Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2014 Jul 17;9(7):e101515. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101515

Preference for Male Traits Differ in Two Female Morphs of the Tree Lizard, Urosaurus ornatus

Matthew S Lattanzio 1,*,¤, Kevin J Metro 1, Donald B Miles 1
Editor: Kevin McGraw2
PMCID: PMC4102484  PMID: 25033282

Abstract

Non-random female mating preferences may contribute to the maintenance of phenotypic variation in color polymorphic species. However, the effect of female preference depends on the types of male traits used as signals by receptive females. If preference signals derive from discrete male traits (i.e., morph-specific), female preferences may rapidly fix to a morph. However, female preference signals may also include condition-dependent male traits. In this scenario, female preference may differ depending on the social context (i.e., male morph availability). Male tree lizards (Urosaurus ornatus) exhibit a dewlap color polymorphism that covaries with mating behavior. Blue morph males are aggressive and defend territories, yellow males are less aggressive and defend smaller territories, and orange males are typically nomadic. Female U. ornatus are also polymorphic in dewlap color, but the covariation between dewlap color and female behavior is unknown. We performed an experiment to determine how female mate choice depends on the visual and chemical signals produced by males. We also tested whether female morphs differ in their preferences for these signals. Female preferences involved both male dewlap color and size of the ventral color patch. However, the female morphs responded to these signals differently and depended on the choice between the types of male morphs. Our experiment revealed that females may be capable of distinguishing among the male morphs using chemical signals alone. Yellow females exhibit preferences based on both chemical and visual signals, which may be a strategy to avoid ultra-dominant males. In contrast, orange females may prefer dominant males. We conclude that female U. ornatus morphs differ in mating behavior. Our findings also provide evidence for a chemical polymorphism among male lizards in femoral pore secretions.

Introduction

Female preference may contribute to the maintenance of morphological variation in species where males exhibit discrete phenotypes [1], [2]. Because females rely on multiple signals to assess potential mates [3], the selective consequences of female behavior depends in part on the types of male traits used as signals to inform preference. In polymorphic species, female preference signals may be based on discrete traits that are correlated with morph genetic differences or condition-dependent ornaments. Discrete sexual signals have the potential to lead to rapid fixation of female preference to a single or multiple male morphs [4], [5]. In contrast, use of condition-dependent male traits as signals for female preference may not favor assortative mating. The importance of condition-dependent or discrete male traits for female preference may differ with respect to the social context a female experiences (i.e., availability of each male morph). Understanding the contribution of different male traits to female preference across different social contexts may provide insight into the role of female preference in the maintenance of morphological variation.

Species exhibiting a discrete color polymorphism within a population present an excellent opportunity to evaluate the use of discrete and condition-dependent male traits in female preference. Color polymorphism occurs in a diverse range of taxa, and color differences are assumed to reflect alternative ecological [6] or reproductive [2], [7] strategies. In the latter case, color morphs are associated with phenotypic syndromes that integrate physiological, morphological, and behavioral traits that influence reproductive success [8], [9]. Multiple factors have been implicated in the maintenance of color morphs and include negative frequency-dependent selection [7] or spatial variation in selection. However, the role of mating preferences in the maintenance of discrete phenotypes is gaining support [1], [10][13].

In several lizard species, discrete male dewlap color differences are associated with alternative reproductive tactics [7], [14][16], and color morphs differ in behaviors that affect their reproductive success [2]. Male ornate tree lizards (Urosaurus ornatus) exhibit a color polymorphism that involves a solid blue, yellow, or orange dewlap linked with differences in aggression and resource-holding potential [8]. Blue males aggressively defend territories, yellow males exhibit satellite behavior or defend territories, and orange males are nomadic. Male tree lizards also possess two additional traits that may influence female preference. First, all males express a blue ventral patch on their abdomen. The size and color components of this patch vary continuously among individual males [17], [18], suggesting that expression of this trait may be condition-dependent [19]. Moreover, ventral patch size has a positive correlation to traits that signal resource-holding potential, such as bite force [17]. Second, individual males may differ in body condition (residual mass per unit body size). Body condition is an estimate of individual energy reserves [20], and may convey information regarding a males’ ability to succeed in agonistic contests, defend resources, or survival potential [20]. Because yellow, orange and blue males overlap in snout-vent length [17], body condition may convey information about male quality uncorrelated with the color signal. Differences in male dewlap color, ventral patch size, and body condition may all convey information to a female tree lizard regarding mate quality.

Female preferences may differ by social context given the costs and benefits of mate choice [5], [21]. For example, although male dewlap color is associated with variation in aggressive behavior and resource holding potential [22], female tree lizards may risk injury by associating with an aggressive male [23]. However, non-aggressive or subordinate males may be unable to defend a territory and access to nest sites, which may favor a sneaker strategy. Female tree lizards may evaluate both discrete (dewlap color) and condition-dependent (ventral patch size and color, and body condition) signals associated with social dominance to choose mates in these contexts. In species like U. ornatus, female preference may also be influenced by female morph [21], [24], [25]. If fitness costs associated with mate choice differ between female morphs, then their mating preferences may also differ. Jointly, context-dependent and morph-specific female preferences may induce variability to preference functions and their contribution to male differences in polymorphic systems [21], [24], [26].

The results of multiple studies also demonstrate the importance of male chemical signals for intraspecific communication in lizards [27][29]. In many lizard species, males exude femoral pore secretions. The compounds present within these secretions may provide information on aspects of male quality, such as immunocompetence [27] and health [30]. In addition, females may also be capable of discriminating among male color morphs by femoral pore secretions alone [31]. Previous work hints that male U. ornatus femoral pore secretions play a role in intraspecific communication [32], but the contribution of these secretions to female preference is unclear.

