Abstract
Objectives. We examined the coevolution of adolescent friendships and peer influences with respect to their risk behaviors and social networking site use.
Methods. Investigators of the Social Network Study collected longitudinal data during fall 2010 and spring 2011 from 10th-grade students in 5 Southern California high schools (n = 1434). We used meta-analyses of stochastic actor-based models to estimate changes in friendship ties and risk behaviors and the effects of Facebook and MySpace use.
Results. Significant shifts in adolescent smoking and drinking occurred despite little change in overall prevalence rates. Students with higher levels of alcohol use were more likely to send and receive friendship nominations and become friends with other drinkers. They were also more likely to increase alcohol use if their friends drank more. Adolescents selected friends with similar Facebook and MySpace use habits. Exposure to friends’ risky online pictures increased smoking behaviors but had no significant effects on alcohol use.
Conclusions. Our findings support a greater focus on friendship selection mechanisms in school-based alcohol use interventions. Social media platforms may help identify at-risk adolescent groups and foster positive norms about risk behaviors.
It is well established that adolescents’ risk behaviors, such as smoking and alcohol use, are associated with their friends’ behaviors.1–6 Friendships have also been shown to be the most salient relationship through which these behavioral influences are transmitted, especially during adolescence,7 because of an increase in shared activities8 and opportunities for socialization.9 Furthermore, the desire for peer affirmation, social status, and group membership10,11 make adolescents particularly susceptible to normative influences.12,13 These influence processes play a significant role in the adoption of risk behaviors such as cigarette smoking and substance use.
PEER SELECTION AND INFLUENCE MECHANISMS
Although the association between adolescents’ behaviors and their friends’ behaviors has long been attributed to peer influence mechanisms between friends,3 an increasing body of research suggests that adolescents are also likely to form friendships around similarities in established risk behaviors. Social network methods have been used to distinguish between the effects of friendship selection and influence (socialization), but with mixed findings across smoking and alcohol use behaviors as well as across developmental stages.2,14,15 Among studies that show both mechanisms at work, many detect greater effects of selection,16–19 but others find that selection and influence both contribute to the association between friendships and risk behaviors20–23 (Appendix A, available as a supplement to this article at http://www.ajph.org).
Two social network principles provide the framework for selection and influence. Homophily is the tendency for similar people to be drawn to each other.24 Adolescents who engage in risk behaviors are likely to be attracted to each other, and these risk behaviors are consequently reinforced as a result of shared time and activities. Adolescents’ self-concept may hinge on the social groups to which they belong, so they alter their behaviors to conform to appropriate group- and self-defined norms.25 Socialization is the process by which information or ideas are diffused through a network by contact or communication.26 The spread of behavior may be driven by a centrally positioned individual or by the prevalence of risk behaviors and associated norms defined by peer environments.27 Influence processes may also be triggered by direct peer pressures or internal cognitive perceptions about the norms and risks associated with the behaviors.
ADOLESCENT ONLINE SOCIAL NETWORKING
Online social networking sites (SNSs) have increased the potential channels through which peer selection and influences take place. In the United States, SNSs are used by approximately 80% of adolescents (aged 12–17 years) and have gradually become the preferred means of communication across all racial and socioeconomic groups.28,29
Adolescents frequent popular SNSs to form and build friendships, foster their sense of social connectedness,30 and manage their self-presentations, according to their current interests.31,32 In accordance with social learning mechanisms through mass media channels, friendships that extend into the virtual context may foster similar but compounded mutual influences on behaviors through online chatting and the exchange of digital content. Online interactions may accelerate peer influence processes between friends because of their ubiquity and ability to cross boundaries of time and space. Broader influences may similarly originate from acquaintances or popular media figures who model behaviors that adolescents are likely to emulate.
There is growing concern that SNSs may also serve as conduits of risk behaviors in instances when risky content is discussed or showcased by friends online.33,34 Adolescents may exhibit fewer inhibitions in their display of risky behaviors such as smoking or alcohol use in an online context because the repercussions that come with face-to-face contact are minimized. Exposures to unfavorable media sources35 or risky online encounters34,36,37 may also distort perceptions of risk.38
Emerging evidence points to an association between adolescent social media use and risk behaviors,33 but the mechanisms of influence are unclear. SNSs are also characterized by distinct user traits,33 which provide a space for adolescents to commune with others who are similar to themselves. Studies that have used SNSs for health interventions have found modest results but increasingly suggest that social media platforms can be effective in facilitating new norms that encourage or discourage risk taking.39
We examined, in the context of face-to-face friendships in a school setting, whether selection—by similarity in smoking and alcohol use—had greater impact on behavior than did peer influence. Secondly, we investigated possible mechanisms leading to an association between adolescents’ online activity and their risk behaviors: (1) Did similarity in SNS use or exposure affect friendship formation and maintenance among students in the same grade (selection)? (2) Did exposure to friends’ risk behaviors online increase the likelihood of adolescent risk behaviors (influence)? Findings will shed light on how social media may alter selection and influence mechanisms and inform the design of social media interventions.
METHODS
Data were from the first 2 waves of the Social Network Study,40 a longitudinal network study of 5 Southern California high schools. We administered paper-and-pencil surveys during class on a regular school day in October 2010 and in May 2011. Of the total 2290 enrolled 10th-grade students, 2016 returned valid parental consent forms (88.0%), with 1823 allowing participation in the study; 28 of these students did not assent. The eligible pool was 1795 students, of whom 1719 completed surveys at time 1 and 1620 at time 2.
We collected sociometric (saturated) network data at the grade level and egocentric (personal) network data at the community level. For grade-level networks, we asked students to nominate up to 19 best friends, identified by ID numbers on a photo roster of all students in their grade.40 We constructed egocentric networks by asking students to “name seven best friends regardless of where they live or go to school.” Of the total egocentric nominations that specified first and last names, we matched 54% to students in respondents’ grade.
