Significance
The ability to form episodic memories in rapid succession depends on the hippocampus, but how do hippocampal neurons represent such memories? Most neurocomputational models envision a sparse distributed coding scheme in which individual neurons each participate in the coding of a few memories, and each memory is coded by a small fraction of hippocampal neurons. We investigated this issue with epilepsy patients undergoing intracranial monitoring and found evidence consistent with sparse distributed coding in the hippocampus. These findings shed new light on the basic neural mechanisms that underlie the ability to remember events. A detailed characterization of those mechanisms is an essential part of the larger effort to understand memory loss associated with normal aging and dementia.
Keywords: recognition memory, intracranial recording, amygdala
Abstract
Neurocomputational models hold that sparse distributed coding is the most efficient way for hippocampal neurons to encode episodic memories rapidly. We investigated the representation of episodic memory in hippocampal neurons of nine epilepsy patients undergoing intracranial monitoring as they discriminated between recently studied words (targets) and new words (foils) on a recognition test. On average, single units and multiunits exhibited higher spike counts in response to targets relative to foils, and the size of this effect correlated with behavioral performance. Further analyses of the spike-count distributions revealed that (i) a small percentage of recorded neurons responded to any one target and (ii) a small percentage of targets elicited a strong response in any one neuron. These findings are consistent with the idea that in the human hippocampus episodic memory is supported by a sparse distributed neural code.
The hippocampus is known to play a fundamental role in declarative memory (1–4), but it is not known how mnemonic information is coded by the activity of individual hippocampal neurons. At least three different coding schemes have been considered: a localist coding scheme, a fully distributed coding scheme, and a sparse distributed coding scheme (5). In a localist coding scheme, an individual neuron (sometimes referred to as a “grandmother cell”) codes only one memory, and each memory is coded by the activity of only one neuron. In a fully distributed coding scheme, each memory is coded instead by a pattern of activity across many hippocampal neurons. Falling between these two extremes is a sparse distributed coding scheme in which each memory is coded by the activity of a small proportion of hippocampal neurons, and each neuron contributes to the representation of only a few memories. Sparse distributed coding has long been hypothesized to be the most efficient way for hippocampal neurons to encode episodic memories (remembering events) in rapid succession without overwriting previously stored memories (6–8).
Most prior work concerned with the coding of declarative memory in the human hippocampus has focused on the neural representation of semantic memories (remembering facts), such as memory for famous people or landmarks (9, 10). The results of these studies suggest that long-established semantic memories may be represented by fewer than 1% of neurons in the hippocampus (11). However, neurocomputational theories are concerned with the representation of episodic memories. The purpose of our study was to test predictions of these neurocomputational theories about how episodic memories are represented by neurons of the hippocampus.
The representation of episodic memory in the hippocampus typically has been investigated using recognition procedures. In recognition, the task is to discriminate between familiar items presented earlier in the experimental session (targets) and novel items not previously presented (foils). An episodic memory signal is evident when neurons exhibit different levels of activity for targets (old items) vs. foils (new items). The first recognition studies with humans (12, 13) and monkeys (14–16) failed to detect evidence of episodic memory in neurons of the hippocampus, but more recent studies have identified hippocampal neurons that differentiate targets from foils (17–21). However, these studies did not investigate how the representation of individual targets is distributed across neurons of the hippocampus. Instead, the aim was to find cells that distinguish the class of targets from the class of foils.
We investigated the representation of individual targets in neurons of the human hippocampus. The participants were nine patients with pharmaco-resistant epilepsy requiring the implantation of intracranial wire electrodes for clinical evaluation and localization of seizure foci for possible surgical resection. Among them, the patients completed a total of 18 recognition memory tasks in which they first studied 32 words and then attempted to distinguish between the 32 targets that had appeared on the study list and 32 foils that had not. Each of the 64 items on the recognition test was presented only once, a format that differs from many other neurophysiology studies that present individual stimuli multiple times to identify neurons with reliable stimulus-specific firing properties. The multiple-presentation method is well-suited to the study of semantic memory (e.g., a neuron that is found to respond reliably to six presentations of the word “baby” likely is responding to its long-established semantic meaning) but is not well-suited to the study of episodic memory. When targets and foils are presented only once on a recognition test, the targets, but not the foils, are represented by an episodic memory formed earlier at the time of learning. Under these conditions, any difference in neural activity associated with targets and foils would indicate episodic memory. Note that, if the test items were presented again, the targets and foils no longer would be clearly differentiated because even the foils would be represented by a recently formed, context-specific episodic memory. Accordingly, instead of using multiple stimulus presentations during the recognition test, we examined the distribution of activity associated with once-presented targets vs. once-presented foils across all recorded neurons. The different coding schemes under consideration here make distinct predictions about the expected distributions of neural activity.