Here we evaluate the contribution of discrete and condition-dependent male traits to female preference in U. ornatus in order to gain insight into the role of female preference in maintaining phenotypic variation in polymorphic species. If female preferences are fixed to a specific morph, then females should exhibit assortative behavior towards that morph when it is present. The influence of male condition-dependent traits on mate choice is predicted to be low or absent in this scenario. Alternatively, if female preferences are not fixed, females may supplement information from fixed signals and use variation in condition-dependent male traits as preference signals. Use of condition-dependent male traits may be enhanced in social contexts where the available males differ in dewlap color from the female.

Next, we conducted an experiment to test the hypothesis that female U. ornatus are capable of discriminating among male morphs using chemical signals alone. We then compared patterns of female behavior in response to male visual (dewlap color) and chemical (femoral pore secretion) signals. Similar non-random patterns in female behavior towards male morphs in both modalities would provide evidence that chemical and dewlap color cues provide complementary information to receptive females.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

Experiments were approved by the Ohio University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol #R06–07) and the Arizona Department of Fish and Game (permit #SP792912).

Capture Methods and Lizard Morphology

Adult male and non-gravid female U. ornatus were collected at the Appleton-Whittell Research Ranch in Santa Cruz County, Arizona (31° 35.428 N, 110° 30.388 W) during July 2011, which falls within the breeding season of U. ornatus at our study site. Lizards were sexed based on the presence of enlarged post-anal scales (males). The frequency of each U. ornatus color morph varies by population. At our study site, male U. ornatus lizards have a solid blue (51%), yellow (20.4%), or orange (18.4%) dewlap (Figure S1). Female U. ornatus have either a solid yellow (48.1%) or orange (40.7%) dewlap (Figure S1). Females without dewlap coloration (11.2%) and bicolor (10.2%) male morphs also occur, but at low encounter frequencies. We excluded these morphs in our study. The morph (dewlap color) of each lizard was recorded upon capture. Body size (snout-vent length, SVL) was measured to the nearest 0.5 mm, and mass was recorded to the nearest 0.1 gram using a Pesola scale (Pesola AG, Baar, Switzerland).

Male U. ornatus possess a blue ventral patch that is exposed to females during courtship [33]. The size of this patch varies among males [17]. Digital images of the ventral patch were obtained by scanning the ventral surface of each individual male lizard in a Canon LiDE flatbed scanner (Canon, Melville, NY, USA). Prior to scanning, we warmed each male lizard under a light (75 W) until it reached a body temperature of ca. 34–36°C to ensure full expression of its blue ventral patch [34]. The size (in pixels) of this patch was then estimated using the polygon tool in the program ImageJ version 1.44 p [35]. We calculated ventral patch size (mm2) from these polygons. A ruler placed on the platen served as a reference for converting patch size measurements.

Color Measurements

Reflectance data from the dewlap and ventral patches of each male lizard were measured following established methodology [36][39]. We used an Ocean Optics USB2000-UV-VIS spectrometer with a pulsed fiber-optic light source (Ocean Optics Worldwide, Dunedin, FL, USA) to record reflectance data. All reflectance measurements were made relative to a WS-1 white standard (Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA) and to darkness. We recorded reflectance data spanning the visual range of lizards (320–700 nm) [40], in 0.3 nm increments. We recorded three reflectance measurements consisting of an average of 20 readings at rostral, central, and caudal portions of each color patch (integration time = 295 ms, boxcar correction = 10) (refer to Figure S1 for photographs of each morph illustrating dewlap and ventral color patch variation among male lizards). For each measurement, we held the probe at a 45° angle above the color patch. A non-reflective opaque tube was affixed to the probe to block ambient light.

Reflectance data were averaged over these three regions for each patch to ensure that we captured the color variation present across the entire surface of a patch. These reflectance data were further divided into visible (vis-, 400–700 nm) and ultraviolet (uv-, 320–399 nm) ranges [41]. Preliminary analyses suggested that only blue morph males reflect >5% in the uv-range in their dewlaps (Figure S2). Male ventral patches also varied little in their uv-range percent reflectance (Figure S2) and the color components extracted from these data (i.e., 19 of 29 males expressed same chromatic values, see below for color extraction procedure). We retained vis-range reflectance data from the ventral color patches for further analyses.

We extracted vis-range values each of brightness, hue, and chroma following methods in [36] implemented using the program Spectre (https://pantherfile.uwm.edu/pdunn/www/Spectre/Spectre.html). We applied a principal component analysis (PCA) to condense the resulting hue, chroma, and brightness values into a single color score based on extraction of the scores from the first principal component axis (PC1) [39], [42]. This axis accounted for between 76.1 and 76.4% of the variation in visible-range color properties of the dewlap and ventral patches, respectively (Table 1).

Table 1. Component loadings of the first axis of two Principle Components Analyses (PCAs) performed on dewlap and ventral color patch color properties, respectively.

Male color patch Variance explained (%) Eigenvalue Factor loadings
Brightness Hue Chroma
Dewlap color patch 76.1 1.82 0.679 −0.66 −0.322
Ventral color patch 76.4 2.29 0.572 0.519 −0.635

We used PCA to describe dewlap and ventral patch hue, chroma, and brightness as an independent color score (based on the first PCA axis, PC1) [36]. Factor loadings indicate the relative direction and magnitude of contribution by each color component to a score.