Measures
To model selection effects, we specified the network as the dependent variable. We conducted analyses on the 5 friendship networks across the 5 schools. Of the 1795 eligible participants in the analytic sample, 1434 (79.9%) 10th graders provided friendship nominations at both time points.
To model peer influence effects, we specified students’ self-reported tobacco and alcohol use as the dependent behavioral variables. We derived a composite smoking score (1 = not susceptible, 2 = susceptible, 3 = ever smoker, 4 = past-month smoker, 5 = daily smoker) from responses to 5 questions on smoking frequency and intention. We deemed the composite ordinal items appropriate for Siena modeling.41 We coded a response of “definitely not” to the first question, “At any time in the next year do you think you will smoke a cigarette?” as not susceptible and all others as susceptible. We defined ever smokers as all students who gave any response other than never having smoked to the questions “How old were you when you first smoked a whole cigarette?” and “Have you ever tried cigarette smoking, even one or two puffs?” For the last 2 questions, “During the past 30 days, on how many days did you smoke?” and “Have you ever smoked cigarettes daily?” we coded responses other than zero days or never smoked as past-month and daily smokers, respectively.
Similarly, we coded alcohol use as a composite score (1= not susceptible, 2 = susceptible, 3 = ever drinker, 4 = past-month drinker, 5 = past-month binge drinker) derived from these items: 12-month drinking intention, age at first drink of alcohol except for religious purposes, number of days having at least 1 drink of alcohol during the past 30 days, and number of days having 5 or more drinks of alcohol in a row during the past 30 days.
We tested 4 social media use variables as covariates in the model. Two items captured the frequency of visits to the SNSs Facebook and MySpace in the past month (1 = never, 2 = rarely [once a month or less], 3 = occasionally [once a week or less], 4 = frequently [once every 2–3 days], and 5 = very frequently [once a day or more]). Two items derived from the students’ egocentric networks captured online risk exposures across SNS sites. These items asked respondents to indicate whether their nominated friends ever (1) posted pictures of themselves partying or drinking alcohol online and (2) talked about partying online. We separately tallied the total number of friends for both items as an indicator for online risk exposure (range = 0–7). Demographic covariates were students’ age, gender, ethnicity (1 = Latino or Hispanic ethnic origin, 2 = other), academic performance, parental smoking, and parental alcohol use.
Analyses
We estimated stochastic actor-oriented models (SAOMs) with RSiena (Simulation Investigation for Empirical Network Analysis) version 4.0,41 a software package for estimating longitudinal coevolution models of social networks,42 offered through the open-source statistical system R.43
The primary advantage of using SAOMs over previous methods for longitudinal network analysis is the ability to simultaneously estimate network and behavioral dynamics while accounting for endogenous network tendencies. These may include the tendencies for friends to nominate each other as friends (reciprocity), the tendency for friends of friends to become friends (transitivity), or the tendency for actors with similar attributes to become friends (homophily).42,44 Individuals within a bounded network are actors, who may change their ties in response to behaviors and attributes of others around them (i.e., selection) or change their behavior because of their current network ties (i.e., influence).
Three main assumptions distinguish the SAOM from conventional methods for estimating individual-level changes within a network.45 First, changes between measurement points are modeled according to a continuous-time Markov process to simulate likely unobserved developmental trajectories between the measurement time points.46 Second, each actor is assumed to independently make decisions about changes in friendship ties or behaviors, without conspiring with others about these decisions. Finally, actors take microsteps of change, 1 network tie or 1 level of behavior at a time, reducing the variation between assessments.
We tested the study aims in 2 stages. First we specified a risk behavior model to simultaneously estimate selection and influence of adolescent smoking and alcohol use. Because there is strong evidence for the co-occurrence of adolescent smoking and alcohol use,23,47 we included both as behavioral dependent variables to simultaneously control for the 2 behaviors. Our second social media model tested whether SNS use parameters added more explanatory power to the network and behavioral dynamics beyond those established in the first set of analyses.
Model Specification
Two main functions are used to govern network and behavior changes in both model estimations. The rate function represents the average number of opportunities for change in the network and behavior between discrete time points. The objective function provides parameters that guide the direction of these changes. We further specified 2 types of objective functions: the network objective function, to estimate the change in friendship ties based on adolescent attributes, and the behavior objective function, to estimate the change in behavior based on friendships and network structure.
We derived a set of a priori parameters (Table 1) from previous literature and theoretical justification, and further tested with a forward selection process that used Neyman–Rao score-type tests for interdependent effects of alternative models against a null model.44 We separately estimated the 2 models for each school, then combined the results in a meta-analysis to test the means and variances of all parameter estimates across schools.44 We used 2 types of tests41 to determine the significance of effects: (1) a likelihood method that used the t ratio (mean parameter estimate divided by its standard error) under iterative weighted least squares modification48 and (2) a Fisher-type combination test of 1-sided P values.49 We computed between-school differences with an approximate χ2 test of parameter variances by the Snijders–Baerveldt method.48
TABLE 1—
Effect | Description |
Network dynamics (selection) | |
Rate parameter—friendship | Average number of opportunities for tie changes over time |
Endogenous structural effects | |
Out-degree (density) | Tendency to form friendship with someone |
Reciprocity | Tendency to form friendship with someone who previously selected participant as friend |
Transitivity | Tendency to form friendship with a friend of a current friend |
3 cycles | Tendency for network closure between 3 friends |
Covariate effects (similarity) | |
Female, grades, Hispanic | Tendency to form friendship with someone of the same gender, with similar academic grades, and with someone who is of Hispanic/Latino descent |
Online risky pictures | Tendency to form friendship with someone who has similar exposures to friends’ online posts of risky pictures |
Online partying | Tendency to form friendship with someone who has similar exposures to friends' online discussions about partying |
Tendency to form friendship with someone who has similar Facebook use habits | |
MySpace | Tendency to form friendship with someone who has similar MySpace use habits |
Behavioral effectsa | |
Behavior alter (popularity) | Effect of behavior on tendency to receive ties |
Behavior ego (activity) | Effect of behavior on tendency to send ties out |
Similarity in risk behavior | Effect of 2 people selecting each other as friends on the basis of similarity in behavior |
Behavior dynamics (influence) | |
Rate parameter—behaviora | Average number of opportunities for changes in behavior over time |
Linear tendency | Linear distribution of behavior |
Quadratic tendency | Quadratic distribution of behavior |
Behavioral effects | |
Behavior alter average | Effect of the average of friends’ behaviors on participants' behavior |
Female | Effect of being female on behavior |
Grades | Effect of grades on behavior |
Parental smoking/alcohol use | Effect of parental smoking or alcohol use on behavior |
Hispanic/Latino | Effect of being Hispanic/Latino on behavior |
Friends post risky pictures online | Effect of friends’ online posts of risky pictures on behavior |
Friends talk about partying online | Effect of friends’ online discussions about partying on behavior |
Effect of Facebook use on behavior | |
MySpace | Effect of MySpace use on behavior |
Current risk behavior | Effect of other risk behavior (smoking or alcohol use) on behavior |
Behavior parameters refer to either smoking or alcohol use.