Results
Behavioral Data.
Recognition decisions were made using an 8-point confidence scale (1 = Sure New … 8 = Sure Old). Confidence ratings of 5 through 8 were counted as hits for target words and as false alarms for foils. Hit rate, false alarm rate, percent correct, and discriminability (d′) scores were computed for each patient. For patients who completed more than one recognition test, these measures were computed separately for each test and then averaged. All nine patients exhibited above-chance memory for every test (mean = 63% correct), although the performance for patient 8 was close to chance (Table S1).
Reaction time (RT) was defined as the interval between the onset of a test item and the mouse click indicating the confidence rating for that test item. All confidence ratings were made 1,500 ms or more after the presentation of the test item, that is, after the occurrence of the spikes counts that were analyzed here (200–1,000 ms after test stimulus presentation; see Fig. S1).
Neural Data.
We recorded neural activity bilaterally from 220 units in the hippocampus (34 single units + 186 multiunits) and 300 units in the amygdala (68 single units + 232 multiunits) over the course of the 18 recognition memory tests. In the hippocampus, the mean firing rate of the 34 single units during the prestimulus baseline period (1,000–200 ms before stimulus presentation) was 1.7 Hz (SD = 2.76), and the mean firing rate of the 186 multiunits was 21.2 Hz (SD = 10.32). In the amygdala, the mean firing rate of the 68 single units during the prestimulus baseline period was 2.0 Hz (SD = 2.02), and the mean firing rate of the 232 multiunits was 20.2 Hz (SD = 11.77). Before analysis, poststimulus spike counts for each unit (200–1,000 ms after test stimulus presentation) were normalized based on the mean and SD of the unit's spike counts during the prestimulus baseline period across all 64 test items.
The normalized spike counts were subjected to three levels of analysis. First, for each patient, we measured the difference between test period activity for targets vs. foils averaged across all units recorded from the hippocampus and, separately, from the amygdala, and we compared that neural difference score with a standard behavioral measure of recognition memory performance (d′). Second, we analyzed the activity of individual units in the hippocampus and the amygdala to identify those for which the average activity level across the 32 targets differed significantly from the average activity level across the 32 foils. Third, at the most fine-grained level of analysis, we compared the distribution of all spike counts recorded for individual targets and compared it with the distribution of all spike counts recorded for individual foils. This analysis most directly addresses the question of how individual episodic memories are represented by neurons of the human hippocampus.
In the first (patient-level) analysis, for each patient (i), a single difference score (D′i) was computed consisting of the mean of all normalized spike counts to targets, μTargeti, minus the mean of all normalized spike counts to foils, μFoili. As an example, 17 hippocampal units (six single units and 11 multiunits) were recorded from patient 3 in one recognition test session. Because the recognition test consisted of 32 targets and 32 foils, 544 normalized scores were averaged to compute μTarget3 (i.e., 17 units × 32 targets = 544 target scores), and 544 normalized scores were averaged to compute μFoil3 (i.e., 17 units × 32 foils = 544 foil scores). The individual neural difference score for patient 3 (D′3) was equal to μTarget3 – μFoil3. D′ scores for all nine patients were computed using a similar procedure. For patients who completed more than one recognition test, these measures were computed separately for each test and then averaged. Using the same procedure, a second D′ score was computed for each patient based on recordings made from the amygdala. The results indicated the detection of an episodic memory signal in the hippocampus but not in the amygdala (Fig. 1A). Moreover, for recordings made from the hippocampus, the larger the difference between the average normalized spike counts between targets and foils for a particular patient, the better recognition memory performance was for that patient as measured by d′ (Fig. 1C). When this same test was performed on recordings from the amygdala, no such relationship was observed (Fig. 1D). These findings are consistent with prior work showing that hippocampal lesions impair recognition memory performance, whereas amygdala lesions do not (1), but they differ from other single-unit studies that have detected episodic memory signals in the amygdala (19, 20).