Female Preference for Male Visual Signals

Our first experiment assessed the preferences of female U. ornatus for the different male morphs. Individual male (n = 29) and female (n = 19) lizards were held in separate 5.7 liter covered terraria (27.9×17.8×12.7 cm, l×w×h, Frey Scientific, Nashua, NH, USA) at a field laboratory throughout the duration of this experiment. We maintained lizards on a 15∶9 h (light:dark) photoperiod. Temperatures fluctuated with ambient conditions. Lizards were offered two calcium gut-loaded crickets (Acheta domestica) daily and provided water ad libitum. Each enclosure had paper affixed to its sides to prevent lizards from interacting prior to experimentation.

Males were grouped by morph (yellow [n = 8], blue [n = 9], or orange [n = 12]; hereafter Y, B, or O). Female yellow (n = 9) and orange (n = 10) morphs were randomly exposed to one of three polymorphic male dyads, Y-O, B-O, or Y-B, for their first trial [43]. Following that trial, each female was then randomly assigned to one of the remaining two dyads. Females were allowed 24 hours to rest between successive trials. Pairs of males were chosen at random from each morph category. We also attempted to minimize body size differences in each dyad (mean ± SD male size difference: SVL, 1.4±1.2 mm; mass, 0.3±0.2 g).

We used an experimental arena (total size: 1×0.3×0.3 m, l×w×h) lined with a 1-cm layer of sand for observing the behavior of females during exposure to males. We placed males into two chambers (0.3×0.3 m, l×w) on opposite sides of this arena. Males were prevented from interacting with a female by clear plastic sheets secured to the arena floor and walls, and each chamber also had covers to prevent airflow between chambers. Because male U. ornatus femoral pore secretions are non-volatile [28], these dividers and lids ensured that female assessment of the males only involved visual signals. Moreover, while the two males could see each other in the arena, results from staged contests between male U. ornatus indicate that they only initiate display behavior with each other when in close proximity (ca. <20 cm, Lattanzio & Miles, unpublished data). We designed our experimental arena so that males are always ≥40 cm apart from each other, and we did not observe any male-male interactions during this experiment. Equal heat and lighting was maintained throughout this arena with the aid of suspended 75 W lamps. We used sand as a substrate in the arena and replaced this sand at the conclusion of each trial. At the beginning of a trial we placed a female lizard into the center of the arena and oriented perpendicular to the two male chambers. Each female was allowed 5 minutes to acclimate to the arena, at which point we simultaneously introduced one male into each side chamber. We randomized the side a male morph was placed for each trial. We allowed females to observe the males for a period of eight hours, beginning between 0800 and 1000 h. This study design ensured that we observed lizards throughout their daily activity period (ca. 0800–1800 h), thereby providing a more accurate assessment of female preference [44]. One observer (KJM) recorded the position of the focal female at 30 min intervals from behind a blind. Each trial resulted in 16 observations. Female visits were scored based on their proximity to each male. We considered a male to be visited if the female was within 10 cm of his chamber, and assigned a score of zero if a female was >10 cm from either male. At the end of each trial we tallied the number of visits toward each male. Tree lizards are sedentary, ambush predators and move infrequently throughout the day (Lattanzio, personal observation). Female U. ornatus do not shuttle even during thermoregulatory behavior, and will spend more time near a male if they exhibit an actual preference for that male in the wild (Miles, unpublished data). Thus, the location of a female at one of our 16 observation points reflects both her position for most (if not all) of the previous 30 min and, depending on her position in the arena, her preference for a male.

Female Preference for Male Chemical Signals

We conducted a second experiment to determine whether female U. ornatus are capable of discriminating among male color morphs using their femoral pore secretions. For these trials, we used the same females from the visual choice experiment. We obtained chemical secretions from a second sample of male lizards captured from the same population. Females did not interact with these males prior to or during experimentation. Upon completion of the visual experiment, we returned all females to their individual enclosures for 10 days. Food (crickets) and water were provided daily. Chemical preference trials were then conducted within a different arena (total size: 1.22×0.15×0.25 m, l×w×h). We suspended 75 W lights above this arena to maintain similar heat and lighting conditions throughout this arena. We used sand as a substrate in this experiment and replaced it between all trials.

We used small plastic trays (80×50×14 mm, l×w×h; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) as tiles in this experiment. The same male dyads were used in this experiment to facilitate comparisons with the visual experiment. We exposed each female to two of the three dyads in a random order. Dyad exposure was randomized so females did not necessarily experience the same two male dyads in this experiment that she experienced in the visual experiment. Femoral pore secretions were collected from each male morph using sterile cotton tipped applicators (Medline Industries, Mundelein, Illinois, USA). To collect pore secretions, we first dipped an applicator into distilled water and then rubbed on the proximal portion of a males’ hind limbs (location of femoral pores) for 60 seconds. Applicators were labeled by morph and stored in individual plastic freezer bags within a larger bag (one per morph) at −20°C in a freezer [45]. To apply a scent, we randomly selected an applicator from the appropriate bag and swabbed the tile surface for a period of 30 seconds, immediately prior to experimentation. Nitrile gloves were worn during collection and application procedures. We placed two scented tiles in the arena so that each tile was approximately 5 cm from its end of the arena. At the start of a trial, we placed a female into the center of an arena, perpendicular to the direction of either tile. The position of the female in the arena was recorded after 20 minutes, and every 10 minutes thereafter, for a total of 90 minutes (eight readings total). Female visits were scored based on their proximity to a tile. A score of zero was assigned if a female was observed >5 cm from either tile. We discarded used tiles and applicators after each trial. One observer (MSL) recorded female positions during this experiment. At the end of each trial we tallied the number of visits toward each male.

Statistical Analysis

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to test whether male morphs differ in dewlap color. We found no relationship between male ventral patch size and ventral patch color score (r = 0.065, t = 0.34, df = 27, P = 0.737). We therefore applied separate analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) to test whether the male morphs differ in ventral patch size or color, respectively. We used body size (SVL) as a covariate in both models. A homogeneity of slopes test indicated no interaction between male morph and SVL in either model (both P>0.1). We used the coefficient of variation (i.e., standard deviation/mean) to describe relative variation in ventral patch size among males of each morph [46].