RESULTS
Network characteristics and risk behaviors of the 5 high schools are presented in Table 2. (School-level descriptive statistics in Appendix B, available as a supplement to this article at http://www.ajph.org.) Students were evenly distributed by gender, and their mean age was 15 years. Average self-reported academic grades were “mostly C’s,” and about two thirds reported being Hispanic–Latino. Students nominated on average 5.17 school friends at time 1 and 5.23 at time 2. The average network densities remained similar but fluctuated slightly within schools. Overall network centralization and reciprocity decreased slightly over time, suggesting that peers formed new friendships with more peripheral members in their grade. Jaccard indices, calculated as the ratio of stable ties divided by the total number of ties across both times, ranged from 0.28 to 0.34. Indices above 0.2 indicated that the number of stable ties was sufficient to accurately estimate effects.42 Tobacco and alcohol use remained steady over time, suggesting that approximately equal numbers of students increased and decreased their risk behaviors. Students reported that most parents did not smoke (70%) and that about half (51%) drank alcohol at least once a week.
TABLE 2—
Time 1, Fall 2010 |
Time 2, Spring 2011 |
|||
Characteristic | Total Sample (n = 1434), Mean | Individual Schools Mean, low/high | Total Sample (n = 1434), Mean | Individual Schools, Mean, low/high |
Network characteristics | ||||
No. | 287 | 215/371 | . . . | . . . |
Average degreea | 5.17 | 4.21/5.87 | 5.23 | 4.17/6.27 |
Densityb | 0.018 | 0.015/0.023 | 0.018 | 0.013/0.025 |
Centralizationc | 0.034 | 0.031/0.044 | 0.031 | 0.023/0.037 |
Reciprocityd | 0.260 | 0.233/0.284 | 0.254 | 0.229/0.299 |
Jaccard indexe | . . . | . . . | 0.302 | 0.28/0.34 |
Descriptive characteristics | ||||
Tobacco usef | 1.79 | 1.58/1.96 | 1.75 | 1.58/1.89 |
Not susceptible | 61.09 | 54.8/71.2 | 63.06 | 57.5/71.6 |
Susceptible | 8.79 | 6.9/12.1 | 7.50 | 5.9/11.3 |
Ever smoker | 21.33 | 14.1/26.2 | 21.48 | 16.3/26.1 |
Past-month smoker | 6.24 | 5.6/6.8 | 5.43 | 3.9/7.1 |
Daily smoker | 2.56 | 0.9/4.7 | 2.52 | 2.0/3.5 |
Alcohol usef | 2.62 | 2.18/2.95 | 2.55 | 2.18/2.87 |
Nonsusceptible | 36.93 | 26.6/50.7 | 39.29 | 31.5/51.5 |
Susceptible | 4.71 | 3.8/6.1 | 5.45 | 4.9/6.0 |
Ever drank | 30.94 | 26.6/33.4 | 31.49 | 27.7/34.0 |
Past-month drinker | 12.58 | 7.9/17.4 | 11.29 | 8.1/14.0 |
Daily drinker | 14.81 | 9.9/18.4 | 12.49 | 7.8/14.9 |
Femaleg | 0.51 | 0.48/0.54 | . . . | . . . |
Hispanicg | 0.66 | 0.42/0.86 | . . . | . . . |
Gradesh | 6.21 | 5.81/6.50 | . . . | . . . |
Parental smokingi | 1.37 | 1.34/1.40 | . . . | . . . |
Parental drinkingi | 1.63 | 1.53/1.69 | . . . | . . . |
Facebook usej | 2.81 | 2.21/3.53 | . . . | . . . |
MySpace usej | 2.70 | 2.11/3.09 | . . . | . . . |
Ego network friends | . . . | . . . | ||
Friends who posted risky pictures online,k no. | 0.36 | 0.25/0.53 | . . . | . . . |
Friends who posted talk about partying online,k no. | 0.77 | 0.43/1.04 | . . . | . . . |
Note. Ellipses indicate not applicable.
Number of nominations an actor received from others in the network.
Ratio between total number of ties and total number of possible ties in network.
Degree to which network ties were directed to 1 or a few actors in the network.
Proportion of ties that were reciprocated.
Ratio between repeated ties over total ties across 2 time points.
Range = 1–5.
Range = 0–1.
Range = 1 (mostly As)–9 (mostly Fs).
Range = 1–3.
Range = 1–5.
Range = 0–7.