In the second (unit-level) analysis, we identified units with spike counts that significantly differentiated targets from foils. For each unit j, where j = 1–220 in the hippocampus and j = 1–300 in the amygdala, a t test was performed comparing the mean normalized spike count across the 32 targets (μTargetj) with the mean normalized spike count across the 32 foils (μFoilj). Under the null hypothesis of no difference, and assuming independence, 5% of these tests would be significant on the basis of chance alone. In our data, only 12 of the 220 units (5.5%) in the hippocampus and only 8 of the 300 units (2.7%) in the amygdala significantly differentiated targets from foils, effects that occurred no more often than would be expected from chance. A similar result was obtained when the analysis was limited to the single units. Only 1 of the 34 single units recorded from the hippocampus (2.9%) significantly differentiated targets from foils, and none of the 68 single units recorded from the amygdala did so. Thus, analyzed in this fashion, the data offer no evidence of a neural episodic memory signal in the hippocampus or amygdala.
The apparent absence of an episodic memory signal at the level of individual units in the hippocampus parallels earlier results from similar episodic memory tests with epilepsy patients (12, 13) and with monkeys (16). However, an an episodic memory signal was evident in our data when the spike counts from the hippocampus were aggregated across units (Fig. 1 A and C). This apparent contradiction is resolved by analyzing the distribution of 220 t scores, instead of simply counting the number of units associated with a significant t score. A null effect would be characterized by a distribution of t scores with a mean of zero. However, when the full distribution of 220 t scores was analyzed, the mean was slightly but significantly greater than 0 (Fig. 2A). Similarly, the mean of the 34 t scores for the single units considered separately also was significantly greater than 0 (Fig. 2B). Although the distribution of t scores was elevated above 0 in the hippocampus, no such effects were evident in the amygdala (i.e., the mean of the t distribution did not differ significantly from 0).
The numerator of the t-score formula for a particular unit j is μTargetj − μFoilj. Thus, the results shown in Fig. 2 A and B suggest that the entire distribution of μTargetj values was significantly elevated relative to the distribution of μFoilj values, and indeed this was the case (Fig. 2 C and D). These results demonstrate that even though the number of units with significant t scores was no greater than would be expected on the basis of chance, most or all of the units were involved in the coding of episodic memory. In that sense, a distributed memory signal was identified, arguing against the notion of a strictly localist (i.e., “grandmother” cell) coding scheme. Under a localist scheme, the likelihood of detecting any memory signal would be extremely low. However, although a distributed signal was identified (Fig. 2 C and D), this analysis does not distinguish between a fully distributed coding scheme (Fig. 3A) and a sparse distributed coding scheme (Fig. 3B).
In the third and most detailed analysis, we investigated whether the significant increase in the μTargetj values in hippocampal neurons (Fig. 2 C and D) occurred because, for each unit, most or all of the 32 targets exhibited elevated spiking relative to the foils (consistent with a fully distributed coding scheme), or because, for each unit, only a few targets elicited elevated spiking relative to the foils (consistent with a sparse distributed coding scheme). To address this issue, we compared the full distribution of individual normalized spike counts for the targets with the full distribution of normalized spike counts for the foils. For the analysis involving all units, there were 7,040 individual target spike counts (32 targets × 220 units) and 7,040 individual foil spike counts (32 foils × 220 units). For a separate analysis involving only single units, there were 1,088 individual target spike counts (32 targets × 34 single units) and 1,088 individual foil spike counts (32 foils × 34 single units). If a modest memory signal were added to all the targets (a fully distributed coding scheme; Fig. 3A), then, for both analyses, the entire target distribution would be shifted slightly rightward relative to the foil distribution. If, instead, a small proportion of the targets generated an especially strong response in a small proportion of neurons, with the large majority of targets eliciting no differential response relative to foils (a sparse distributed coding scheme; Fig. 3B), then a bimodal target distribution of normalized spike counts should be observed. That is, the large majority of target spike counts should coincide with the distribution of foil spike counts, but a small percentage of targets should be drawn from a distribution with a much higher mean.
The spike-count frequency distributions (Fig. 4 A and B) do not show clear visual evidence of a bimodal distribution for the targets. However, the bimodal target distribution predicted by the sparse distributed account would be difficult to detect visually in a frequency distribution because only a few targets would be expected to yield values associated with the upper distribution, and those few values would not necessarily be tightly organized in a visually apparent distribution. Some targets (and fewer foils) do fall in the far right tails of the frequency distributions. To determine whether the target distributions in Fig. 4 A and B are slightly right-shifted or instead are bimodal, we constructed empirical quantile–quantile (Q–Q) plots (22).