Fisher’s exact tests were used to evaluate whether yellow or orange females exhibited a bias towards either side of the arena, independent of male morph, across the first and second trials for each female. We used a poisson regression to determine whether female morphs differed in mating preferences (number of visits to each male). In this model we included four factors accounting for the interaction between female morph, male dyad, and each male trait (morph, body condition, ventral patch size, or ventral patch color). Body condition was calculated as the residuals of a regression predicting mass from SVL [20]. A significant result associated with any of these predictors would suggest that female morphs differ in their preferences for male lizards based on the trait considered. Ventral patch size and color were treated as separate predictors because these patch properties are uncorrelated (F 1,27 = 0.3, r = 0.03, P = 0.872).

We also used Fisher’s exact tests to investigate side bias and a poisson regression to evaluate the number of visits by females to each morph in each dyad in the chemical experiment. Finally, we used an ANOVA to compare the strength of preference (SOP) by each morph to the different male morphs between the visual and chemical experiments with an experiment×female morph×male dyad interaction as a factor. Here, SOP refers to the absolute value of the difference in number of visits to each male in the dyad by a female during a trial. A non-significant effect of the experiment×female morph×male dyad interaction provides evidence that male U. ornatus visual and chemical signals convey similar information to a receptive female. We included female identity as a covariate in this analysis.

Data were log10-transformed when necessary, and we used post-hoc pairwise comparisons to compare factors with >2 levels. We conducted all statistical analyses in r 3.02 [47] and SPSS 19 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

Morphological Variation among Male U. ornatus Lizards

Male U. ornatus dewlap color scores differed among the three morphs (F 2,26 = 8.23, P = 0.002). Blue males had brighter dewlaps than orange (pairwise comparisons, P = 0.015) and yellow males (pairwise comparisons, P = 0.002), but yellow and orange males did not differ (pairwise comparisons, P = 0.451, Figure 1). In contrast, morphs do not differ in ventral patch size (F 2,25 = 1.59, P = 0.225; B males, 202.5±10.1 mm2; O males, 185.1±6 mm2; Y males, 185.2±6.3 mm2). Intra-morph variation in patch size was also similar (coefficient of variation, %: CVB = 14.9%, CVY = 9.6%, CVO = 11.2%). Ventral patch size does not covary with body size (F 1,25 = 2.06, P = 0.163). Male morphs also exhibit similar ventral patch color scores (F 2,25 = 0.47, P = 0.628). There is no relationship between ventral patch color score and body size (F 1,25 = 1.21, P = 0.283).

Figure 1. Dewlap color differences among male tree lizard (Urosaurus ornatus) morphs.

Figure 1

Dewlap color refers to scores from the first principal component axis derived from hue, chroma, and brightness data collected from male dewlaps (n = 29 males, see Materials and Methods for color procedure). Male lizards are grouped by morph: blue, orange, or yellow (see Figure S1). Values are mean ±1.0 standard error (SE).

Female Preference for Male Visual Signals

Yellow and orange morph female U. ornatus did not bias their visits towards a particular side of the arena (yellow females, Fisher’s exact test, P = 0.729; orange females, Fisher’s exact test, P = 0.878). Yellow and orange morph female U. ornatus differed in their responses to male lizards with respect to male morph and ventral patch size only (female morph×male dyad×male morph, χ 2 = 26.32, df = 11, P = 0.006; female morph×male dyad×male ventral patch size, χ 2 = 21.97, df = 6, P = 0.001). In contrast, the two female morphs did not differ in their responses to male lizards with respect to male body condition and ventral patch color score (female morph×male dyad×body condition, χ 2 = 5.49, df = 6, P = 0.483; female morph×male dyad×ventral patch color score, χ 2 = 6.91, df = 6, P = 0.329).

Both male morph and ventral patch size predicted the number of visits to a male by the two female U. ornatus morphs. Yellow and orange females behaved similarly in the Y-O dyad (pairwise comparisons, all P>0.07), with the exception that orange females visited yellow males more often than yellow females visited orange males (pairwise comparisons, P = 0.002). Both yellow and orange female morphs preferred yellow males in this dyad (pairwise comparisons, P = 0.041 and P = 0.016, respectively) (Figure 2). Moreover, orange females visited males with a larger ventral patch more often than males with a smaller patch in this dyad (χ 2 = 4.61, df = 1, P = 0.032) (Figure 3).

Figure 2. Preference of female Urosaurus ornatus when presented with orange and yellow males (visual experiment).

Figure 2

Preference by yellow (light grey) and orange (dark grey) morph females refers to a greater number of visits towards one male over another and bars are mean+1.0 standard error (SE). Both yellow and orange females preferred yellow over orange morph males (see Results).

Figure 3. Relationship between male Urosaurus ornatus ventral patch size and orange female preference (visual experiment).

Figure 3

Preference refers to the number of visits by orange females to males that differ in ventral patch size (mm2) in the Y-O dyad. The grey shaded region denotes 95% confidence intervals around predicted values from a poisson regression.

Neither female morph exhibited a preference for a single male morph in the B-O dyad (pairwise comparisons, yellow females, P = 0.606; orange females, P = 0.077). However, yellow females visited males with smaller ventral patches more often than males with larger ventral patches in the B-O dyad (χ 2 = 7.66, df = 1, P = 0.006) (Figure 4). Moreover, both yellow and orange females visited orange males more often in the B-O dyad than in the Y-O dyad (pairwise comparisons, yellow females, P<0.001; orange females, P = 0.003). Both female morphs behaved similarly in the Y-B dyad and did not exhibit a preference for either male morph (pairwise comparisons, all P>0.07).