Coevolution of School-Based Friendships and Risk Behaviors
Two sets of meta-analysis outcomes are presented for both models (Tables 3 and 4); however, effects derived from the Fisher 1-sided test (α/2 = 0.025) are described because the study sample represented the entire school district under study.41 We assessed parameter variances between schools with the χ2 test and 4 degrees of freedom (N–1). With the exceptions of the endogenous structural parameters and similarity in Hispanic–Latino ethnicity, we observed few between-school differences. SAOM estimates derived with a dichotomous indicator of smoking and alcohol achieved nearly identical results (although 1 school did not converge).
TABLE 3—
Snijders–Baerveldt Method |
Fisher’s Combination 1-Sided Test (df = 10) |
|||||
Variable | B (SE) | Between-School Differencea | χ2 (Left Side) | P | χ2 (Right Side) | P |
Network dynamics | ||||||
Rate friendship | 13.17*** (1.039) | 2.32*** | 0 | > .999 | 3051.50 | < .001 |
Out-degree (density) | −2.66*** (0.048) | 0.11*** | 3469.06 | < .001 | 0 | > .999 |
Reciprocity | 1.85*** (0.083) | 0.19*** | 0 | > .999 | 2704.18 | < .001 |
Transitive triplets | 0.49*** (0.036) | 0.08*** | 0 | > .999 | 1628.16 | < .001 |
3 cycles | -0.35*** (0.014) | 0.03 | 230.68 | < .001 | 0 | > .999 |
Female similarity | 0.42*** (0.023) | 0.05 | 0 | > .999 | 375.72 | < .001 |
Grades similarity | 0.44*** (0.053) | 0.12 | 0 | > .999 | 92.81 | < .001 |
Hispanic similarity | 0.19 (0.078) | 0.17*** | 2.21 | .994 | 129.28 | < .001 |
Smoking alter | 0.03 (0.015) | 0.03 | 3.74 | .958 | 16.17 | .095 |
Smoking ego | -0.02 (0.021) | 0.05 | 14.51 | .151 | 7.27 | .7 |
Smoking similarity | 0.08 (0.143) | 0.32* | 9.74 | .464 | 16.98 | .075 |
Drinking alter | 0.04* (0.013) | 0.03 | 0.98 | > .999 | 31.88 | < .001 |
Drinking ego | 0.03 (0.014) | 0.03 | 2.84 | .985 | 23.58 | .009 |
Drinking similarity | 0.39** (0.081) | 0.18 | 1.28 | .999 | 53.80 | < .001 |
Smoking behavior dynamics | ||||||
Rate smoking | 1.43*** (0.073) | 0.16 | 0 | > .999 | 218.93 | < .001 |
Linear shape | −1.08*** (0.082) | 0.18 | 227.33 | < .001 | 0 | > .999 |
Quadratic shape | 0.16** (0.024) | 0.05 | 0.75 | > .999 | 37.92 | < .001 |
Average alter smoking | 0.03 (0.164) | 0.37 | 6.81 | .743 | 11.76 | .301 |
Female | -0.25* (0.076) | 0.17 | 19.57 | .034 | 2.55 | .99 |
Grades | -0.06 (0.022) | 0.05 | 16.50 | .086 | 3.30 | .973 |
Parental smoking | 0.22 (0.091) | 0.20 | 2.97 | .982 | 22.76 | .012 |
Hispanic | -0.07 (0.160) | 0.36 | 10.06 | .436 | 9.17 | .516 |
Current drinking | 0.18* (0.053) | 0.12 | 1.97 | .997 | 32.04 | < .001 |
Drinking behavior dynamics | ||||||
Rate drinking | 2.24*** (0.125) | 0.28 | 0 | > .999 | 272.88 | < .001 |
Linear shape | -0.32** (0.054) | 0.12 | 107.97 | < .001 | 0.02 | > .999 |
Quadratic shape | 0.09* (0.028) | 0.06 | 3.73 | .959 | 31.53 | < .001 |
Average alter drinking | 0.19 (0.084) | 0.18 | 2.76 | .987 | 18.37 | .049 |
Female | 0.09 (0.105) | 0.24 | 7.34 | .693 | 17.99 | .055 |
Grades | < −0.01 (0.016) | 0.04 | 8.24 | .606 | 8.70 | .561 |
Parental drinking | 0.13 (0.060) | 0.13 | 2.87 | .984 | 23.78 | .008 |
Hispanic | < −0.01 (0.071) | 0.16 | 7.74 | .654 | 9.17 | .516 |
Current smoking | 0.17* (0.054) | 0.12 | 1.22 | > .999 | 42.27 | < .001 |
Significance derived from approximate χ2 test (df = 4).
*P < .05; **P < .01; ***P < .001.