An empirical Q–Q plot simply displays one rank-ordered dataset (i.e., the sorted normalized spike counts for the targets) against another rank-ordered dataset (i.e., the sorted normalized spike counts for the foils). This graphical analysis method provides more accurate information about the relative shapes of two distributions than can be obtained from a visual inspection of the frequency distributions alone. A shifted distribution would yield a linear pattern of scores elevated above the diagonal line (illustrated with hypothetical data in Fig. 4C), whereas a bimodal distribution would yield a pattern of scores characterized by a sharp departure from the diagonal line (illustrated with hypothetical data in Fig. 4D). The empirical Q–Q plots (Fig. 4 E and F) show clear evidence of a bimodal distribution for the targets, as predicted by a sparse distributed coding scheme (Fig. 3B).
We next tested whether the apparent departure from a shifted function in the Q–Q plots was statistically significant. Specifically, we tested how often a departure that large would have occurred if the target distribution were simply right-shifted and not bimodal. To do so, we conducted a bootstrap analysis of the normalized spike count data. One bootstrap analysis was performed on the data from all units combined, and a second bootstrap analysis was performed only on the data from the single units. On each iteration of the bootstrap analysis, a foil distribution was constructed by randomly sampling (with replacement) n normalized foil spike count scores (n = 7,040 for the full analysis, and n = 1088 for the single unit analysis). Next, a target distribution was constructed by independently sampling a second set of n normalized foil spike count scores and then adding a constant α to each score. The value of α was set to the average difference between the target and foil distributions in the empirical data (α = 0.06 for the full analysis, and α = 0.10 for the single units). Except for random error introduced by the sampling-with-replacement process, the target distribution created in this manner was not bimodal but instead was right-shifted relative to the scores used to represent the foil distribution. A Q–Q plot then was constructed for the hypothetical foil and target values, and a statistic was formed by computing the sum of the squared differences between the paired target and foil values. Ten thousand bootstrap iterations were run, and the proportion of these iterations in which the sum-of-squared differences was larger than the sum-of-squared differences in the empirical data was calculated. The results showed that sum-of-squared differences as large as observed in the empirical data occurred with probability 0.004 in the bootstrap trials for all of the units and with probability 0.011 in the bootstrap trials for the single units. Thus, the apparent evidence for a bimodal target distribution in the Q–Q plots is unlikely to have occurred by chance. The results of these bootstrap statistical tests were nearly identical when α was a random variable (instead of being a constant) drawn from exponential distributions with means of 0.06 and 0.10 for the full and single unit analyses, respectively.
For the Q–Q plot with all units combined (Fig. 4E), there are ∼112 target values (1.6% of the 7,040 target-by-unit spike counts) that account for the upward trending portion of the curve that begins at ∼2.5 on the x and y axes (indicated by the dashed gray lines). In the single-unit Q–Q plot (Fig. 4F), there are ∼30 target values (2.8% of the 1,088 target-by-unit spike counts) that account for the upward trending portion of the curve. For both plots, the remaining target values (>97%) appear to be coincident with the foil values. Such a small percentage of target-by-unit spike counts exhibiting elevated activity is consistent with the percentage of active units in hippocampal subfields in the rat, which have been found to range from 0.5% in dentate gyrus to 2.5% in CA1 and CA3 (23).
In Fig. 4F, the ∼30 elevated target-by-unit spike counts are spread across the 34 single units. Seven of the nine patients and 22 different single units are represented in the top 30 single-unit target responses. Two targets appear four times (“shallow” and “hand”), one target appears three times (“family”), two targets appear twice (“organ” and “sweat”), and 15 targets appear once (a total of 20 different words). Thus, generally speaking, each single unit was responsive to only a few targets, the pattern that is anticipated by the sparse distributed account (Fig. 3B). Note that the strong responses in some units to these target items cannot be attributed to random spiking activity because they occurred significantly more often in response to targets than to foils.
Despite their critical contribution to the significantly elevated single-unit t distribution (Fig. 2B), these 30 normalized spike counts were not associated with what usually would be regarded as a large increase in the absolute level of spiking activity relative to baseline. For example, for the 30 target-by-unit normalized spikes found to be elevated during the test period, the average number of raw spikes increased from 2.3 during the 800-ms prestimulus baseline period to 4.1 during the 800-ms poststimulus test period. This increase represents a difference of only 1.8 spikes, not even double the baseline count (see Fig. S2 for a representative raster plot).
Discussion
Several previous studies in humans and monkeys failed to identify any memory-related neurons in the hippocampus (e.g., 12, 15). Other studies identified only a few such neurons. For example, a continuous recognition study in the macaque (24) found that only 2.3% of hippocampal neurons (15 of 660) significantly differentiated repeated items from nonrepeated items. Similarly, we found that only 2.9% of the single units we recorded (1 of 34) significantly differentiated targets from foils. Findings such as these have been taken to mean that a small proportion of hippocampal neurons is involved in recognition, but our findings suggest otherwise. Even though only a few neurons yielded statistically significant differences in their firing rates in response to targets vs. foils, most or all of the single units we recorded appeared to be involved in recognition memory for at least some of the target items. Their involvement was suggested by the shift of the entire distribution of t scores to above 0 (Fig. 2 A and B). By itself, that finding does not distinguish between the three coding schemes under consideration here (localist, fully distributed and sparse distributed), although, as noted earlier, the mere fact that we detected neural evidence of a memory signal weighs against a strictly localist scheme. A further analysis of the individual target and foil spike-count distributions—an analysis that has not been performed in prior studies—showed that our data (Fig. 4 E and F) accord with predictions made by the sparse distributed coding scheme illustrated in Fig. 3B.
Although studies often do not find a greater-than-expected number of hippocampal neurons involved in episodic memory, some previous studies have identified hippocampal neurons that significantly differentiated targets from foils in numbers that were greater than would be expected on the basis of chance. This result has been observed in studies with humans (19, 20) and monkeys (21). However, these studies did not examine how spiking activity was distributed across test items, so it is not known if the neurons were responding to a general class of items (e.g., to all previously seen items) or to a relatively small subset of items that happened to generate strong enough responses to yield significant t tests. Conceivably, a bimodal distribution of spiking activity occurred in those studies as well (consistent with sparse distributed coding), in which case those findings would accord with the results reported here.
Another difference between the results of our study and those of previous studies is that we found no evidence of neurons responding to stimulus novelty (i.e., a stronger response to foils than to targets), but several prior studies have reported this effect (19–21). A possible explanation for this discrepancy is that the stimuli used in prior studies consisted of unfamiliar pictures, whereas the stimuli we used consisted of familiar words. Presumably, a novelty response is more likely to be detected when the foils are truly novel. This difference in stimulus materials (pictures vs. words) also may explain why prior studies have detected an episodic memory signal in the amygdala as well as in the hippocampus (19, 20), whereas we detected an episodic signal only in the hippocampus.
The pattern of results shown in Fig. 4 E and F indicates sparse distributed coding of episodic memory, and the pattern was the same whether the analysis was based largely on multiunits (Fig. 4E) or was limited to single units (Fig. 4F). In both cases, a bimodal distribution of spiking activity associated with recently encoded targets was observed, with the upper distribution consisting of a small percentage of recorded target activity. The most straightforward interpretation of why single units and multiunits exhibit the same pattern is that the target items are represented in the hippocampus by distributed clusters of localized neural activity. Under those conditions, a bimodal target distribution would be evident for single units and multiunits alike.
Previous work with humans has suggested that the representation of semantic memory in the hippocampus is relatively sparse (11). In addition, one study (10) found that episodic memory of a particular video clip (tested using recall) was preceded by the selective reactivation of the same neuron that had reliably responded to the presentation of that clip on an earlier test of semantic memory. This finding suggested that episodic memory (like semantic memory) might be represented by a small fraction of highly selective hippocampal neurons. However, episodic memory generally involves the retrieval of both specific episodic details (e.g., memory for context) as well as general semantic knowledge (25, 26). Thus, the neural activity measured when a clip was recalled could easily reflect the same semantic memory signal that was activated by the initial presentation of the clip. The goal of our study was to measure neural activity associated with a series of recently encoded memories that were unambiguously episodic in nature and that were formed rapidly on a single learning trial (and then tested only once).