Figure 4. Relationship between male Urosaurus ornatus ventral patch size and yellow female preference (visual experiment).

Figure 4

Preference refers to the number of visits by yellow females to males that differ in ventral patch size (mm2) in the B-O dyad. The grey shaded region denotes 95% confidence intervals around predicted values from a poisson regression.

Female Preference for Male Chemical Signals

A preliminary study revealed that female U. ornatus are able to discriminate between tiles scented with male femoral pore secretions and unscented (distilled water), based on differences in number of tongue-flicks towards each stimulus (χ2 = 5.33, df = 1, P = 0.021, n = 12 females [six orange, six yellow]). We found no evidence of side bias for either female morph in this experiment (yellow females, Fisher’s exact test, P = 0.864; orange females, Fisher’s exact test, P = 0.266).

Both female morphs exhibited similar mating preferences with respect to male morph in the chemical experiment (female morph×male dyad×male morph, χ 2 = 15.08, df = 11, P = 0.178). Yellow females preferred yellow males over orange males in the Y-O dyad (pairwise comparisons, P = 0.002) (Figure 5). In addition, yellow females exhibited a stronger preference for yellow males than orange females did for either male morph in this dyad (pairwise comparisons, P = 0.021 and P = 0.013 for yellow and orange males, respectively).

Figure 5. Yellow female Urosaurus ornatus preference for chemical signals of yellow and orange males.

Figure 5

Preference refers to the number of visits by yellow females to either tile in the Y-O dyad. Bars are mean+1.0 standard error (SE). Yellow females preferred tiles scented with yellow male secretions (see Results).

Patterns of Female Behavior across Signal Modalities

Yellow and orange females displayed a similar SOP for males in both experiments (experiment×female morph×male dyad interaction, F 11,63 = 0.64, P = 0.791). In other words, male femoral pore secretions and dewlap color elicited similar behavioral responses by females with respect to their mating preferences (Figure 6). Repeated use of individual females did not affect female SOP (F 1,63 = 2.3, P = 0.135). These results are also robust to weighting SOP by the total number of visits to both males in each trial (both P>0.43).

Figure 6. Strength of preference for male Urosaurus ornatus by the two female morphs.

Figure 6

Strength of preference (absolute difference in number of visits) for both yellow (A) and orange (B) morph female U. ornatus in each dyad. Bars are shaded by experiment (grey: visual, white: chemical) and are mean+1.0 standard error (SE).

Discussion

Our findings highlight heterogeneity in mate preferences by two female U. ornatus morphs for the three male morphs. Both yellow and orange female morphs had a preference for a specific male morph, but this preference was contingent on the pairing presented to the female. Both yellow and orange females preferred yellow males, but only over orange males. Orange females also exhibited a preference for males with a larger ventral patch (a continuous trait) in this dyad. In contrast, yellow females only used male ventral patch size signals when both males differed in dewlap color from the female (i.e., the B-O dyad). Although yellow and orange morph females behaved similarly in the chemical experiment, yellow females exhibited a stronger preference for yellow males over orange males. Overall, both female morphs behaved similarly towards male morphs across the visual and chemical experiments (see Figure 6), supporting that male morphs may have different femoral pore secretions. We conclude that female preferences in U. ornatus vary depending on male morph availability. Our work also provides some evidence for a chemical polymorphism among male U. ornatus.

In male U. ornatus, lizards with a larger ventral patch have a greater bite force, a trait linked with social dominance [17]. In support of our prediction, ventral patch size appeared to influence yellow females when offered a choice between a blue and an orange male. In U. ornatus and many other species, males defend territories and access to critical resources (e.g., oviposition sites). In U. ornatus, males with blue dewlaps are the dominant, highly-aggressive morph in resource contests [8]. However, females risk injury or mortality by associating with an aggressive phenotype [23], [48], [49], suggesting that aggressive males should not always be preferred mates [50]. For example, in three-spine sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus), highly-aggressive males often fail at mating attempts but less-aggressive males are able to mate repeatedly with the same female [51]. Likewise, female Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) prefer males that lose over males that win competitive interactions [52].

Preference by yellow female U. ornatus for yellow over orange males in one social context and towards males with smaller ventral patches in a different context (i.e., choice between a blue and an orange male) suggests that yellow female U. ornatus may also avoid associating with highly-aggressive males [17]. Females preferring less-aggressive males may lose both direct (e.g., access to resources) and indirect (e.g., offspring sired by higher-genetic quality males) reproductive benefits [50]. However, because all three male U. ornatus morphs are capable of defending resources to a limited degree [53], preference for a less-aggressive male may provide a female with reproductive benefits (e.g., resource access) and minimal risk of injury during courtship.

When offered a choice between a yellow and an orange male, orange females preferred both yellow males and males with a larger ventral patch. It may be that orange females prefer the male that may be more likely to succeed in an agonistic encounter (i.e., males with a larger ventral patch) [17]. In other dyads, orange females did not exhibit a preference for males based on any traits considered. However, when offered a choice between a yellow and a blue male, the more-aggressive male morphs in U. ornatus (Lattanzio, unpublished data) [8], orange females tended to prefer the male having the larger ventral patch (75% of trials). This preference may provide reproductive benefits to an orange female (i.e., access to higher-quality nest sites) that may outweigh potential costs [50]. However, the extent to which males injure females during courtship (or other social interactions) in this species is unclear. Observations of male-female interactions in the wild or a laboratory setting are needed to add further insight into the costs and benefits of female choice in U. ornatus.