TABLE 4—
Snijders–Baerveldt Method |
Fisher’s Combination 1-Sided Test (df = 10) |
|||||
Variable | B (SE) | Between-School Differencea | χ2 (Left Side) | P | χ2 (Right Side) | P |
Network dynamics | ||||||
Rate friendship | 13.19*** (1.048) | 2.34*** | 0 | > .999 | 2676.61 | < .001 |
Out-degree (density) | −2.67*** (0.050) | 0.11*** | 31786.19 | < .001 | 0 | > .999 |
Reciprocity | 1.84*** (0.083) | 0.19*** | 0 | > .999 | 2669.08 | < .001 |
Transitive triplets | 0.48*** (0.037) | 0.08*** | 0 | > .999 | 1433.43 | < .001 |
3 cycles | -0.35*** (0.013) | 0.03 | 210.64 | .001 | 0 | > .999 |
Female similarity | 0.42*** (0.022) | 0.05 | 0 | > .999 | 393.15 | < .001 |
Grades similarity | 0.42** (0.056) | 0.12 | 0.01 | > .999 | 83.53 | < .001 |
Hispanic similarity | 0.17 (0.076) | 0.17*** | 2.33 | .993 | 109.47 | < .001 |
Online risk picture similarity | 0.06*** (0.007) | 0.01 | 1.60 | .999 | 20.54 | .033 |
Online party similarity | 0.01 (0.026) | 0.06 | 9.10 | .523 | 12.38 | .261 |
Facebook similarity | 0.13** (0.019) | 0.04 | 0.41 | > .999 | 40.53 | < .001 |
MySpace similarity | 0.20** (0.034) | 0.08 | 0.18 | > .999 | 52.82 | < .001 |
Smoking alter | 0.02 (0.015) | 0.03 | 3.96 | .949 | 15.58 | .112 |
Smoking ego | -0.02 (0.019) | 0.04 | 15.22 | .124 | 6.00 | .815 |
Smoking similarity | 0.06 (0.132) | 0.29 | 9.15 | .518 | 14.35 | .158 |
Drinking alter | 0.05* (0.014) | 0.03 | 0.89 | > .999 | 36.75 | < .001 |
Drinking ego | 0.04* (0.014) | 0.03 | 2.17 | .995 | 27.45 | .004 |
Drinking similarity | 0.35* (0.100) | 0.22 | 1.87 | .997 | 48.54 | < .001 |
Smoking behavior dynamics | ||||||
Rate smoking | 1.39*** (0.059) | 0.13 | 0 | > .999 | 272.48 | < .001 |
Linear shape | −1.11*** (0.079) | 0.18 | 217.36 | .001 | 0 | > .999 |
Quadratic shape | 0.13** (0.022) | 0.05 | 1.17 | > .999 | 26.62 | .003 |
Average alter smoking | 0.04 (0.204) | 0.46 | 6.83 | .741 | 11.16 | .345 |
Female | -0.33* (0.120) | 0.27 | 24.85 | .006 | 2.33 | .993 |
Grades | -0.07* (0.019) | 0.04 | 16.91 | .076 | 2.71 | .987 |
Parental smoking | 0.24 (0.104) | 0.23 | 3.41 | .97 | 23.99 | .008 |
Hispanic | 0.04 (0.216) | 0.48 | 8.54 | .576 | 11.61 | .312 |
Online risky pictures | 0.43 (0.210) | 0.47 | 3.53 | .966 | 26.03 | .004 |
Online party | -0.01 (0.213) | 0.48* | 12.04 | .283 | 13.50 | .197 |
0.02 (0.044) | 0.10 | 7.47 | .68 | 12.68 | .242 | |
MySpace | -0.02 (0.073) | 0.16 | 13.53 | .196 | 10.36 | .41 |
Current drinking | 0.16* (0.056) | 0.13 | 2.69 | .988 | 23.34 | .01 |
Drinking behavior dynamics | ||||||
Rate drinking | 2.19*** (0.126) | 0.28 | 0 | > .999 | 283.82 | < .001 |
Linear shape | -0.34** (0.055) | 0.12 | 104.52 | .001 | 0.02 | > .999 |
Quadratic shape | 0.08 (0.034) | 0.08 | 4.26 | .935 | 31.37 | < .001 |
Average alter drinking | 0.18 (0.086) | 0.19 | 3.12 | .978 | 18.00 | .055 |
Female | 0.06 (0.102) | 0.23 | 7.73 | .655 | 15.41 | .118 |
Grades | < 0.01 (0.010) | 0.02 | 6.92 | .733 | 8.03 | .626 |
Parental drinking | 0.13 (0.065) | 0.15 | 3.04 | .981 | 25.53 | .004 |
Hispanic | 0.02 (0.072) | 0.16 | 6.37 | .783 | 10.44 | .403 |
Online risky pictures | -0.05 (0.072) | 0.16 | 10.68 | .383 | 6.09 | .807 |
Online party | 0.11 (0.068) | 0.15 | 4.95 | .895 | 14.41 | .155 |
0.02 (0.023) | 0.05 | 4.98 | .892 | 14.96 | .134 | |
MySpace | 0.02 (0.039) | 0.09 | 8.88 | .544 | 14.67 | .145 |
Current smoking | 0.16* (0.057) | 0.13 | 1.36 | .999 | 38.09 | < .001 |
Significance derived from approximate χ2 test (df = 4).
*P < .05; **P < .01; ***P < .001.
Covariate effects.
In the risk behavior model (Table 3), parameter estimates of the 4 endogenous structural effects were statistically significant (P < .001), indicating that best friends were likely to reciprocate friendships (reciprocity), become friends with someone who was a friend of an existing friend (transitivity), and form network closure with triadic friendship ties (3 cycles). Because we limited the number of nominations to 19 in the survey, the network density was significantly less than 50% (negative out-degree). Of the actor covariates, students who were the same gender, both Hispanic–Latino, and similar in academic achievement were likely to form new friendships and maintain their existing ones over time (P < .001).
Among the covariates used to predict drinking and smoking, being female was marginally protective against the increase in smoking risk, but academic achievement and being of Hispanic origin did not have effects on either risk behavior. Parental smoking and drinking significantly predicted higher levels of these risk behaviors at time 2 (P = .012 and P = .008, respectively). Furthermore, an adolescent’s engagement in either smoking or alcohol use at time 1 significantly predicted higher levels of the other risk behavior at time 2 (both P < .001), suggesting a significant degree of co-occurrence between the 2 risk behaviors.
Predictors of school-based friendship networks.
Students who had higher rates of smoking showed a marginal trend toward receiving more nominations (χ2 = 16.17; P = .095) but did not send out more ties. Students who were similar in smoking status had a tendency to form and maintain friendships (χ2 = 16.98; P = .075). Friendship selection and maintenance effects were stronger for alcohol use: students who reported more drinking had higher tendencies to both receive (χ2 = 31.88; P < .001) and send (χ2 = 23.58; P = .009) more nominations than those who reported low rates of drinking. Students who were similar in drinking status were also significantly more likely to form and maintain their friendships (χ2 = 53.80; P < .001).
Predictors of school-based smoking and alcohol risk.
Regarding behavior dynamics, we found no influence effects attributable to smoking friends, but adolescents with drinking friends had marginally higher tendencies to maintain or increase their drinking behaviors over the past 6 months (χ2 = 18.37; P = .049). Students were more likely to change their drinking status than their smoking status (2.24 vs 1.43 times). The negative linear shape effects indicated that the majority of students reported no use or low levels of tobacco and alcohol use. The significant positive quadratic shape effects indicated that risks for both smoking and drinking were self-reinforcing, suggesting that those who exhibited higher risk at time 1 were likely to continue the risk behavior at time 2 and that those who were abstinent were likely to remain abstinent. The associations between adolescent and friend risk behaviors were better explained by selection than by influence mechanisms.
Effects of Social Media Use
In the social media model (Table 4), we added the effects of SNS use and online exposures to risk behaviors. Effects estimated in the first model remained stable. Students who were similar in Facebook and MySpace use were likely to form or maintain ties (χ2 = 40.53 and χ2 = 52.82, respectively; both, P < .001). Friends who were similarly exposed to risky online postings by personal network friends also had a tendency to maintain or form new friendship ties (χ2 = 20.54; P = .033).
Regarding behavioral influence, frequency of Facebook and MySpace use did not significantly predict higher smoking and alcohol risk. Effects of exposure to risky pictures posted online by personal network friends was significantly predictive of adolescents’ likelihood of increasing or maintaining their smoking levels (χ2 = 26.03; P = .004); however, exposure to friends talking about partying online had no effect.
DISCUSSION
Consistent with some previous studies, selection effects, particularly for alcohol use, were stronger than influence effects among adolescents in our sample.17–20,23,50–52 Adolescent drinkers had stronger tendencies to send and receive friendship nominations from other 10th graders, which indicated that students who used alcohol were more outgoing and popular than those who did not. Students who had similar alcohol use habits were also more likely to select each other as friends. We observed peer influence effects only for alcohol use, suggesting that norms favoring alcohol use may have been more widespread among the adolescents in our sample and regarded as more social than smoking.50 The weaker selection effects associated with smoking may have resulted from the simultaneous modeling of both behaviors, diluting effects that have been found in previous single-behavior models. Statistically significant smoking selection effects confirmed this when we conducted SAOM meta-analyses independently from alcohol use (results not shown).
When we jointly considered structural and behavioral effects, a more detailed view of how these evolve in a secondary school emerged. Although the overall prevalence of smoking and drinking did not increase over 6 months, we saw significant shifts in behavior and friendships. In essence, behaviors coalesced: friends increased their reciprocity and became friends of existing friends. Likewise, friendship evolved: behaviors created attractors for network dynamics, partitioning friendship groups into those who embraced the behavior and those who did not.
We also shed light on potential selection and influence mechanisms related to adolescents’ use of SNSs. Friendships were more likely to exist between students who had similar Facebook and MySpace use habits and between students similarly exposed to their friends’ online displays of drinking and partying. If exposures indicate higher acceptance of risk behavior norms, the results may suggest an affinity between students with similar normative perceptions of these risk behaviors. Adolescents are drawn toward specific SNSs because of their demographic or other behavioral characteristics,33 so the selection and maintenance of friends might also be conceptualized as the result of similarity in these domains.
Although use of Facebook and MySpace did not affect the tendencies for adolescents to smoke or use alcohol, adolescents who were exposed to their friends’ risky online displays reported an increase in smoking at time 2. This supports previous findings that SNS use alone does not pose risks, but rather it is the content exchanged through SNSs that triggers behavioral influences.33 Pictures posted on SNSs, which are readily transmitted, may compound biased normative beliefs among friends connected through online social networks. This is especially pertinent for close friends who also interact daily in school. Through vicarious learning mechanisms,53 online social media content may exert greater influences than traditional media channels as media figures are replace by actual friends.
SNS influences from friends outside of the 10th grade who had higher perceived rates of smoking and drinking (not presented) and who represented almost half of the egocentric nominations, might also have been a source of peer influence. To test this possibility, we added to the SAOM models an indicator derived from the ratio of nominations of egocentric friends who were not grade-level friends to total egocentric friends and found it to be a significant predictor of network change for both risk behavior and SNS models (χ2 = 21.13 and χ2 = 22.16, respectively; both P = .01), while behavior was not affected.
We observed a stronger association of online risk exposures with smoking than with alcohol use. Online mechanisms may be a more comfortable environment for adolescents to exhibit smoking behaviors, which are less prevalent and less normative than alcohol use. In the wake of increasing prevalence and expanded utility of SNSs among adolescents, these findings provide timely evidence that peer influence effects may be transmitted through online displays of risk behavior.
Limitations
We drew our study sample from 1 Southern California high school district; findings may not be generalizable to the wider US adolescent population. Results from other districts may vary by prevalence of alcohol and tobacco consumption as well as the cultural mores of social media use. Although egocentric friend influences extended beyond the influences assessed within participants’ grade, for which we modeled selection and influence mechanisms, the use of self-reported attributes of egocentric friends might not have fully captured these adolescents’ online exposures. Nevertheless, the self-reported data of friends’ displays from our respondents can be considered a proximal source of influence, by contrast with exposures through general Web browsing activity. Future studies might consider including direct measures of online risk activity to more accurately estimate selection and influence effects attributable to network exposures.
We restricted sociometric friendship nominations to students in the same grade, which therefore might not have accounted for all possible sources of selection and influence effects on the 2 risk behaviors under study. However, participants could nominate up to 19 grade-level friends, so our results provide a reliable representation of grade-level peer influences.
We collectively modeled the change and maintenance of friendship ties, and thus our analyses did not distinguish between newly created, dissolved, and maintained ties over time. Recent studies that have found these differential selection effects in relation to smoking and alcohol use54,55 suggest that further investigation of these mechanisms and their associations with both risk behaviors and SNS experiences is warranted.