Most neurocomputational models dating back to Marr (6) hold that episodic memory representations in the dentate gyrus/CA3 region of the hippocampus are supported by a sparse distributed neural code (Fig. 3B). Although our electrodes were not localized to particular hippocampal subfields, our findings nevertheless are consistent with this idea. Other evidence consistent with a sparse code in the hippocampus has been reported in studies using rats (e.g., 27, 28). However, these studies involved tasks in which memories were acquired over an extended period (allowing for the development of place fields), not tasks in which multiple memories were formed in rapid succession on a single trial. The same is true of prior evidence for sparse coding of semantic memory in the human medial temporal lobe (11). Our findings suggest that, as has long been predicted, rapidly formed episodic memories are supported by a sparse distributed code in the human hippocampus.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
The participants were nine patients with drug-resistant epilepsy requiring the implantation of depth electrodes (Ad-Tech Medical) for clinical evaluation and consideration of possible surgical resection of their seizure focus. The mean age of the patients was 39 y (range 19–50 y), five were female, eight were right-handed, and all had temporal lobe epilepsy. All patients provided informed consent to participate in the research using a protocol that was approved by the Institutional Review Board of St. Joseph’s Hospital and Medical Center.
Materials.
Stimuli for the experimental trials consisted of 192 words taken from the Medical Research Council Psycholinguistic database (29), three to seven letters in length, with a range in concreteness rating of 550–700. Half of the words (i.e., 96) were high-frequency words, and half were low-frequency words. The 192 words were randomly divided into three unique sets for each patient (64 words per set). Each set consisted of 32 targets (words that would appear on the study list and again on the recognition test) and 32 foils (words that would appear only on the recognition test) with equal representation of high- and low-frequency words. Therefore, participants could perform up to three recognition memory study/test cycles with different words.
Memory Task.
Participants were told that they would be presented with a series of words and that, following the presentation of the list, their memory would be tested. During the study phase, a trial began with a fixation cross that appeared in the center of the computer screen for 750 ms, followed by the presentation of a word for 2 s. Half of the study words were presented at the top of the screen, and the other half were at the bottom, randomly selected on each trial for each participant.
During the test phase, 2 min later, 32 targets and 32 foils were presented in a randomly determined order. Test trials began with a 450-ms fixation cross, which appeared in the center of the screen, followed by a centrally presented test word. After 500 ms, a confidence rating scale appeared at the bottom of the screen, with boxes labeled from 1 (very sure new) to 8 (very sure old). To indicate their memory decision, participants clicked one of the boxes using a computer mouse. When responses were equal to or greater than 5 (“old” decision), participants then made a Remember-Know-Guess (R-K-G) judgment about their subjective memory experience. All R-K-G judgments were followed by “source” judgments, in which participants clicked one of two boxes (labeled “top” and “bottom”) to indicate whether the word had been studied at the top or the bottom of the screen. The confidence ratings, R-K-G judgments, and source memory decisions are not directly relevant to the issue of sparse vs. distributed coding and are not analyzed here. All responses were self-paced.
Participants received practice trials to familiarize themselves with the task. A session (involving a single 32-item list and a 64-item recognition test) required ∼20 min to complete. Four participants completed three recognition memory tests in separate sessions, one participant completed two tests in separate sessions, and the remaining four participants completed one test (Table S1). Thus, there were 18 recognition tests in all. Different sets of words were used for each test.
Microwire Recordings.
Microwire implantation, recording, and spike-sorting details are described in SI Materials and Methods. The test period during which spike counts were recorded (200–1,000 ms after the presentation of the test item) was chosen because a previous study (9) found that selective responses of hippocampal neurons began ∼300 ms after stimulus presentation and because the large majority of behavioral responses occur after 1 s. Fig. S3 shows waveforms for a single unit recorded from the hippocampus.
