:437–448. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2010-0562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koch G, Oliveri M, Torriero S, Carlesimo GA, Turriziani P, Caltagirone C. rTMS evidence of different delay and decision processes in a fronto-parietal neuronal network activated during spatial working memory. NeuroImage. 2005;24:34–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohler S, Paus T, Buckner RL, Milner B. Effect of left inferior prefrontal stimulation on episodic memory formation: A two-stage fMRI-rTMS study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006;16:178–188. doi: 10.1162/089892904322984490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koski L, Molnar-Szakacs I, Iacoboni M. Exploring the contributions of premotor anparietal cortex to spatial compatibility using image-guided TMS. Neuroimage. 2005;24:296–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakatos P, Shah AS, Knuth KH, Ulbert I, Karmos G, Shroeder CE. An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:1904–1911. doi: 10.1152/jn.00263.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1129:305–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lou H, Luber B, Stanford AD, Lisanby SH. Self-specific processing in the default network: A single-pulse TMS study. Exp. Brain Res. 2010;207:27–38. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2425-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luber B, Kinnunen LH, Rakitin BC, Ellsasser R, Stern Y, Lisanby SH. Facilitation of performance in a working memory task with rTMS stimulation of the precuneus: Frequency and time-dependent effects. Brain Res. 2007a;1128:120–129. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luber B, Balsam P, Nguyen T, Gross M, Lisanby SH. Classical conditioned learning using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 2007b;183:361–369. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1052-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luber B, Stanford AD, et al. Remediation of sleep-deprivation induced visual working memory impairment with fMRI-guided Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:2077–2085. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luber B, Steffener J, et al. Extended remediation of sleep deprivation-induced working memory deficits using fMRI-guided repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Sleep. 2013 doi: 10.5665/sleep.2712. In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansur CG, Fregni F, Boggio PS, et al. A sham stimulation-controlled trial of rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere in stroke patients. Neurology. 2005;64:1802–1804. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000161839.38079.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthaus L, Trillenberg P, Fadini T, Finke M, Schweikard A. Brain mapping with transcranial magnetic stimulation using a refined correlation ratio and Kendall's tau. Stat. Med. 2008;27:5252–5270. doi: 10.1002/sim.3353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKinley RA, Bridges N, Walters CM, Nelson J. Modulating the brain at work using noninvasive transcranial stimulation. NeuroImage. 2012;59:129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miniussi C, Ruzzoli M, Walsh V. The mechanism of transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognition. Cortex. 2010;46:128–130. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mottaghy FM, Hungs M, et al. Facilitation of picture naming after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology. 1999;53(8):1806–1812. doi: 10.1212/wnl.53.8.1806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mottaghy FM, Gangitano M, Sparing R, Pascual-Leone A. Segregation of areas related to visual working memory in the prefrontal cortex revealed by rTMS. Cereb. Cortex. 2002;12:369–375. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.4.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muri RM, Rivaud S, et al. Role of the prefrontal cortex in the control of express saccades. A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:199–206. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00094-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nardone R, Bergmann J, et al. Effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of Alzheimer Disease: A review. Int. J. Alzheimer's Disease. Article ID. 2012;687909:5p. doi: 10.1155/2012/687909. 2012. doi:10.1155/2012/687909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveri M, Zhaoping L, Mangano GR, Turriziani P, Smirni D, Cipolotti L. Facilitation of bottom-up feature detection following rTMS-interference of the right parietal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(4):1003–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Reardon JP, Solvason HB, et al. Efficacy and safety of transcranial magnetic stimulation in the acute treatment of major depression: a multisite randomized controlled trial. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62:1208–1216. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.01.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parasurman R, Rizzo M. Neuroergonomics: The Brain at Work. Oxford University Press; New York, NY.: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone A, Valls-Sollé J, Wassermann EM, Brasil-Neto J, Cohen LG, Hallett M. Effects of focal transcranial magnetic stimulation on simple reaction time to acoustic, visual and somatosensory stimuli. Brain. 1992;115:1045–1059. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.4.1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone A, Houser CM, et al. Safety of rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation in normal volunteers, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1993;89:120–130. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90094-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone A, Valls-Sole J, Wassermann EM, Hallett M. Responses to rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex. Brain. 1994;117:847–858. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.4.847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone A, Hallett M. Induction of errors in a delayed response task by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. NeuroReport. 1994;5:2517–2520. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199412000-00028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone A, Bartres-Faz D, Keenan JP. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: studying the brain-behaviour relationship by induction of ‘virtual lesions.’. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1999;354:1229–1238. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0476. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peinemann A, Reimer B, Loer C, Quartarone A, Munchau A, Conrad B, Siebner HR. Long-lasting increase in corticospinal excitability after 1800 pulses of subthreshold 5Hz repetitive TMS to the primary motor cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;115(7):1519–1526. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterchev AV, Murphy DL, Lisanby SH. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulator with controllable pulse parameters. J. Neural. Eng. 2011;8:036016. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/3/036016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragert P, Dinse HR, et al. Combination of 5 Hz repetitive transcranial stimulation (rTMS) and tactile coactivation boosts tactile discrimination in humans. Neurosci. Letters. 2003;348:105–108. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00745-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ragert P, Becker M, Tegenthoff M, Pleger B, Dinse HR. Sustained increase of somatosensory cortex excitability by 5 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation studied by paired median nerve stimulation in humans. Neurosci. Letters. 2004;356:91–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reithler J, Peters JC, Sack AT. Multimodal transcranial magnetic stimulation: using concurrent neuroimaging to reveal the neural network dynamics of noninvasive brain stimulation. Prog. Neurobiol. 2011;94:149–165. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renzi C, Vecchi T, Silvanto J, Cattaneo Z. Overlapping representations of numerical magnitude and motion direction in the posterior parietal cortex: A TMS-adaptation study. Neurosci. Lett. 2011;490:145–149. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romei V, Rihs T, Brodbeck V, Thut G. Resting electroencephalogram alpha-power over posterior sites indexes baseline visual cortex excitability. NeuroReport. 2008;19:203–208. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f454c4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romei V, Gross J, Thut G. On the role of prestimulus alpha rhythms over occipto-parietal areas in visual input regulation: Correlation or causation? J. Neurosci. 2010;30:8692–8697. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0160-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romei V, Driver J, Schyns PG, Thut G. Rhythmic TMS over parietal cortex links distinct brain frequencies to global versus local visual processing. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:334–337. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rushworth MFS, Passingham RE, Nobre AC. Components of switching intentional set. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002;14:1139–1150. doi: 10.1162/089892902760807159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruzzoli M, Marzi CA, Miniussi C. The neural mechanisms of the effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on perception. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103:2982–2989. doi: 10.1152/jn.01096.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruzzoli M, Abrahamyan A, Clifford CWG, Marzi CA, Miniussi C, Harris JA. The effect of TMS on visual motion sensitivity: an increase in neural noise or a decrease in signal strength? J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:138–143. doi: 10.1152/jn.00746.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sack AT, Cohen Kadosh R, Schuhmann T, Moerel M, Walsh V, Goebel R. Optimizing functional accuracy of TMS in cognitive studies: a comparison of methods. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009;21:207–221. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai KL, Noguchi Y, Takeuchi T, Watanabe E. Selective priming of syntactic processing of event-related transcranial magnetic stimulation of Broca's Area. Neuron. 2002;35:1177–1182. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00873-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandburg A, Bostrom N. Converging cognitive enhancements. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 2006;1093:201–227. doi: 10.1196/annals.1382.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnitzler A, Gross J. Normal and pathological oscillatory communication in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005;6:285–296. doi: 10.1038/nrn1650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schutter DJ, van Honk J. Increased positive emotional memory after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the orbitofrontal cortex. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2006;31(2):101–104. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzkopf DS, Silvanto J, Rees G. Stochastic resonance effects reveal the neural mechanisms of transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:3143–3147. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4863-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seyal M, Ro T, Rafal R. Increased sensitivity to ipsilateral cutaneous stimuli following transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal lobe. Annals Neurol. 1995;38:264–267. doi: 10.1002/ana.410380221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siebner HR. Preconditioning of low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation with transcranial direct current stimulation: evidence for homeostatic plasticity in the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:3379–3385. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5316-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silva S, Basser PJ, Miranda PC. Elucidating the mechanisms and loci of neuronal excitation by transcranial magnetic stimulation using a finite element model of cortical sulcus. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008;119:2405–2413. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvanto J, Muggleton N. New light through old windows: Moving beyond the “virtual lesion” approach to transcranial magnetic stimulation. NeuroImage. 2008;39:549–552. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvanto J, Muggleton N, Walsh V. State-dependency in brain stimulation studies of perception and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008a;12:447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvanto J, Cattaneo Z, Batteli L, Pascual-Leone A. Baseline cortical excitability determines whether TMS disrupts or facilitates behavior. J. Neurophysiol. 2008b;99:2725–2730. doi: 10.1152/jn.01392.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sirota A, Montgomery S, Fujisawa S, Isomura Y, Zugaro M, Buzsaki G. Entrainment of neocortical neurons and gamma oscillations by the hippocampal theta rhythm. Neuron. 2009;60:683–697. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder AW, Bahramali H, Hawker T, Mitchell DJ. Savant-like numerosity skills revealed in normal people by magnetic pulses. Perception. 2006;35:837–845. doi: 10.1068/p5539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder AW. Explaining and inducing savant skills: privileged access to lower level, less-processed information. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2009;364:1399–1405. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sole-Padulles C, Bartres-Faz D, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation effects on brain function and cognition among elders with memory dysfunction. A randomized sham-controlled study. Cereb. Cortex. 2006;16:1487–1493. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparing R, Mottaghy FM, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation effects on language function depend on the stimulation parameters. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001;18(4):326–330. doi: 10.1097/00004691-200107000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefan K, Kunesch E, Cohen LG, Benecke R, Classen J. Induction of plasticity in the human motor cortex by paired associative stimulation. Brain. 2000;123:572–584. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.3.572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoeckel C, Gough PM, Watkins KE, Devlin JT. Supramarginal gyrus involvement in visual word recognition. Cortex. 2009;45:1091–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tadin D, Silvanto J, Pascual-Leone A, Battelli L. Improved motion perception and impaired spatial suppression following disruption of cortical area MT/V5. J. Neurosci. 2011;4:1279–1283. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4121-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi N, Chuma T, Matsuo Y, Watanabe I, Ikoma K. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of contralesional primary motor cortex improves hand function after stroke. Stroke. 2005;36:2681–2686. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000189658.51972.34. 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tegenthoff M, Ragert P, et al. Improvement of tactile discrimination performance and enlargement of cortical somatosensory maps after 5 Hz rTMS. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e362. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terao Y, Ugawa Y, et al. Shortening of simple reaction time by peripheral electrical and submotor-threshold magnetic cortical stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 1997;115(3):541–545. doi: 10.1007/pl00005724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thickbroom GW. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and synaptic plasticity: experimental framework and human models. Exp. Brain Res. 2007;180:583–593. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0991-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thielscher A, Opitz A, Windhoff M. Impact of the gyral geometry on the electric field induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. NeuroImage. 2011;54:234–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thut G, Nietzel A, Pascual-Leone A. Dorsal posterior parietal rTMS affects voluntary orienting of visuospatial attention. Cereb. Cortex. 2005;15(5):628–638. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thut G, Pascual-Leone A. A review of combined TMS-EEG studies to characterize lasting effects of repetitive TMS and assess their usefulness in cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Brain Topogr. 2009;22:219–232. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0115-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thut G, Veniero D, Romei V, Miniussi C, Schyna P, Gross J. Rhythmic TMS causes local entrainment of natural oscillatory signatures. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:1176–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Topper R, Mottaghy FM, Brugmann M, Noth J, Huber W. Faciliation of picture naming by focal transcranial magnetic stimulation of Wernicke's area. Exp. Brain Res. 1998;121:371–378. doi: 10.1007/s002210050471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Touge T, Gerschlager W, Brown P, Rothwell JC. Are the after-effects of low-frequency rTMS on motor cortex excitability due to changes in the efficacy of cortical synapses? Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001;112:2138–2145. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00651-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Der Werf YD, Paus T. The neural response to transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human motor cortex. I. Intracortical and cortico-cortical contributions. Exp. Brain Res. 2006;175:231–245. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0551-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Ettinger-Veenstra HM, Huijbers W, Gutteling TP, Vink M, Kenemans JL, Neggers SFW. fMRI-guided TMS on cortical eye fields: The frontal but not intraparietal eye fields regulate the coupling between visuospatial attention and eye movements. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;102:3469–3480. doi: 10.1152/jn.00350.2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh V, Ellison A, Battelli L, Cowey A. Task-specific impairments and enhancements induced by magnetic stimulation of human visual area V5. Proc. Biol. Sci. 1998;265:537–543. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang R-Y, Tseng H-Y, Liao K-K, Wang C-J, Lai K-L, Yang Y-R. rTMS combined with task-oriented training to improve symmetry of interhemispheric corticomotor excitability and gait performance after stroke: A randomized trial. Neurorehab Neural Repair. 2012;26:222–230. doi: 10.1177/1545968311423265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wassermann EM, Grafman J, et al. Use and safety of a new repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulator, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1996;101:412–417. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waterston ML, Pack CC. Improved discrimination of visual stimuli following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(4):1–10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wipfli M, Felblinger J, Mosimann UP, Hess CW, Schlaepfer TE, Muri RM. Double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the frontal eye field facilitates triggering of memory-guided saccades. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;14:571–575. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01671.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolters A, Sandbrink F, Schlottmann A. A temporally asymmetric Hebbian rule governing plasticity in the human motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2339–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.00900.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamanaka K, Yamagata B, Tomioka H, Kawasaki S, Mimura M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal cortex facilitates spatial working memory: Near infrared spectroscopy study. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:1037–1045. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]