Implications
Despite the limitations, our study provides further evidence of risk-related selection mechanisms in school-based peer networks, and these deserve more attention in the design and implementation of school-based smoking and alcohol use prevention interventions. Interventions that incorporate SNSs or similar platforms may enhance peer selection and influence processes through higher levels of engagement, extended contact, and accelerated diffusion of positive social norms. SAOMs can be an effective way to examine the use patterns of existing SNSs such as Facebook and MySpace and to determine whether SNS activity might be indicative of risk-related social norms.
Future studies might incorporate more precise measurements of online risk exposure to better inform the mechanisms of online social influences. Use of SNSs or other online health applications could be modeled as a behavioral dependent variable to assess direct effects of online participation on risk behaviors.50 SAOMs can furthermore inform the efficacy of SNS-based interventions by examining whether users of specific social media applications are perceived as popular, whether users are effectively reaching out to others through the applications, and whether direct associations exist between intervention use and health outcomes.
Continued application of the SAOM is necessary to test potential social and environmental factors that may contribute to selection and influence processes. It could be useful to investigate the effects of SNS use and friendship reciprocity52 on degree of mutual influence as well as the differential effects of smoking and alcohol use on the creation, dissolution, and maintenance of friendships over time. Future studies might also focus on cross-behavior influences of selection and influence effects to uncover mechanisms that drive the simultaneous progression of alcohol and smoking35 among high-risk groups. Just as a calm sea masks the dramatic activities that occur below it, static rates of given behaviors can disguise the considerable dynamic influence and selection activities that adolescents engage in over time.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (grant 1RC1AA019239-01 to T. W. V.) and the National Cancer Institute Ruth L. Kirschstein NRSA award (T32 CA- 009492-28).
An earlier iteration of this study was presented at the Society for Prevention Research 20th Annual Meeting, May 29–June 1, 2012; Washington, DC.
We thank the El Monte Union High School District in Los Angeles County for their assistance.
Human Participant Projection
All study protocols were approved by the institutional review board of the University of Southern California.
References
- 1.Alexander C, Piazza M, Mekos D, Valente T. Peers, schools, and adolescent cigarette smoking. J Adolesc Health. 2001;29(1):22–30. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(01)00210-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hoffman BR, Monge PR, Chou CP, Valente TW. Perceived peer influence and peer selection on adolescent smoking. Addict Behav. 2007;32(8):1546–1554. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kobus K. Peers and adolescent smoking. Addiction. 2003;98(suppl 1):37–55. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.98.s1.4.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Nash SG, McQueen A, Bray JH. Pathways to adolescent alcohol use: family environment, peer influence, and parental expectations. J Adolesc Health. 2005;37(1):19–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Trucco EM, Colder CR, Wieczorek WF. Vulnerability to peer influence: a moderated mediation study of early adolescent alcohol use initiation. Addict Behav. 2011;36(7):729–736. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ennett ST, Faris R, Hipp J et al. Peer smoking, other peer attributes, and adolescent cigarette smoking: a social network analysis. Prev Sci. 2008;9(2):88–98. doi: 10.1007/s11121-008-0087-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fujimoto K, Valente TW. Social network influences on adolescent substance use: disentangling structural equivalence from cohesion. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(12):1952–1960. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fujimoto K, Valente TW. Alcohol peer influence of participating in organized school activities: a network approach. Health Psychol. 2013;32(10):1084–1092. doi: 10.1037/a0029466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Steinberg L, Monahan KC. Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Dev Psychol. 2007;43(6):1531–1543. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cleveland MJ, Feinberg ME, Bontempo DE, Greenberg MT. The role of risk and protective factors in substance use across adolescence. J Adolesc Health. 2008;43(2):157–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Oetting ER, Donnermeyer JF. Primary socialization theory: the etiology of drug use and deviance. I. Subst Use Misuse. 1998;33(4):995–1026. doi: 10.3109/10826089809056252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Simons-Morton BG, Farhat T. Recent findings on peer group influences on adolescent smoking. J Prim Prev. 2010;31(4):191–208. doi: 10.1007/s10935-010-0220-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Urberg KA, Degirmencioglu SM, Pilgrim C. Close friend and group influence on adolescent cigarette smoking and alcohol use. Dev Psychol. 1997;33(5):834–844. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.5.834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Burk WJ, van der Vorst H, Kerr M, Stattin H. Alcohol use and friendship dynamics: selection and socialization in early-, middle-, and late-adolescent peer networks. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73(1):89–98. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mercken L, Steglich C, Knibbe R, de Vries H. Dynamics of friendship networks and alcohol use in early and mid-adolescence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73(1):99–110. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mundt M. Social network analysis of peer effects on binge drinking among U.S. adolescents. In: Greenberg A, Kennedy W, Bos N, editors. Social Computing, Behavioral-Cultural Modeling and Prediction. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 2013. pp. 123–134. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Knecht AB, Burk WJ, Weesie J, Steglich C. Friendship and alcohol use in early adolescence: a multilevel social network approach. J Res Adolesc. 2011;21(2):475–487. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Mercken L, Steglich C, Sinclair P, Holliday J, Moore L. A longitudinal social network analysis of peer influence, peer selection, and smoking behavior among adolescents in British schools. Health Psychol. 2012;31(4):450–459. doi: 10.1037/a0026876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mercken L, Snijders TAB, Steglich C, Vartiainen E, de Vries H. Dynamics of adolescent friendship networks and smoking behavior. Soc Networks. 2010;32(1):72–81. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Steglich C, Snijders TAB, West P. Applying SIENA: an illustrative analysis of the coevolution of adolescents’ friendship networks, taste in music, and alcohol consumption. Methodology. 2006;2(1):48–56. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Green HD, Jr, Horta M, de la Haye K, Tucker JS, Kennedy DR, Pollard M. Peer influence and selection processes in adolescent smoking behavior: a comparative study. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15(2):534–541. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Schaefer DR, Haas SA, Bishop NJ. A dynamic model of US adolescents’ smoking and friendship networks. Am J Public Health. 2012;102(6):e12–e18. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kiuru N, Burk WJ, Laursen B, Salmela-Aro K, Nurmi JE. Pressure to drink but not to smoke: disentangling selection and socialization in adolescent peer networks and peer groups. J Adolesc. 2010;33(6):801–812. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM. Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Annu Rev Sociol. 2001;27:415–444. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hogg MA, Abrams D, Otten S, Hinkle S. The social identity perspective. Small Group Res. 2004;35(3):246–276. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Valente TW. Social Networks and Health: Models, Methods, and Applications. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Valente TW, Ritt-Olson A, Stacy A, Unger JB, Okamoto J, Sussman S. Peer acceleration: effects of a social network tailored substance abuse prevention program among high-risk adolescents. Addiction. 2007;102(11):1804–1815. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01992.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lenhart A, Madden M, Smith A, Purcell K, Zickuhr K, Rainie L. Teens, kindness and cruelty on social network sites. Pew Internet and American Life Project. Available at: http://www.pewinternet.org/files/old-media/Files/Reports/2011/PIP_Teens_Kindness_Cruelty_SNS_Report_Nov_2011_FINAL_110711.pdf. Accessed April 1, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lenhart A. Teens, smartphones and texting. Pew Internet and American Life Project. Available at: http://www.pewinternet.org/files/old-media//Files/Reports/2012/PIP_Teens_Smartphones_and_Texting.pdf. Accessed April 1, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Grieve R, Indian M, Witteveen K, Tolan GA, Marrington J. Face-to-face or Facebook: can social connectedness be derived online? Comput Human Behav. 2013;29(3):604–609. [Google Scholar]
- 31.boyd d. Why youth [heart] social network sites: the role of networked publics in teenage social life. In: Buckingham D, editor. Youth, Identity, and Digital Media. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2007. pp. 119–142. John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Series on Digital Media and Learning. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Utz S. Show me your friends and I will tell you what type of person you are: how one’s profile, number of friends, and type of friends influence impression formation on social network sites. J Comput Mediat Commun. 2010;15(2):314–335. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Huang GC, Unger JB, Soto D et al. Peer influences: the impact of online and offline friendship networks on adolescent smoking and alcohol use. J Adolesc Health. 2014;54(5):508–514. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Moreno MA, Parks MR, Zimmerman FJ, Brito TE, Christakis DA. Display of health risk behaviors on MySpace by adolescents: prevalence and associations. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(1):27–34. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Leatherdale ST, Ahmed R. Alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco use among Canadian youth: do we need more multi-substance prevention programming? J Prim Prev. 2010;31(3):99–108. doi: 10.1007/s10935-010-0211-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Christofides E, Muise A, Desmarais S. Information disclosure and control on Facebook: are they two sides of the same coin or two different processes? Cyberpsychol Behav. 2009;12(3):341–345. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Festl R, Quandt T. Social relations and cyberbullying: the influence of individual and structural attributes on victimization and perpetration via the Internet. Hum Commun Res. 2013;39(1):101–126. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Young SD, Jordan AH. The influence of social networking photos on social norms and sexual health behaviors. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2013;16(4):243–247. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Chou WS, Prestin A, Lyons C, Wen K. Web 2.0 for health promotion: reviewing the current evidence. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(1):e9–e18. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Valente TW, Fujimoto K, Unger JB, Soto DW, Meeker D. Variations in network boundary and type: a study of adolescent peer influences. Soc Networks. 2013;35(3):309–316. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ripley RM, Snijders TAB, Boda Z, Voros A, Preciado P. Manual for RSiena. Oxford, UK: University of Oxford, Department of Statistics, Nuffield College; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Snijders TAB, Van de Bunt GG, Steglich CEG. Introduction to stochastic actor-based models for network dynamics. Soc Networks. 2010;32(1):44–60. [Google Scholar]
- 43.R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Snijders TAB, Steglich CEG, Schweinberger M. Modeling the co-evolution of networks and behavior. In: van Montfort K, Oud H, Satorra A, editors. Longitudinal Models in the Behavioral and Related Sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2007. pp. 41–71. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Steglich C, Snijders TAB, Pearson M. Dynamic networks and behavior: separating selection from influence. Sociol Methodol. 2010;40(1):329–393. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Norris JR. Markov Chains. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Mathys C, Burk WJ, Cillessen AHN. Popularity as a moderator of peer selection and socialization of adolescent alcohol, marijuana, and tobacco use. J Res Adolesc. 2013;23(3):513–523. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Snijders TAB, Baerveldt C. A multilevel network study of the effects of delinquent behavior on friendship evolution. J Math Sociol. 2003;27(2–3):123–151. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for Meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pearson M, Sweeting H, West P, Young R, Gordon J, Turner K. Adolescent substance use in different social and peer contexts: a social network analysis. Drugs. 2006;13(6):519–536. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pearson M, Steglich C, Snijders T. Homophily and assimilation among sport-active adolescent substance users. Connections. 2006;27(1):47–63. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mercken L, Candel M, Willems P, de Vries H. Disentangling social selection and social influence effects on adolescent smoking: the importance of reciprocity in friendships. Addiction. 2007;102(9):1483–1492. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01905.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bleakley A, Hennessy M, Fishbein M, Jordan A. Using the integrative model to explain how exposure to sexual media content influences adolescent sexual behavior. Health Educ Behav. 2011;38(5):530–540. doi: 10.1177/1090198110385775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Cheadle JE, Stevens M, Williams DT, Goosby BJ. The differential contributions of teen drinking homophily to new and existing friendships: an empirical assessment of assortative and proximity selection mechanisms. Soc Sci Res. 2013;42(5):1297–1310. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2013.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.DeLay D, Laursen B, Kiuru N, Salmela-Aro K, Nurmi JE. Selecting and retaining friends on the basis of cigarette smoking similarity. J Res Adolesc. 2013;23(3):464–473. [Google Scholar]