Because some patients received up to three recognition memory tests on three separate days, it is possible, although unlikely, that some units contributed to recordings made in more than one session (the microwires are fixed to the skull 3–4 cm from their tips). However, each recognition test involved an entirely different set of words for both targets and foils. In addition, our analyses were not based on selected units (30). Thus, our findings were not disproportionately influenced by the activity of selected units that might have been responsive to particular words.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank the patients for participating in these experiments. We also thank Tom Albright for comments on an earlier version of this paper and Elaine Cabrales for technical assistance. This work was supported by the Medical Research Service of the Department of Veterans Affairs, National Institute of Mental Health Grant 24600, National Institute on Deafness and Other Communications Disorders Grant 1R21DC009781, the Barrow Neurological Foundation, and the University of California, San Diego Kavli Institute for Brain and Mind.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1408365111/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Squire LR, Zola-Morgan S. The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science. 1991;253(5026):1380–1386. doi: 10.1126/science.1896849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Eichenbaum H, Cohen NJ. From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Memory Systems of the Brain. New York: Oxford Univ Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev. 1992;99(2):195–231. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Squire LR, Wixted JT. The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:259–288. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rolls ET, Treves A. The neuronal encoding of information in the brain. Prog Neurobiol. 2011;95(3):448–490. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Marr D. Simple memory: A theory for archicortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1971;262(841):23–81. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev. 1995;102(3):419–457. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Norman KA, O’Reilly RC. Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: A complementary-learning-systems approach. Psychol Rev. 2003;110(4):611–646. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Quiroga RQ, Reddy L, Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature. 2005;435(7045):1102–1107. doi: 10.1038/nature03687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gelbard-Sagiv H, Mukamel R, Harel M, Malach R, Fried I. Internally generated reactivation of single neurons in human hippocampus during free recall. Science. 2008;322(5898):96–101. doi: 10.1126/science.1164685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Waydo S, Kraskov A, Quian Quiroga R, Fried I, Koch C. Sparse representation in the human medial temporal lobe. J Neurosci. 2006;26(40):10232–10234. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2101-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Heit G, Smith ME, Halgren E. Neural encoding of individual words and faces by the human hippocampus and amygdala. Nature. 1988;333(6175):773–775. doi: 10.1038/333773a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Heit G, Smith ME, Halgren E. Neuronal activity in the human medial temporal lobe during recognition memory. Brain. 1990;113(Pt 4):1093–1112. doi: 10.1093/brain/113.4.1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brown MWF, Wilson FA, Riches IP. Neuronal evidence that inferomedial temporal cortex is more important than hippocampus in certain processes underlying recognition memory. Brain Res. 1987;409(1):158–162. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90753-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Riches IP, Wilson FAW, Brown MW. The effects of visual stimulation and memory on neurons of the hippocampal formation and the neighboring parahippocampal gyrus and inferior temporal cortex of the primate. J Neurosci. 1991;11(6):1763–1779. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-06-01763.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Xiang JZ, Brown MW. Differential neuronal encoding of novelty, familiarity and recency in regions of the anterior temporal lobe. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37(4-5):657–676. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00030-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Fried I, MacDonald KA, Wilson CL. Single neuron activity in human hippocampus and amygdala during recognition of faces and objects. Neuron. 1997;18(5):753–765. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80315-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Viskontas IV, Knowlton BJ, Steinmetz PN, Fried I. Differences in mnemonic processing by neurons in the human hippocampus and parahippocampal regions. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18(10):1654–1662. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rutishauser U, Mamelak AN, Schuman EM. Single-trial learning of novel stimuli by individual neurons of the human hippocampus-amygdala complex. Neuron. 2006;49(6):805–813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rutishauser U, Schuman EM, Mamelak AN. Activity of human hippocampal and amygdala neurons during retrieval of declarative memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(1):329–334. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706015105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jutras MJ, Buffalo EA. Recognition memory signals in the macaque hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(1):401–406. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908378107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chambers JM, Cleveland WS, Kleiner B, Tukey PA. Graphical Methods for Data Analysis. Pacific Grove, CA: Wadsworth; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 23.O’Reilly RC, McClelland JL. Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus. 1994;4(6):661–682. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rolls ET, Cahusac PMB, Feigenbaum JD, Miyashita Y. Responses of single neurons in the hippocampus of the macaque related to recognition memory. Exp Brain Res. 1993;93(2):299–306. doi: 10.1007/BF00228398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hemmer P, Steyvers M. Integrating episodic memories and prior knowledge at multiple levels of abstraction. Psychon Bull Rev. 2009;16(1):80–87. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Nelson AB, Shiffrin RM. The co-evolution of knowledge and event memory. Psychol Rev. 2013;120(2):356–394. doi: 10.1037/a0032020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jung MW, McNaughton BL. Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granular layer. Hippocampus. 1993;3(2):165–182. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007;315(5814):961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Coltheart M. The MRC psycholinguistic database. Q J Exp Psychol A. 1981;33:497–505. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Steinmetz PN, Thorp C. Testing for effects of different stimuli on neuronal firing relative to background activity. J Neural Eng. 2013;10(5):056019. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/10/5/056019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.