Female U. ornatus dewlap color is fixed at sexual maturity (Lattanzio unpublished data) [54], suggesting that other traits such as reproductive behavior may correlate with color differences among the morphs [55]. Our findings provide evidence that yellow and orange female morphs differ in signals used for mate choice. Female preference is likely dynamic in polymorphic species [21], and variable depending on the social context and morph of the female [24], [25]. Although yellow and orange morph female U. ornatus may use similar signals (dewlap color, ventral patch size) to select a male, their use of these signals differs both between them and among the social contexts examined in this study. Our inability to detect non-random behavior by yellow and orange females exposed to yellow and blue males may not necessarily reflect a lack of preference by females in these contexts. Rather, for yellow females at least, yellow males were preferred by yellow females in 83% of trials in this social context. This suggests a hierarchy in yellow female behavior: associate with same-colored males when they are available and, if not, associate with males having a smaller ventral patch. And, it may be that orange females use additional signals derived from other male traits as mate choice criteria, such as body size [43]. Because we controlled for body size in selecting the dyads, we are unable to detect preference for this trait by females. Body size influences both the outcome of male territorial contests [56] and female choice experiments in other lizard species [43]. For orange females, preference for larger-sized males would complement our findings that this morph prefers dominant males, but more work is needed to address this consideration.

Unlike dewlap color or ventral patch size, the role of femoral pore secretions in the mating behavior of U. ornatus is unclear. Previous work suggests that both sexes are capable of differentiating between conspecific femoral pore secretions and those produced by themselves [32]. We show that both chemical and dewlap color signals may elicit similar patterns of female behavior, suggesting that femoral pore chemical secretions also differ among male U. ornatus morphs. Yellow females discriminated among yellow and orange male morphs using chemical signals alone. Orange females behaved randomly with respect to male chemical signals. However, we did observe that orange females associated with a yellow male in 63% of all Y-O dyad trials in the chemical experiment. Male femoral pore secretions may signal individual quality, competitive ability, and dominance status to a conspecific [57], and consequently may be important for guiding female mating decisions [27], [30], [45], [58]. Moreover, the evolutionary maintenance of both visual and chemical signals in intersexual communication may be favored if they invoke redundant behavioral responses by a female [29]. Male U. ornatus chemical secretions may therefore be under selection to covary with the reproductive behavior differences that characterize their color polymorphism [31]. Our observation of similar patterns of female behavior with respect to male dewlap color and femoral pore secretions in these experiments provides some initial support for this hypothesis. Additional work is required to determine the degree of morph-specificity in the chemical profiles of male U. ornatus secretions [59]. An association between femoral pore secretions and male visual signals would provide further support that they are redundant in their role in female mate choice in U. ornatus [31], [59], [60].

Conclusions

The combination of male morphs and non-random female preference within a population may enhance rates of sympatric divergence, provided that female preferences are fixed to discrete male traits and do not vary across different social contexts [4], [5]. An alternative hypothesis is that female preference is context-dependent and involves evaluating signals deriving from both discrete and condition-dependent male traits. These preferences, in conjunction with other mechanisms of selection acting in polymorphic systems (e.g., male-male competition and predation), might serve to contribute to the maintenance of discrete male phenotypes in a population. The results of [17] and our study support this hypothesis, and further suggest that female preference and male-male competition may be antagonistic in their effects on male ventral patch size, favoring the maintenance of overlap in patch size among all males [61]. Female mate selection may also be affected by male-male competition if the spatial dispersion of males in a population relative to a females’ preferred resources (e.g., oviposition sites) is non-random. Resources are abundant at our study site and the spatial dispersion of male morphs in the population is effectively random with respect to those resources (i.e., trees, Lattanzio & Miles, unpublished data). We therefore do not expect male competition to exert a significant influence on the probability of a female encountering a preferred male at this site. Alternatively, competition in resource-limited habitats may favor spatial reorganization within a population towards the monopolization of preferred resources by aggressive males [62]. In this scenario, male-male interactions will affect the context and probability of female encounters with each male morph in a population. An interaction between resource availability, morph status signals (visual and chemical), and female preference should have important implications for the maintenance and spatial dispersion of polymorphic phenotypes across a species’ range.

Supporting Information

Figure S1

Adult male (A) and female (B) Urosaurus ornatus color morphs. In U. ornatus, both sexes exhibit a polymorphism in dewlap color which is fixed at maturity. Individual males also exhibit variation in the size of their blue ventral patch, but these size differences are not fixed to different morphs (P>0.2, see Results). Female U. ornatus do not express this patch.

(TIF)

Figure S2

Percent reflectance of male Urosaurus ornatus dewlap and ventral patches. For dewlap patches (A), lines are colored by male morph: blue, orange, or yellow. For ventral patches (B), lines are shaded by male morph: blue (n = 16, black line), orange (n = 8, dark grey line), or yellow (n = 5, light grey line). Male U. ornatus lizards included in this figure were captured at the same study site as males used in the current study. The spectral range shown in both graphs includes ultraviolet (300–399 nm, grey shaded region) and visible light (400–700 nm). Values used to construct these graphs are mean ±1.0 standard error (SE) percent reflectance at 10-nm intervals.

(TIFF)

Raw Data S1

Female preference experiment, male morphology, and Figure S2 databases.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

Kortney Jaworski and Katherine Allen assisted in the field. Linda Kennedy provided access to the study site and logistical support. Vincent Farallo, Kortney Jaworski, Michelle Rodrigues, Kelly Williams, and four anonymous reviewers provided useful comments on earlier permutations of this manuscript.

