Abstract
Dietary assessment has long been known to be challenged by measurement error. A substantial amount of literature on methods for determining the effects of error on causal inference has accumulated over the past decades. These methods have unrealized potential for improving the validity of data collected for research studies and national nutritional surveillance, primarily through the NHANES. Recently, the validity of dietary data has been called into question. Arguments against using dietary data to assess diet–health relations or to inform the nutrition policy debate are subject to flaws that fall into 2 broad areas: 1) ignorance or misunderstanding of methodologic issues; and 2) faulty logic in drawing inferences. Nine specific issues are identified in these arguments, indicating insufficient grasp of the methods used for assessing diet and designing nutritional epidemiologic studies. These include a narrow operationalization of validity, failure to properly account for sources of error, and large, unsubstantiated jumps to policy implications. Recent attacks on the inadequacy of 24-h recall–derived data from the NHANES are uninformative regarding effects on estimating risk of health outcomes and on inferences to inform the diet-related health policy debate. Despite errors, for many purposes and in many contexts, these dietary data have proven to be useful in addressing important research and policy questions. Similarly, structured instruments, such as the food frequency questionnaire, which is the mainstay of epidemiologic literature, can provide useful data when errors are measured and considered in analyses.
Introduction
Over the past 40 years, there has been a proliferation of research aimed at understanding the role of diet in health, with 93% of articles with a MeSH heading including the word “diet” or diet as a text word, published from 1973 to late May 2014. The bulk of this work in humans involved epidemiologic studies assessing the influence of diet and nutrition on disease risks. The vast majority of these studies are observational, with only limited experimental-trial representation. In addition, there is a smaller literature based on ongoing nutrition surveys designed to assess and monitor the content and quality of diet in populations.
Reflecting the growing interest in diet and health and building on national health surveys conducted primarily in the 1960s, the U.S. government began long-term monitoring of the food and nutrient intake and nutritional status of the U.S. population through the first NHANES performed from 1971 to 1973 (1). Several other waves of the NHANES were conducted, including one focused on Hispanics. In each of these, representative samples of the U.S. population were surveyed (2). In 1999, the NHANES operations were converted into a continuous, ongoing survey activity, now overseen by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (3). The data derived, including estimates of food and nutrient intake and measurements of nutritional biomarkers among numerous other health indicators, formed a basis for examining secular trends and to provide data to help inform federal food and nutrition policy.
Although NHANES and other national survey data help inform federal nutrition policy, it has long been recognized that self-reported dietary data are challenged by systematic and random measurement error. This is true not only for survey-derived measures but for all data derived from standard interview- or questionnaire-based dietary assessment methods (4). These assessment methods include the 24-h dietary recall interview (24HR)14, which forms the backbone of the NHANES dietary assessment but is used much less frequently in nutritional epidemiology because of its high costs. Reporting errors also exist in the food frequency questionnaire (FFQ), a structured questionnaire that is a mainstay of analytic epidemiologic studies (5, 6).
A recent article by Archer et al. (7) underscored this known limitation by stating that “across the 39-y history of the NHANES, [energy intake] data on the majority of respondents … was not physiologically plausible.” That article and 2 published since (8, 9) attempt to discredit a long history of nutritional research that has provided a robust literature consisting of >1 million articles published in the broad area of diet and health since 1946 and that formed the basis of numerous health recommendations and guided the nutrition-related public policy debate. Recommendations by scientific advisory bodies charged with addressing the role of diet in health parallel and reinforce some of the concerns raised about errors in dietary self-report (10). Therefore, it is important that the criticisms be clarified so that they may be addressed. The purpose of this article is to identify specific issues raised by these authors with respect to putative flaws in dietary assessment and their role in both epidemiologic studies and nutritional surveys.
Issues Drawn from Recent Literature
Nine issues were identified from the Advisory Committee on the Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2010, which recently published criticisms of nutritional survey data that relate to measuring diet, identifying and controlling for errors in self-reports, designing studies, conducting surveys, and drawing inferences to inform public policy (7–10). Comprehending both the nature and consequences of measurement error is necessary to allow for continuing improvement in dietary assessment methodologies and to make informed inferences from existing sources of information.
First, it is well known that surveys based on dietary self-report underestimate total energy intake (EI) compared with estimates of metabolic need (11–14). Recognition of a 20–25% underestimate in mean EI observed in large-scale population-based surveys, such as the NHANES, spawned extensive research that has deepened the understanding of self-report errors (5, 6, 15–38) and led to improved measurement methods (6, 37, 39–44) and analytic techniques (5, 21, 24, 26, 45–48) to mitigate their effects (Table 1). This culminated in practical applications for estimating the effect of diet on health outcomes of free-living individuals (49, 50). In contrast, definition of validity using “disparity values,” e.g., testing if measured EI falls within a 95% CI for predicted EI (7), is limited because it fails to provide a measure of the signal strength relative to the underlying error structure of the data.
TABLE 1.
Key advances | Suggestions for continued improvement |
1. Increased understanding of self-report errors with improved measurement methods and analytic techniques to mitigate their effects (5, 6, 15–40, 42–48, 127, 128). | Explore sources of reporting biases and develop means to measure and reduce the bias or mitigate its effect. Explore construct validators. |
2. Single days of the 24-h dietary recall interview have large intra-individual variation that can be reduced with multiple days of recall (51–53). | When practical, increase the number of 24-h dietary recall interviews in those circumstances in which that method is appropriate. |
3. Better understanding of intra-individual variability in energy intake led to improvements in accounting for this source of variability (55–60). | Conduct research in disparate populations to increase understanding of the sources of this variability. |
4. Improved understanding of the many factors that play a role in underestimation of energy intake, including portion-size estimation errors, omissions of foods consumed, inclusion of foods not consumed, and self-report biases (e.g., social desirability or overweight/obesity status) (30, 32–38, 51, 52, 63–65, 70). | Total energy intake is an important factor in energy balance and therefore may be an important determinant of health outcomes, so future work should focus on improving its estimation. This could include designing small, focused studies that use the method of triads (129, 130) using a variety of criterion validators, such as energy expenditure from doubly-labeled water. Design studies to test whether or not the new technology-based methods for dietary assessment, such as the National Cancer Institute Automated Self-Administered 24-H Recall, collect better data than the traditional non-technology–driven method. Also, use of multiple recalls/records as main instrument combined with FFQ data could provide better energy intake estimates. |
5. Rigorous studies of identified sources of bias showed them to be of sufficient magnitude to explain observed errors in measurement of energy intake (5, 32, 59, 127, 128). | Identify and quantify self-report biases in measuring diet, including total energy intake. |
6. Conversion of foods to nutrients introduces error that may bias temporal comparisons but is not a major source of systematic bias in either group comparisons or analytic epidemiologic studies of point estimates of dietary intake and health outcomes (84–86). | Improve nutrient databases and conduct research to deepen understanding regarding the conversion of foods to nutrients and other bioactive compounds. |
7. Research showing that errors in nutrient measurement do not necessarily lead to misclassification and bias; careful collection of additional data are needed to understand the extent to which bias may occur with systematic errors in measurement (37, 63, 70, 88). | Use existing databases and conduct future research to understand the effect of measurement error in misclassification on estimating health effects in epidemiologic studies. |
8. Nutrition-related policy is based on evidence from multiple sources; inferences drawn from nutrition surveys alone rarely drive policy (90, 92, 93, 131). | Monitor how policy-oriented decisions are made in relation to the scientific basis for decision-making. Seek and exploit opportunities for better-quality observational studies and trials to inform key diet and health questions. This would include establishing cohorts under favorable circumstances with some combination of greater exposure variability, reduced bias, and improved measurement methods. |
9. Evidence of diet effects on health come from a variety of research designs and sources, with studies using national nutrition surveys providing only a small fraction of the total evidence base (123, 132–133). | Educate the scientific community on the use of existing databases, including national and other surveys, to estimate the effect of diet on health. |
Second, the recent literature critiquing dietary data focuses heavily on the use of the 24HR in surveillance, especially the NHANES. This method was selected to characterize population or group intake and not for use as a measure of individual intake. When this survey-design decision was made, it was well known that self-reported EI based on a single 24HR is not necessarily indicative of usual or habitual intake, on either an individual or group basis. Any particular day represents the potential for extremes which, were they to represent a long-term average, might be biologically implausible. Multiple days of 24HR, including weekend days, are needed to account for day-to-day variation, and a minimum number of days is necessary to estimate EI with adequate precision (51, 52). Moreover, the number of days of data needed varies by weight category (53). The use of a single day of data to characterize an individual’s usual diet (see reference 7) is now considered insufficient to generate reliable estimates of intake. As a result, we have seen over time an evolution in the methodology in which multiple days, including weekends, are randomly sampled, and the multi-pass interview protocol is used, which demonstrated better agreement (54).
Third, examining extreme values in survey data requires an understanding of statistical and related issues. Large-scale survey data based on a single-day 24HR, such as those from the NHANES, reflect both interperson and intraperson sources of variability (55). Although this may have little or no effect on the estimated group mean value, failing to take into account the effect of adding this relatively large intraperson error component, which typically accounts for at least half of the total variability for a variable such as EI, overestimates the variance and inflates the proportion of the population in the extreme portions of the distribution (56–60). Indeed, when we reported NHANES data to compare homogeneity of within-U.S. population nutrient intake to international norms, we were careful to adjust for intra-individual variability (55, 61). Failing to do so would result in inflating the number of individuals who provide either lower-than-average or higher-than-average estimates of dietary intake, i.e., the very group on which some authors tend to base their conclusions. Furthermore, using a reported EI/basal metabolic rate (BMR) cut point of 1.35 (as in reference 7) would inflate the number of implausibly low values if adjustments are not made for intra-individual variability. Bingham (45) cautioned that values <1.20 (especially if predicted rather than measured) should be excluded from analyses with certainty as erroneous EI. Goldberg et al. (62), who developed EI/BMR cut points for assessing underreporting of EI, advised using a cut point of 1.35 only if BMR was actually measured; however, when an estimate of habitual dietary intake is attempted, for example, by use of a single 24HR, then a more liberal cut point of 0.92 is warranted.
Fourth, a core group of factors play a role in underestimation, including the following: 1) portion-size estimation errors; 2) omissions of foods consumed; and 3) self-report biases (e.g., social desirability or overweight/obesity status) (38, 63). The first 24HR in a sequence underestimates EI to a greater extent than subsequent 24HR. This is likely due in part to more food omissions (51, 52, 64) and portion estimation errors (64, 65) that diminish over time, possibly as the result of increasing familiarity with the method that comes from repeated interviews or changes in the demand characteristics that could modify response bias. These are systematic biases that lead to underestimation in intake estimates but are not necessarily differential in nature and may not interfere with the ability to differentiate or rank individuals or groups in a population. Although the older NHANES datasets are limited to a single day of 24HR, this limitation does not apply to other survey data or to most of the research studies that use 24HR (38, 51, 66, 67). Using NHANES methodology (i.e., the 5-step automated multiple-pass method and 2 24HRs) and modifying it to include a third 24HR has shown greater agreement with estimates of energy expenditure using doubly-labeled water and resulted in less underreporting (54). In addition, under-eating as a conscious effort to lose weight is highly prevalent among Americans, particularly those who are overweight and obese, and this is yet another reason why 1-d estimates of EI could be lower than expected (68, 69).
Fifth, literature accumulated over the past 20 y identifies specific sources of bias associated with response sets, such as social desirability and social approval (30, 32–37, 70). This research required the rigorous design and implementation of a variety of studies, including the use of criterion validators, to quantify potential biases that, in turn, required understanding cognitive issues in formulating self-reports. Using these data in predictive models, biologic constructs, such as serum lipid concentrations, can be predicted using self-report data (49, 71) with accuracy and precision similar to results produced using data collected in metabolic wards (72–74). Use of model systems that rely on biologic constructs, such as serum lipids, that respond predictably to produce average changes in populations provides important validation when criterion measures are unavailable, as usually is the case in free-living populations (75). Likewise, changes in body mass can be predicted with similar adjustments for error (50). With regard to error specification, errors due to social desirability are in the range of what was reported for the NHANES data (7) [e.g., 375 kcal/d across the full range of measured social desirability scores for 24HR compared with total energy expenditure from doubly-labeled water for women in the Energy Study (5)]. A difference of this magnitude from a single potential bias could explain a substantial portion of the crude differences noted. It also should be noted that these errors are not limited to the self-report of diet. For example, we have observed biases in reports of physical activity (76) that are similar to those observed in dietary self-reports (5, 30, 32, 33, 36, 37, 77) and are consistent with observations made by others (78, 79). These developments aimed at improving assessment methods are consistent with the recommendation of Webb et al. (4) in their 2013 article “Strategies to Optimize the Impact of Nutritional Surveys and Epidemiological Studies.”
Sixth, conversion of foods to nutrients is not a major source of systematic bias in group comparisons. These errors are not differential, and they are specific to the underlying food composition database. There is no evidence in the references provided by Archer et al. (7) that would indicate that errors introduced in this stage of preparing data for analyses would either exacerbate biases in self-report or influence the ability to estimate health outcomes. Indeed, numerous enhancements in food/nutrient databases occurred over the past couple of decades that improve nutrient intake estimates based on reported food intake data (80–83). The dynamic nature of the food supply and the rapid discovery of new bioactive substances related to health outcomes are other factors that influence changes in food composition databases. Understanding the evolution of these developments in the conversion of foods to nutrient intake is important, because changes may bias comparisons of intake over time if they are not taken into account (84–86). However, such changes should not bias single time-point estimation of diet in relation to health outcomes, which is nearly always the estimation of effect that is performed in nutritional epidemiology.
Seventh, to appreciate the consequence of measurement error, it is essential to understand the exact nature, and not just the crude overall magnitude, of the errors. Ultimately, the aim is to account for or control for identified errors to use data collected under “real-world” conditions to adjust estimates of health effects. Estimating risk in epidemiologic studies almost always requires comparison across categories of exposure (e.g., to obtain RR estimates). Therefore, it is essential to know how errors affect classification into these categories to know whether there is any distortion in risk estimation. Random error may attenuate observable risk, but it should not result in spurious risk estimates (87). Recent publications citing this as an issue provide no evidence regarding how errors in these self-report measures are distributed or how they relate to potential confounders and effect modifiers (7, 8).
Epidemiologic studies typically control for potential confounders and consider effect modifiers in the analysis. As we showed previously with social desirability, some of these errors are associated with psychological predispositions (e.g., acquiescent personality type), sex, and education, factors that are known to be related to many health outcomes. As we demonstrated nearly 2 decades ago in our original article on the subject of response set biases (37) and in correspondence published in its aftermath (77, 88), the modeling of effect modification and confounding is a complicated business about which relevant data must be collected to estimate their effects on predicting health outcomes. Without such information, there is no way of knowing whether misclassification occurs because of these errors or how they are related, either organically or statistically, to known or suspected effect modifiers or confounders. Therefore, the results presented in the recent literature (7, 8) are uninformative regarding their effect on risk estimation and, by logical extension, on inferences that might lead to informing public policy.
Eighth, there is a large, unsubstantiated jump from detecting potential problems with measurement error to policy implications. Virtually never do dietary data alone—or data on any exposure, for that matter—result directly in policy recommendations. It is impossible to make meaningful inferential assertions about the effect of errors without knowing whether they influence the prediction of health outcomes. Making policy recommendations requires access to results based on relevant health outcomes. For example, large-scale global investment to prevent malnutrition in young children did not begin garnering its current high global priority until after demonstration that approximately half of young-child deaths are caused by the synergistic effect of malnutrition with infection (89). Attempts to limit tobacco exposure did not occur without first understanding the effects of tobacco on health outcomes (90, 91). Furthermore, when recommendations were promulgated and laws implemented, they were not based on market or use surveys but rather on estimates of health effects derived primarily from epidemiologic studies (90, 92, 93).
On their own, the data from the NHANES will rarely provide sufficient evidence to inform inferences regarding diet–disease relations. Recognizing impediments imposed by studies conducted within populations having limited variability in dietary exposures, several of us wrote on problems with nutritional homogeneity (55, 94, 95) and proposed a variety of solutions, including international studies (61) and intra-country studies with large contrasts (59, 95). For example, the Multiethnic Cohort Study (96, 97) and the EPIC (European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition) study (98, 99) were designed with this purpose in mind. These “natural” contrasts may be much more desirable from a methodologic perspective than trying to create them within the context of randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Once the diet or nutrient–disease relation has been firmly established from relevant research studies, then data from the NHANES can help to estimate the potential attributable risk in the population and recommend potential avenues most amenable to intervention.
The complaint made about measurement validity undermining the support of health effects due to diet (7) are reminiscent of protests by the tobacco industry and its allies that occurred over the many decades during which they challenged the nature and quality of the epidemiologic evidence linking tobacco to health (100, 101). This industry challenged the validity of epidemiologic evidence and made demands, unreasonable on both ethical and pragmatic grounds, to accept evidence only from RCTs. Based on Bradford Hill’s Criteria for Judging Causality (102, 103), which remain hallmarks for assessing whether or not putative risk factors constitute “cause,” the expert panel convened by the Surgeon General of the United States in 1964 concluded that RCTs were not needed to assert that tobacco “caused” a variety of health outcomes, including lung cancer (90).
Similar arguments have been made for nutritional research, citing errors in diet assessment as a reason for dismissing observational studies of diet and health and calling for RCTs as the only answer (8). This argument would leave us with little additional evidence on diet and health for many years and with uncertain promise of evidence on relevant questions on diet and health in the future. Advocates of this argument often cite examples of successful trials of diet, such as the recently published trial showing benefits of the Mediterranean diet in heart disease prevention (104). However, this is the exception, and there are other examples of expensive and lengthy trials that failed to provide definitive answers to the questions that provided their rationale. For many dietary issues, trials are neither feasible nor ethical and may be limited in the generalizability of their findings (105–107).
Trials are not immune to the challenges of diet measurement. They are susceptible to errors in measurement of diet in relation to implementing the intervention and monitoring compliance (108). For example, we showed that individuals in the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI) who were eligible for the diet modification arm overestimated their self-report dietary intake by ~ 169 kcal/d relative to women who were ineligible (32). Even for relatively simple interventions, it will be necessary to measure diet, and this example underlines that biased estimates of intake need to be understood and, as has been done for observational studies, estimated and controlled. Despite the enormous expense and time it required, the diet modification arm of the WHI provided only ambiguous, uncertain results for the benefits of diet, and the primary question tested (total dietary fat reduction) was considered outdated (supplanted by alterations in type of fat) by the time the results went to press (109, 110). This problem is certainly not unique to the WHI and will likely apply to other large-scale, long-term trials of dietary effects on chronic disease risks.
The reality is that conventional agent-oriented RCTs may focus only on 1 or 2 exposures at a time, potentially limiting the relevance of their findings for the effects of diet in population health. For example, assuming that distilling complex dietary patterns into a single agent (e.g., a vitamin supplement) that is characteristic of dietary pattern contrasts can be misleading. Single-agent dietary trials also may turn out to be of limited value because they inadvertently studied the wrong population or the wrong type of exposure at the wrong time in the disease process. For example, the ATBC (Alpha-Tocopherol, Beta-Carotene Cancer Prevention) study and the CARET (β-Carotene and Retinol Efficacy Trial) (111, 112) unexpectedly found evidence for a detrimental effect of β-carotene supplements on risk of lung cancer in older smoking men, thus contributing ambiguous and inconsistent evidence on the role of these agents in reducing cancer risks for the larger population. The results contradicted a belief based on hundreds of studies showing salubrious effects of whole food diets rich in antioxidant and anti-inflammatory micronutrients on cancers of various sites (113–116). The reasons for these paradoxical results are only partly understood but include design decisions made for efficiency and cost, such as studying only high-risk populations (e.g., older smokers) and exposures relatively late in terms of cancer latency. These problems were not foreseen at the time these trials were initiated.
RCTs that study diet, and therefore choices of free-living study participants, face a host of problems in attempting to create large contrasts in free-living populations, as seen in the WHI and other trials (117, 118). Changing behaviors is challenging, and these trials may require extreme commitment to make and sustain large changes. Furthermore, it is unlikely that either someone who is willing to accept randomization would have the motivation to persevere if randomly assigned to an intensive intervention or would not seek out other means for achieving change if randomly assigned to a “no-treatment” control. For example, the PPT (Polyp Prevention Trial) found no effect of a low-fat, high-fiber, high-fruit and vegetable intervention on adenoma recurrence (117, 118). However, analyses of carotenoid biomarker data and FFQ data in the PPT revealed that participants consuming diets rich in dry beans, vegetables, and fruits (as sources of carotenoids and flavonoids) were at reduced risk of adenoma or advanced adenoma recurrence, regardless of intervention arm assignment. Perhaps this result is partially explained by the larger contrast in exposure able to be obtained in the observational compared with the experimental (intervention vs. control) analyses (119–121).
For the foreseeable future, trials may answer only a few limited questions, and observational studies will remain the primary means for evaluating relations between diet and health outcomes. Such studies constitute a major portion of the evidence that underlies food and nutrition guidelines such as those of the Dietary Guidelines for Americans (10) or the diet and physical activity guidelines for cancer prevention of the World Cancer Research Fund and the American Institute for Cancer Research (122). Future observational studies should attempt to improve on methods in design, analysis, and presentation (123, 124), with better consideration of errors in measurement and potential biases, and by establishing cohorts under favorable circumstances with greater exposure variability, reduced bias, and/or better diet information.
Ninth, although there are some studies linking diet to health outcomes using data from national and other surveys such as the NHANES, these studies represent only a tiny fraction of the literature linking diet to health. It is also well recognized that diet and health studies using NHANES data are among the least informative of such analytic epidemiologic studies, based in part on the use of a single 24HR to represent individuals' food or nutrient intake, for the reasons outlined above. Despite this recognized limitation, the results from these NHANES studies are broadly consistent with those obtained from many other studies using more robust measures of individual dietary exposures.
Conclusions
Nothing is measured without error. Virtually everything we measure represents a combination of truth and error—usually both from random sources, such as from use of a single day to represent “usual” intake, and systematic biases, such as may result from social desirability. What is important, and what validity implies, is whether a method is suitable for providing useful analytical measurement for a given purpose and context (125, 126). For many purposes and in many contexts, 24HR data from surveys such as the NHANES proved to be useful in helping to address important research and policy questions, despite their known errors. Likewise, despite their well-acknowledged flaws, FFQ data produced results across a wide variety of studies and in many different populations and cultural contexts that are broadly consistent with one another and form the mainstay of what we know about diet and health.
Acknowledgments
All authors actively participated in writing and editing the manuscript and approved the final version.
Footnotes
Abbreviations used: BMR, basal metabolic rate; EI, energy intake; FFQ, food frequency questionnaire; RCT, randomized controlled trial; WHI, Women’s Health Initiative; 24HR, 24-hour dietary recall interview.
References
- 1. U.S. Department of Agriculture. What we eat in America, NHANES 2007–2008 data: dietary interview—total nutrients intakes—first day (DR1TOT_C). Hyattsville (MD): U.S. Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service, Beltsville Human Nutrition Research Center, Food Surveys Research Group (Beltsville, MD) and U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 2010 [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ogden CL, Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Johnson CL. Prevalence and trends in overweight among US children and adolescents, 1999–2000. JAMA 2002;288:1728–32 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey data. Hyattsville (MD): National Center for Health Statistics; 2013 [cited 2013 July 13]. Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/about/major/nhanes/nh3data.htm
- 4.Webb D, Leahy MM, Milner JA, Allison DB, Dodd KW, Gaine PC, Matthews RA, Schneeman BO, Tucker KL, Young SS. Strategies to optimize the impact of nutritional surveys and epidemiological studies. Adv Nutr 2013;4:545–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hebert JR, Ebbeling CB, Matthews CE, Ma Y, Clemow L, Hurley TG, Druker S. Systematic errors in middle-aged women’s estimates of energy intake: comparing three self-report measures to total energy expenditure from doubly labeled water. Ann Epidemiol 2002;12:577–86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Neuhouser ML, Tinker L, Shaw PA, Schoeller D, Bingham SA, Horn LV, Beresford SAA, Caan B, Thomson C, Satterfield S, et al. Use of recovery biomarkers to calibrate nutrient consumption self-reports in the Women’s Health Initiative. Am J Epidemiol 2008;167:1247–59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Archer E, Hand GA, Blair SN. Validity of U.S. Nutritional surveillance: National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey caloric energy intake data, 1971–2010. PLoS One 2013;8:e76632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ioannidis JP. Implausible results in human nutrition research. BMJ 2013;347:f6698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mitka M. Do flawed data on caloric intake from NHANES present problems for researchers and policy makers? JAMA 2013;310:2137–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee. Report of the Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee on the Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2010. To the Secretary of Agriculture and the Secretary of Health and Human Services. Beltsville (MD): U.S. Department of Agriculture and Health and Human Services; 2010 [Google Scholar]
- 11.Block G, Rosenberger WF, Patterson BH. Calories, fat and cholesterol: intake patterns in the US population by race, sex and age. Am J Public Health 1988;78:1150–5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Beaton GH, Burema J, Rittenbaugh C. Errors in the interpretation of dietary assessments. Am J Clin Nutr 1997;65:1100S–7S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hill RJ, Davies PS. The validity of self-reported energy intake as determined using the doubly labelled water technique. Br J Nutr 2001;85:415–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Trabulsi J, Schoeller DA. Evaluation of dietary assessment instruments against doubly labeled water, a biomarker of habitual energy intake. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 2001;281:E891–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Toobert DJ, Strycker LA, Hampson SE, Westling E, Christiansen SM, Hurley TG, Hebert JR. Computerized portion-size estimation compared to multiple 24-hour dietary recalls for measurement of fat, fruit, and vegetable intake in overweight adults. J Am Diet Assoc 2011;111:1578–83 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Higgins JA, LaSalle AL, Zhaoxing P, Kasten MY, Bing KN, Ridzon SE, Witten TL. Validation of photographic food records in children: are pictures really worth a thousand words? Eur J Clin Nutr 2009;63:1025–33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rumpler WV, Kramer M, Rhodes DG, Moshfegh AJ, Paul DR. Identifying sources of reporting error using measured food intake. Eur J Clin Nutr 2008;62:544–52 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baxter SD, Smith AF, Hardin JW, Nichols MD. Conclusions about children’s reporting accuracy for energy and macronutrients over multiple interviews depend on the analytic approach for comparing reported information to reference information. J Am Diet Assoc 2007;107:595–604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Carroll RJ, Midthune D, Freedman LS, Kipnis V. Seemingly unrelated measurement error models, with application to nutritional epidemiology. Biometrics 2006;62:75–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Baxter SD, Smith AF, Litaker MS, Baglio ML, Guinn CH, Shaffer NM. Children’s social desirability and dietary reports. J Nutr Educ Behav 2004;36:84–9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kipnis V, Subar AF, Midthune D, Freedman LS, Ballard-Barbash R, Troiano RP, Bingham S, Schoeller DA, Schatzkin A, Carroll RJ. Structure of dietary measurement error: results of the OPEN biomarker study. Am J Epidemiol 2003;158:14–21; discussion 2–6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fraser GE. A search for truth in dietary epidemiology. Am J Clin Nutr 2003;78(Suppl 3):521S–5S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kipnis V, Midthune D, Freedman LS, Bingham S, Schatzkin A, Subar A, Carroll RJ. Empirical evidence of correlated biases in dietary assessment instruments and its implications. Am J Epidemiol 2001;153:394–403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kipnis V, Carroll RJ, Freedman LS, Li L. Implications of a new dietary measurement error model for estimation of relative risk: application to four calibration studies. Am J Epidemiol 1999;150:642–51 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Prentice RL. Measurement error and results from analytic epidemiology: dietary fat and breast cancer. J Natl Cancer Inst 1996;88:1738–47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Plummer M, Clayton D. Measurement error in dietary assessment: an investigation using covariance structure models. Part II. Stat Med 1993;12:937–48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kohlmeier L. Overview of validity, quality control and measurement error issues in nutritional epidemiology. Eur J Clin Nutr 1993;47:S1–5 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Espeland MA, Kumanyika S, Wilson AC, Wilcox S, Chao D, Bahnson J, Reboussin DM, Easter L, Zheng B. Lifestyle interventions influence relative errors in self-reported diet intake of sodium and potassium. Ann Epidemiol 2001;11:85–93 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Guinn CH, Baxter SD, Royer JA, Hardin JW, Mackelprang AJ, Smith AF. Fourth-grade children’s dietary recall accuracy for energy intake at school meals differs by social desirability and body mass index percentile in a study concerning retention interval. J Health Psychol 2010;15:505–14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hebert JR, Hurley TG, Peterson KE, Resnicow K, Thompson FE, Yaroch AL, Ehlers M, Midthune D, Williams GC, Greene GW, et al. Social desirability trait influences on self-reported dietary measures among diverse participants in a multicenter multiple risk factor trial. J Nutr 2008;138:226S–34S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tooze JA, Subar AF, Thompson FE, Troiano R, Schatzkin A, Kipnis V. Psychosocial predictors of energy underreporting in a large doubly labeled water study. Am J Clin Nutr 2004;79:795–804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hebert JR, Patterson RE, Gorfine M, Ebbeling CB, St. Jeor ST, Chlebowski RT. Differences between estimated caloric requirements and self-reported caloric intake in the Women’s Health Initiative. Ann Epidemiol 2003;13:629–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hébert JR, Peterson KE, Hurley TG, Stoddard AM, Cohen N, Field AE, Sorensen G. The effect of social desirability trait on self-reported dietary measures among multi-ethnic female health center employees. Ann Epidemiol 2001;11:417–27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Taren DL, Tobar M, Hill A, Howell W, Shisslak C, Bell I, Ritenbaugh C. The association of energy intake bias with psychological scores of women. Eur J Clin Nutr 1999;53:570–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Smith AF, Thompson FE, Subar AF, Brown CC, Jobe JB, Sharbaugh CO, Mittl E. Social desirability, social approval, and reports of food frequency. Eur J Clin Nutr 1998;52: Supp 2:S35 [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hebert JR, Ma Y, Clemow L, Ockene IS, Saperia G, Stanek EJ, Merriam PA, Ockene JK. Gender differences in social desirability and social approval bias in dietary self report. Am J Epidemiol 1997;146:1046–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hebert JR, Clemow L, Pbert L, Ockene IS, Ockene JK. Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. Int J Epidemiol 1995;24:389–98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Novotny JA, Rumpler WV, Riddick H, Hebert JR, Rhodes D, Judd JT, Baer D, McDowell M, Ervin B, Briefel R. Personal characteristics as predictors of under-reporting of energy intake on 24h dietary recall interviews. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:1146–51 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Novotny JA, Rumpler WV, Judd JT, Riddick PH, Rhodes D, McDowell M, Briefel R. Diet interviews of subject pairs: how different persons recall eating the same foods. J Am Diet Assoc 2001;101:1189–93 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Haraldsdóttir J. Minimizing error in the field: quality control in dietary surveys. Eur J Clin Nutr 1993;47:S19–24 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hebert JR, Hurley TG, Chiraboga DE, Barone J. A comparison of selected nutrient intakes derived from three diet assessment methods used in a low-fat maintenance trial. Public Health Nutr 1998;1:207–14 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hebert JR, Ockene IS, Merriam P, Botelho L, Ellis S. editors. Development and testing of a seven day dietary recall. Presented at the 2nd International Conference on Dietary Assessment Methods, 1995 Jan 22–24; Boston.
- 43.Paul DR, Kramer M, Stote KS, Spears KE, Moshfegh AJ, Baer DJ, Rumpler WV. Estimates of adherence and error analysis of physical activity data collected via accelerometry in a large study of free-living adults. BMC Med Res Methodol 2008;8:38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wengreen HJ, Munger RG, Wong SS, West NA, Cutler R. Comparison of a picture-sort food-frequency questionnaire with 24-hour dietary recalls in an elderly Utah population. Public Health Nutr 2001;4:961–70 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bingham SA. The use of 24hr urine samples and energy expenditure to validate dietary assessments. Am J Clin Nutr 1994;59:227S–31S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Clayton D, Gill C. Covariate measurement errors in nutritional epidemiology: effects and remedies. In: Margetts BM, Nelson M, editors. Design concepts in nutritional epidemiology. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press;. 1997. p. 87–104. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Carroll RJ, Freedman L, Pee D. Design aspects of calibration studies in nutrition, with analysis of missing data in linear measurement error models. Biometrics 1997;53:1440–57 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Carroll RJ, Freedman LS, Kipnis V, Li L. A new class of measurement-error models, with applications to dietary data. Can J Stat 1998;26:467–77 [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hebert JR, Ockene IS, Hurley TG, Luippold R, Well AD, Harmatz MG. Development and testing of a seven-day dietary recall. J Clin Epidemiol 1997;50:925–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hebert JR, Ebbeling CB, Hurley TG, Ma Y, Clemow L, Olendzki BC, Saal N, Ockene JK. Change in women's diet and body mass following intensive intervention in early-stage breast cancer. J Am Diet Assoc 2001;101:421–31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ma Y, Olendzki BC, Pagoto SL, Hurley TG, Magner RP, Ockene IS, Schneider KL, Merriam PA, Hebert JR. Number of 24-hour diet recalls needed to estimate energy intake. Ann Epidemiol 2009;19:553–9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hébert JR, Hurley TG, Cavicchia P, Ma Y, Magner RP, Olendzki BC, Merriam PA, Ockene IS, Nebeling L. Response to Dr. Arab et al on “Number of 24-hour diet recalls needed to estimate energy intake”. Ann Epidemiol 2010;20:87–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Stote KS, Radecki SV, Moshfegh AJ, Ingwersen LA, Baer DJ. The number of 24 h dietary recalls using the US Department of Agriculture’s automated multiple-pass method required to estimate nutrient intake in overweight and obese adults. Public Health Nutr 2011;14:1736–42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Moshfegh AJ, Rhodes DG, Baer DJ, Murayi T, Clemens JC, Rumpler WV, Paul DR, Sebastian RS, Kuczynski KJ, Ingwersen LA, et al. The US Department of Agriculture Automated Multiple-Pass Method reduces bias in the collection of energy intakes. Am J Clin Nutr 2008;88:324–32 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Wynder EL, Hebert JR. Homogeneity in nutritional exposure: an impediment in cancer epidemiology. J Natl Cancer Inst 1987;79:605–7 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Beaton GH, Milner J, Corey P, McGuire V, Cousins M, Stewart E, de Ramos M, Hewitt D, Grambsch V, Kassim N, et al. Source of variance in 24-hour dietary recall data: implications for nutrition study design and interpretation. Am J Clin Nutr 1979;32:2546–59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Beaton GH, Milner J, McGuire V, Feather TE, Little JA. Source of variance in 24-hour dietary recall data: implications for nutrition study design and interpretation. carbohydrate sources, vitamins, and minerals. Am J Clin Nutr 1983;37:986–95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hebert JR, Backlund JYC, Engle A, Barone J, Biener K. Intra- and inter-person sources of variability in fat intake in a feeding trial of 14 men. Eur J Epidemiol 1990;6:55–60 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hebert JR, Gupta PC, Mehta H, Ebbeling CB, Bhonsle RR, Varghese F. Sources of variability in dietary intake in two distinct regions of rural India: implications for nutrition study design and interpretation. Eur J Clin Nutr 2000;54:479–86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Tarasuk V, Beaton GH. The nature and individuality of within-subject variation in energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 1991;54:464–70 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Hebert JR. Epidemiologic studies of diet and cancer: the case for international collaboration. Austro-Asian J Cancer. 2005;4:125–34 [Google Scholar]
- 62.Goldberg GR, Black AE, Jebb SA, Cole TJ, Murgatroyd PR, Coward WA, Prentice AM. Critical evaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 1. derivation of cut-off limits to identify under-recording. Eur J Clin Nutr 1991;45:569–81 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Klesges RC, Eck LH, Ray JW. Who underreports dietary intake in a dietary recall? Evidence from the Second National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES II). J Consult Clin Psychol 1995;63:438–44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Gemming L, Doherty A, Kelly P, Utter J, Ni Mhurchu C. Feasibility of a SenseCam-assisted 24-h recall to reduce under-reporting of energy intake. Eur J Clin Nutr 2013;67:1095–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Krebs-Smith SM, Graubard BI, Kahle LL, Subar AF, Cleveland LE, Ballard-Barbash R. Low energy reporters vs others: a comparison of reported food intakes. Eur J Clin Nutr 2000;54:281–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Shivappa N, Steck SE, Hurley TG, Hussey JR, Ma Y, Ockene IS, Tabung F, Hebert JR. A population-based dietary inflammatory index predicts levels of C-reactive protein (CRP) in the Seasonal Variation of Blood Cholesterol Study (SEASONS). Public Health Nutr. 2013. Oct 10 (Epub ahead of print; DOI:10.1017/S1368980013002565) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Merriam PA, Ockene IS, Hebert JR, Rosal MC, Matthews CE. Seasonal variation of blood cholesterol levels: study methodology. J Biol Rhythms 1999;14:330–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Péneau S, Menard E, Mejean C, Bellisle F, Hercberg S. Sex and dieting modify the association between emotional eating and weight status. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;97:1307–13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Andreyeva T, Long MW, Henderson KE, Grode GM. Trying to lose weight: diet strategies among Americans with overweight or obesity in 1996 and 2003. J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110:535–42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Klesges LM, Baranowski T, Beech B, Cullen K, Murray DM, Rochon J, Pratt C. Social desirability bias in self-reported dietary, physical activity and weight concerns measures in 8- to 10-year-old African-American girls: results from the Girls Health Enrichment Multisite Studies (GEMS). Prev Med 2004;38(Suppl):S78–87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hebert JR, Ebbeling CB, Ockene IS, Ma Y, Rider L, Merriam PA, Ockene JK, Saperia GM. A dietitian-delivered group nutrition program leads to reductions in dietary fat, serum cholesterol, and body weight: findings from the Worcester Area Trial for Counseling in Hyperlipidemia (WATCH). J Am Diet Assoc 1999;99:544–52 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Keys A, Anderson JT, Grande F. Serum cholesterol response to changes in the diet-IV. Particular fatty acids in the diet. Metabolism 1965;14:776–87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Keys A, Anderson JT, Grande F. Serum cholesterol response to changes in the diet-III. Differences among individuals. Metabolism 1965;14:766–75 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hegsted DM, McGandy RB, Myers ML, Stare FJ. Quantitative effects of dietary fat on serum cholesterol in man. Am J Clin Nutr 1965;17:281–95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hebert JR, Ma Y, Ebbeling CB, Matthews CE, Ockene IS. Self-report data. In: Ockene IS, Burke LE, editors. Compliance in healthcare and research. Armonk (NY): Futura; 2001. p. 163–79 [Google Scholar]
- 76.Adams SA, Matthews CE, Ebbeling CB, Moore CG, Cunningham JE, Fulton J, Hebert JR. The effect of social desirability and social approval on self-reports of physical activity. Am J Epidemiol 2005;161:389–98 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Hebert JR. Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. Implications for diet disease relationships [letter; author’s response] Int J Epidemiol 1996;25:223–5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Tooze JA, Troiano RP, Carroll RJ, Moshfegh AJ, Freedman LS. A measurement error model for physical activity level as measured by a questionnaire with application to the 1999–2006 NHANES questionnaire. Am J Epidemiol 2013;177:1199–208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Paul DR, Kramer M, Moshfegh AJ, Baer DJ, Rumpler WV. Comparison of two different physical activity monitors. BMC Med Res Methodol 2007;7:26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Beecher GR, Stewart KK, Holden JM, Harnly JM, Wolf WR. Legacy of Wilbur O. Atwater: human nutrition research expansion at the USDA–interagency development of food composition research. J Nutr 2009;139:178–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Haytowitz DB, Pehrsson PR, Holden JM. The National Food and Nutrient Analysis Program: a decade of progress. J Food Compost Anal 2008;21: S1:S94–102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Phillips KM, Patterson KY, Rasor AS, Exler J, Haytowitz DB, Holden JM, Pehrsson PR. Quality-control materials in the USDA National Food and Nutrient Analysis Program (NFNAP). Anal Bioanal Chem 2006;384:1341–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Phillips KM, Wolf WR, Patterson KY, Sharpless KE, Holden JM. Reference materials to evaluate measurement systems for the nutrient composition of foods: results from USDA’s National Food and Nutrient Analysis Program (NFNAP). Anal Bioanal Chem 2007;389:219–29 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Merchant AT, Dehghan M. Food composition database development for between country comparisons. Nutr J 2006;5:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Davis DR, Epp MD, Riordan HD. Changes in USDA food composition data for 43 garden crops, 1950 to 1999. J Am Coll Nutr 2004;23:669–82 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Dwyer J, Picciano MF, Raiten DJ, National H, Nutrition Examination S. Food and dietary supplement databases for What We Eat in America—NHANES. J Nutr 2003;133:624S–34S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Freedman LS, Schatzkin A, Midthune D, Kipnis V. Dealing with dietary measurement error in nutritional cohort studies. J Natl Cancer Inst 2011;103:1086–92 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Heitmann BL. Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. Implications for diet disease relationships [letter] Int J Epidemiol 1996;25:223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Pelletier DL, Frongillo EA, Jr, Habicht JP. Epidemiologic evidence for a potentiating effect of malnutrition on child mortality. Am J Public Health 1993;83:1130–3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.U.S. Department of Health Education and Welfare. Smoking and health: report of the Advisory Committee to the Surgeon General of the Public Health Services. Washington: Public Health Services; 1964; Public Health Services Publication No. 1103 [Google Scholar]
- 91.Wynder EL, Graham EA. Tobacco smoking as a possible etiologic factor in bronchiogenic carcinoma: a study of six hundred and eighty-four proved cases. JAMA 1950;143:329–36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Mathers CD, Loncar D. Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Med. 2006;3:e442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Lopez AD, Mathers CD. Measuring the global burden of disease and epidemiological transitions: 2002–2030. Ann Trop Med Parasitol 2006;100:481–99 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hebert JR, Miller DR. Methodologic considerations for investigating the diet-cancer link. Am J Clin Nutr 1988;47:1068–77 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.White E, Kushi LH, Pepe MS. The effect of exposure variance and exposure measurement error on study sample size: implications for the design of epidemiologic studies. J Clin Epidemiol 1994;47:873–80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.He J, Stram DO, Kolonel LN, Henderson BE, Le Marchand L, Haiman CA. The association of diabetes with colorectal cancer risk: the Multiethnic Cohort. Br J Cancer 2010;103:120–6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Kolonel LN, Henderson BE, Hankin JH, Nomura AM, Wilkens LR, Pike MC, Stram DO, Monroe KR, Earle ME, Nagamine FS. A multiethnic cohort in Hawaii and Los Angeles: baseline characteristics. Am J Epidemiol 2000;151:346–57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Schulz M, Liese AD, Boeing H, Cunningham JE, Moore CG, Kroke A. Associations of short-term weight changes and weight cycling with incidence of essential hypertension in the EPIC-Potsdam Study. J Hum Hypertens 2005;19:61–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Dossus L, Lukanova A, Rinaldi S, Allen N, Cust AE, Becker S, Tjonneland A, Hansen L, Overvad K, Chabbert-Buffet N, et al. Hormonal, metabolic, and inflammatory profiles and endometrial cancer risk within the EPIC cohort–a factor analysis. Am J Epidemiol 2013;177:787–99 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Palladino P. Discourses of smoking, health, and the just society: yesterday, today, and the return of the same? Soc Hist Med 2001;14:313–35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Smith P, Bansal-Travers M, O’Connor R, Brown A, Banthin C, Guardino-Colket S, Cummings KM. Correcting over 50 years of tobacco industry misinformation. Am J Prev Med 2011;40:690–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Hill AB. Observation and experiment. N Engl J Med 1953;248:995–1001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Hill AB. The environment and disease: association or causation? Proc R Soc Med 1965;58:295–300 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Estruch R, Ros E, Salas-Salvado J, Covas MI, Corella D, Aros F, Gomez-Gracia E, Ruiz-Gutierrez V, Fiol M, Lapetra J, et al. Primary prevention of cardiovascular disease with a Mediterranean diet. N Engl J Med 2013;368:1279–90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Gibson TM, Ferrucci LM, Tangrea JA, Schatzkin A. Epidemiological and clinical studies of nutrition. Semin Oncol 2010;37:282–96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Blumberg J, Heaney RP, Huncharek M, Scholl T, Stampfer M, Vieth R, Weaver CM, Zeisel SH. Evidence-based criteria in the nutritional context. Nutr Rev 2010;68:478–84 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Byers T. What can randomized controlled trials tell us about nutrition and cancer prevention? CA Cancer J Clin 1999;49:353–61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Prentice RL, Anderson GL. The Women’s Health Initiative: lessons learned. Annu Rev Public Health 2008;29:131–50 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Yngve A, Hambraeus L, Lissner L, Serra Majem L, Vaz de Almeida MD, Berg C, Hughes R, Cannon G, Thorsdottir I, Kearney J, et al. The Women’s Health Initiative. What is on trial: nutrition and chronic disease? Or misinterpreted science, media havoc and the sound of silence from peers? Public Health Nutr 2006;9:269–72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Howard BV, Van Horn L, Hsia J, Manson JE, Stefanick ML, Wassertheil-Smoller S, Kuller LH, LaCroix AZ, Langer RD, Lasser NL, et al. Low-fat dietary pattern and risk of cardiovascular disease: the Women’s Health Initiative Randomized Controlled Dietary Modification Trial. JAMA 2006;295:655–66 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Omenn GS, Goodman G, Thornquist M, Grizzle J, Rosenstock L, Barnhart S, Balmes J, Cherniack MG, Cullen MR, Glass A, et al. The beta-carotene and retinol efficacy trial (CARET) for chemoprevention of lung cancer in high risk populations: smokers and asbestos-exposed workers. Cancer Res 1994;54:2038s–43s [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.The Alpha Tocopherol Beta Carotene Cancer Prevention Group. The effect of vitamin E and beta carotene on the incidence of lung cancer and other cancers in male smokers. N Engl J Med 1994;330:1029–35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Aune D, Chan DS, Vieira AR, Navarro Rosenblatt DA, Vieira R, Greenwood DC, Norat T. Dietary compared with blood concentrations of carotenoids and breast cancer risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Clin Nutr 2012;96:356–73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Birt DF. Update on the effects of vitamins A, C, and E and selenium on carcinogenesis. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 1986;183:311–20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Rock CL, Flatt SW, Natarajan L, Thomson CA, Bardwell WA, Newman VA, Hollenbach KA, Jones L, Caan BJ, Pierce JP. Plasma carotenoids and recurrence-free survival in women with a history of breast cancer. J Clin Oncol 2005;23:6631–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Ziegler RG. Epidemiologic studies of vitamins and cancer of the lung, esophagus, and cervix. Adv Exp Med Biol 1986;206:11–26 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Lanza E, Yu B, Murphy G, Albert PS, Caan B, Marshall JR, Lance P, Paskett ED, Weissfeld J, Slattery M, et al. The polyp prevention trial continued follow-up study: no effect of a low-fat, high-fiber, high-fruit, and -vegetable diet on adenoma recurrence eight years after randomization. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2007;16:1745–52 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Schatzkin A, Lanza E, Corle D, Lance P, Iber F, Caan B, Shike M, Weissfeld J, Burt R, Cooper MR, et al. Lack of effect of a low-fat, high-fiber diet on the recurrence of colorectal adenomas. Polyp Prevention Trial Study Group [see comment] N Engl J Med 2000;342:1149–55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Steck-Scott S, Forman MR, Sowell A, Borkowf CB, Albert PS, Slattery M, Brewer B, Caan B, Paskett E, Iber F, et al. Carotenoids, vitamin A and risk of adenomatous polyp recurrence in the polyp prevention trial. Int J Cancer 2004;112:295–305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Bobe G, Sansbury LB, Albert PS, Cross AJ, Kahle L, Ashby J, Slattery ML, Caan B, Paskett E, Iber F, et al. Dietary flavonoids and colorectal adenoma recurrence in the Polyp Prevention Trial. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2008;17:1344–53 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Lanza E, Hartman TJ, Albert PS, Shields R, Slattery M, Caan B, Paskett E, Iber F, Kikendall JW, Lance P, et al. High dry bean intake and reduced risk of advanced colorectal adenoma recurrence among participants in the polyp prevention trial. J Nutr 2006;136:1896–903 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Food, nutrition and physical activity and the prevention of cancer: a global perspective. Washington, DC: American Institute for Cancer Research; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Dreyer NA, Schneeweiss S, McNeil BJ, Berger ML, Walker AM, Ollendorf DA, Gliklich RE, Initiative G. GRACE principles: recognizing high-quality observational studies of comparative effectiveness. Am J Manag Care 2010;16:467–71 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gotzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP, Initiative S. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Ann Intern Med 2007;147:573–7 Erratum in: Ann Intern Med 2008;148:168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Rothman KJ, Lash TL, Greenland S. Modern epidemiology. 3rd edition. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 126.Frongillo EA., Jr Validation of measures of food insecurity and hunger. J Nutr. 1999;129(Suppl 2S):506S–9S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Hebert JR, Gupta PC, Bhonsle RB, Sinor PN, Mehta H, Mehta FS. Development and testing of a quantitative food frequency questionnaire for use in Gujarat, India. Public Health Nutr 1999;2:39–50 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Hebert JR, Gupta PC, Bhonsle RB, Murti PR, Mehta H, Verghese F, Aghi M, Krishnaswamy K, Mehta FS. Development and testing of a quantitative food frequency questionnaire for use in Kerala, India. Public Health Nutr 1998;1:123–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Fowke JH, Hebert JR, Fahey JW. Urinary excretion of dithiocarbamates and self-reported Cruciferous vegetable intake: application of the “method of triads” to a food-specific biomarker. Public Health Nutr 2002;5:791–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Kabagambe EK, Baylin A, Allan DA, Siles X, Spiegelman D, Campos H. Application of the method of triads to evaluate the performance of food frequency questionnaires and biomarkers as indicators of long-term dietary intake. Am J Epidemiol 2001;154:1126–35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Attanasio OP. Evidence on public policy: methodological issues, political issues and examples. Scand J Public Health 2014;42(Suppl 13):28–40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Tovey D. The role of the Cochrane Collaboration in support of the WHO Nutrition Guidelines. Adv Nutr 2014;5:35–9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Berkman ND, Lohr KN, Ansari M, McDonagh M, Balk E, Whitlock E, Reston J, Bass E, Butler M, Gartlehner G, et al. Grading the strength of a body of evidence when assessing health care interventions for the effective health care program of the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality: an update. Rockville (MD): Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality 2013. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality Publication No. 13(14)-EHC130-EF. [PubMed]