Funding Statement

Support for this research was provided by an Ariel-Appleton Research Fund and an Ohio University Graduate Student Senate Original Work Grant, both awarded to MSL, and a stipend from the Ohio University Program to Aid in Career Exploration awarded to KJM. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Pryke SR, Griffith SC (2007) The relative role of male vs. female mate choice in maintaining assortative pairing among discrete colour morphs. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20: 1512–1521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Gray SM, McKinnon JS (2007) Linking color polymorphism maintenance and speciation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 22: 71–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Candolin U (2003) The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biological Reviews 78: 575–595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fisher R (1930) The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Dover, New York.
  • 5. Sinervo B, Svensson E (2002) Correlational selection and the evolution of genomic architecture. Heredity 89: 329–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Forsman A, Ringblom K, Civantos E, Ahnesjo J (2002) Coevolution of color polymorphism pattern and thermoregulatory behavior in polymorphic pygmy grasshoppers Tetrix undulata . Evolution 56: 349–360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Sinervo B, Lively CM (1996) The rock-paper-scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies. Nature 380: 240–243. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Thompson CW, Moore MC (1991) Throat colour reliably signals status in male tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus . Animal Behaviour 42: 745–753. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Pryke SR, Griffith SC (2006) Red dominates black: agonistic signalling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biological Sciences Series B 273: 949–957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Healey M, Uller T, Olsson M (2008) Variety is the Spice of Life: Female Lizards Choose to Associate with Colour-Polymorphic Male Dyads. Ethology 114: 231–237. [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Pérez i de Lanuza G, Font E, Carazo P (2013) Color-assortative mating in a color-polymorphic lacertid lizard. Behavioral Ecology 24: 273–279. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Roulin A (2004) The evolution, maintenance and adaptive function of genetic colour polymorphism in birds. Biological Reviews 79: 815–848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Kingston JJ, Rosenthal GG, Ryan MJ (2003) The role of sexual selection in maintaining a colour polymorphism in the pygmy swordtail, Xiphophorus pygmaeus . Animal Behaviour 65: 735–743. [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Sinervo B, Heulin B, Surget-Groba Y, Clobert J, Miles DB, et al. (2007) Models of density-dependent genic selection and a new rock-paper-scissors social system. American Naturalist 170: 663–680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Huyghe K, Vanhooydonck B, Herrel A, Tadić Z, Van Damme R (2007) Morphology, performance, behavior and ecology of three color morphs in males of the lizard Podarcis melisellensis . Integrative and Comparative Biology 47: 211–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Calsbeek B, Hasselquist D, Clobert J (2010) Multivariate phenotypes and the potential for alternative phenotypic optima in wall lizard (Podarcis muralis) ventral colour morphs. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23: 1138–1147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Meyers JJ, Irschick DJ, Vanhooydonck B, Herrel A (2006) Divergent roles for multiple sexual signals in a polygynous lizard. Functional Ecology 20: 709–716. [Google Scholar]
  • 18. McElroy EJ, Marien C, Meyers JJ, Irschick DJ (2007) Do Displays Send Information about Ornament Structure and Male Quality in the Ornate Tree Lizard, Urosaurus ornatus ? Ethology 113: 1113–1122. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. San-Jose LM, Granado-Lorencio F, Sinervo B, Fitze PS (2013) Iridophores and not carotenoids account for chromatic variation of carotenoid-based coloration in common lizards (Lacerta vivipara). The American Naturalist 181: 396–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Schulte-Hostedde AI, Zinner B, Millar JS, Hickling GJ (2005) Restitution of mass-size residuals: validating body condition indices. Ecology 86: 155–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Alonzo SH, Sinervo B (2001) Mate choice games, context-dependent good genes, and genetic cycles in the side-blotched lizard, Uta stansburiana . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 49: 176–186. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Carpenter GC (1995) Modeling Dominance: The Influence of Size, Coloration, and Experience on Dominance Relations in Tree Lizards (Urosaurus ornatus). Herpetological Monographs 9: 88–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Moore AJ, Gowaty PA, Wallin WG, Moore PJ (2001) Sexual conflict and the evolution of female mate choice and male social dominance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 268: 517–523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Bleay C, Sinervo B (2007) Discrete genetic variation in mate choice and a condition-dependent preference function in the side-blotched lizard: implications for the formation and maintenance of coadapted gene complexes. Behavioral Ecology 18: 304–310. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Vercken E, Massot M, Sinervo B, Clobert J (2007) Colour variation and alternative reproductive strategies in females of the common lizard Lacerta vivipara . Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20: 221–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Lancaster LT, Hipsley CA, Sinervo B (2009) Female choice for optimal combinations of multiple male display traits increases offspring survival. Behavioral Ecology 20: 993–999. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. López P, Martín J (2005) Female Iberian wall lizards prefer male scents that signal a better cell-mediated immune response. Biology Letters 1: 404–406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Alberts AC (1991) Phylogenetic and adaptive variation in lizard femoral gland secretions. Copeia: 69–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Thompson JT, Bissell AN, Martins E, iacute, lia P (2008) Inhibitory interactions between multimodal behavioural responses may influence the evolution of complex signals. Animal Behaviour 76: 113–121. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Kopena R, Martín J, López P, Herczeg G (2011) Vitamin E Supplementation Increases the Attractiveness of Males’ Scent for Female European Green Lizards. PLoS ONE 6: e19410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. López P, Moreira PL, Martín J (2009) Chemical Polymorphism and Chemosensory Recognition between Iberolacerta monticola Lizard Color Morphs. Chemical Senses 34: 723–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Quinn VS, Hews DK (2005) Detection and response to conspecific chemical cues by ornate tree lizards (Urosaurus ornatus). Journal of Herpetology 39: 496–499. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Carpenter CC, Grubitz G III (1961) Time-Motion Study of a Lizard. Ecology 42: 199–200. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Morrison RL, Sherbrooke WC, Frost-Mason SK (1996) Temperature-Sensitive, Physiologically Active Iridophores in the Lizard Urosaurus ornatus: An Ultrastructural Analysis of Color Change. Copeia 1996: 804–812. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Abràmoff MD, Magalhães PJ, Ram SJ (2004) Image processing with Image J. Biophotonics International. 11: 36–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Endler JA (1990) On the Measurement and Classification of Color in Studies of Animal Color Patterns. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 41: 315–352. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Robertson JM, Rosenblum EB (2009) Rapid divergence of social signal coloration across the White Sands ecotone for three lizard species under strong natural selection. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 98: 243–255. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Rosenblum EB (2006) Convergent evolution and divergent selection: Lizards at the White Sands ecotone. The American Naturalist 167: 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Martin M, eacute lissa, Meylan S, Gomez D, et al. (2013) Ultraviolet and carotenoid-based coloration in the viviparous lizard Zootoca vivipara (Squamata: Lacertidae) in relation to age, sex, and morphology. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 110: 128–141. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Loew ER, Fleishman LJ, Foster RG, Provencio I (2002) Visual pigments and oil droplets in diurnal lizards: a comparative study of Caribbean anoles. Journal of Experimental Biology 205: 927–938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Mennill D, Doucet S, Montgomerie R, Ratcliffe L (2003) Achromatic color variation in black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapilla: black and white signals of sex and rank. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 53: 350–357. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Cuthill IC, Bennett ATD, Partridge JC, Maier EJ (1999) Plumage Reflectance and the Objective Assessment of Avian Sexual Dichromatism. The American Naturalist 153: 183–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Huyghe K, Vanhooydonck B, Herrel A, Tadić Z, Van Damme R (2012) Female lizards ignore the sweet scent of success: Male characteristics implicated in female mate preference. Zoology 115: 217–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Hamilton PS, Sullivan BK (2005) Female mate attraction in ornate tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus: a multivariate analysis. Animal Behaviour 69: 219–224. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. López P, Amo L, Martín J (2006) Reliable signaling by chemical cues of male traits and health state in male lizards, Lacerta monticola . Journal of Chemical Ecology 32: 473–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Lewontin RC (1966) On the Measurement of Relative Variability. Systematic Zoology 15: 141–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.R Development Core Team (2012) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria.
  • 48. Manson JH (1994) Male aggression: a cost of female mate choice in Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques. Animal Behaviour 48: 473–475. [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Parker G (1979) Sexual selection and sexual conflict. Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects 123: 166. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Qvarnström A, Forsgren E (1998) Should females prefer dominant males? Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13: 498–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Ward G, Fitzgerald GJ (1987) Male aggression and female mate choice in the threespine stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus L. Journal of Fish Biology. 30: 679–690. [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Ophir AG, Galef BG (2003) Female Japanese quail that ‘eavesdrop’on fighting males prefer losers to winners. Animal Behaviour 66: 399–407. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Knapp R, Hews DK, Thompson CW, Ray LE, Moore MC (2003) Environmental and endocrine correlates of tactic switching by nonterritorial male tree lizards (Urosaurus ornatus). Hormones and Behavior 43: 83–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Villaverde GA, Zucker N (1998) Sperm Storage Resulting in Viable Offspring in the Tree Lizard Urosaurus ornatus (Sauria: Phrynosomatidae). The Southwestern Naturalist 43: 92–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Sinervo B, Bleay C, Adamopoulou C (2001) Social causes of correlational selection and the resolution of a heritable throat color polymorphism in a lizard. Evolution 55: 2040–2052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Calsbeek R, Sinervo B (2002) The Ontogeny of Territoriality during Maturation. Oecologia 132: 468–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Aragón P, López P, Martín J (2001) Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: implications of field spatial relationships between males. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50: 128–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Martín J, López P (2012) Supplementation of Male Pheromone on Rock Substrates Attracts Female Rock Lizards to the Territories of Males: A Field Experiment. PLoS ONE 7: e30108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Pellitteri-Rosa D, Martín J, López P, Bellati A, Sacchi R, et al. (2014) Chemical polymorphism in male femoral gland secretions matches polymorphic coloration in common wall lizards (Podarcis muralis). Chemoecology 24: 67–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Martín J, López P (2006) Links between male quality, male chemical signals, and female mate choice in Iberian rock lizards. Functional Ecology 20: 1087–1096. [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Hunt J, Breuker CJ, Sadowski JA, Moore AJ (2009) Male-male competition, female mate choice and their interaction: determining total sexual selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 22: 13–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Fretwell SD (1972) Populations in a seasonal environment: Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure S1

Adult male (A) and female (B) Urosaurus ornatus color morphs. In U. ornatus, both sexes exhibit a polymorphism in dewlap color which is fixed at maturity. Individual males also exhibit variation in the size of their blue ventral patch, but these size differences are not fixed to different morphs (P>0.2, see Results). Female U. ornatus do not express this patch.

(TIF)

Figure S2

Percent reflectance of male Urosaurus ornatus dewlap and ventral patches. For dewlap patches (A), lines are colored by male morph: blue, orange, or yellow. For ventral patches (B), lines are shaded by male morph: blue (n = 16, black line), orange (n = 8, dark grey line), or yellow (n = 5, light grey line). Male U. ornatus lizards included in this figure were captured at the same study site as males used in the current study. The spectral range shown in both graphs includes ultraviolet (300–399 nm, grey shaded region) and visible light (400–700 nm). Values used to construct these graphs are mean ±1.0 standard error (SE) percent reflectance at 10-nm intervals.

(TIFF)

Raw Data S1

Female preference experiment, male morphology, and Figure S2 databases.

(XLSX)


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES