Abstract
In recent years there has been substantial support for heart rate variability biofeedback (HRVB) as a treatment for a variety of disorders and for performance enhancement (Gevirtz, 2013). Since conditions as widely varied as asthma and depression seem to respond to this form of cardiorespiratory feedback training, the issue of possible mechanisms becomes more salient. The most supported possible mechanism is the strengthening of homeostasis in the baroreceptor (Vaschillo et al., 2002; Lehrer et al., 2003). Recently, the effect on the vagal afferent pathway to the frontal cortical areas has been proposed. In this article, we review these and other possible mechanisms that might explain the positive effects of HRVB.
Keywords: heart rate variability, biofeedback, resonance, baroreflex, homeostasis
INTRODUCTION
In recent years there has been substantial support for heart rate variability biofeedback (HRVB) for a variety of disorders and for performance enhancement (Gevirtz, 2013). Since conditions as widely varied as asthma and irritable bowel syndrome seem to respond to this form of cardiorespiratory feedback training, the issue of possible mechanisms becomes more salient. The most supported possible mechanism is the strengthening of homeostasis in the baroreceptor (Vaschillo et al., 2002, 2006; Lehrer et al., 2003). Recently, the effect on the vagal afferent pathway to the frontal cortical areas has been proposed. In this article, we review these and other possible mechanisms that might explain the positive effects of HRVB.
In the 1990s Lehrer et al. (2000) began experimenting with a form of cardiorespiratory intervention that has subsequently been labeled HRVB, respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) biofeedback, or resonance frequency feedback (RFF). The procedure consists of feeding back beat by beat heart rate data during slow breathing maneuvers such that the participant tries to maximize RSA, create a sine-wave-like curve of peaks and valleys, and match RSA to heart rate patterns. RSA is the heart pattern that occurs when heart rate increases during inhalation and decreases during exhalation. Thus as can be seen in Figure 1, the participant uses feedback or a breath pacing device to produce the characteristic maximized RSA.
Gevirtz (2013) recently reviewed all of the available literature on the outcomes of HRVB. He looked at the following application categories: asthma, COPD, IBS, cyclic vomiting, recurrent abdominal pain, fibromyalgia, cardiac rehabilitation, hypertension, chronic muscle pain, pregnancy induced hypertension, depression, anxiety, PTSD, insomnia, and performance1. While few areas have extensive support by way of controlled studies, the overall picture seems to be very promising for this intervention. As can be seen, the applications are quite varied. We have begun to explore what physiological and/or psychological mechanisms might be contributing to these positive outcomes.
MECHANISMS BY WHICH HIGH-AMPLITUDES OF HRV ARE ACHIEVED DURING HRV BIOFEEDBACK
Heart rate variability (HRV) has a complex structure, often referred to as “chaotic,” involving various superimposed oscillation frequencies, non-linearly related to each other (Ivanov et al., 1999; Pikkujamsa et al., 1999). Some processes involved in this pattern are caused by known reflexes, some with modulatory functions, often controlled by different autonomic pathways. These can be described as “negative feedback loops,” which operate as closed loop system components that help maintain allostatic balance, while allowing adaptation to environmental demands (Lehrer and Eddie, 2013). During HRV biofeedback, the amplitude of heart rate oscillations grows to many times the amplitude at rest, while the pattern becomes simple and sinusoidal. This pattern occurs in almost everyone, and is often achievable within a fraction of a minute even in persons who have never previously been exposed to the technique. The mechanism for this effect lies in a confluence of processes: (1) phase relationships between heart rate oscillations and breathing at specific frequencies, (2) phase relationships between heart rate and blood pressure oscillations at specific frequencies, (3) activity of the baroreflex, and (4) resonance characteristics of the cardiovascular system.
PHASE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN HEART RATE OSCILLATIONS AND BREATHING
During normal breathing, one of the many oscillations in heart rate usually occurs at the same frequency as breathing. People often breathe at differing frequencies at different times, and various individuals tend to breathe at different rates. For most people, most of the time, breathing frequency is between 0.15 and 0.4 Hz, or 9 to 24 breaths per minute. The corresponding oscillations in heart rate are called RSA, which can be interpreted as influences of respiration on the sinoatrial node of the heart. The frequency range of 0.15–0.4 Hz is the “high frequency” (HF) band in the HRV spectrum, and spectral amplitude within this range is often used as an index of RSA (Berntson et al., 1997). However, although RSA is often driven by breathing, it also may be influenced by respiratory pacemaker oscillations in the central nervous system, which occasionally differ from actual breathing. These processes might be influenced by external factors (e.g., sudden exercise or stress, sighs, etc.), where both pacemaker and actual respiration may both produce heart rate oscillations. These can sometimes occur at different frequencies and with different patterns, as shown in dissociation between RSA and breathing during mechanical ventilation (Van de Louw et al., 2010), apnea (Passino et al., 1997), and paced breathing (Song and Lehrer, 2003). At resting respiratory rates, the phase relationship between breathing and HR is far from synchronous, such that heart rate increases tend to follow inhalation at about the mid-breath point, and heart rate decreases follow exhalation also at about the mid-breath point (Vaschillo et al., 2002).
Respiratory sinus arrhythmia is known to have important regulatory functions. It controls the rate of gas exchange at the alveoli, such that heart rate tends to be higher when air in the lung is richest in oxygen, and exhalation occurs when carbon dioxide in the lung is highest. It is notable, however, that the partial out-of-phase relationship between heart rate and breathing is not the most efficient pattern for gas exchange. Animal experiments by Hayano et al. (1996) in Japan have found that gas exchange at the alveoli is most efficient when heart rate starts increasing at the beginning of inhalation, and starts decreasing just as exhalation starts, i.e., a 0° phase relationship. In these studies, denervated dogs were artificially ventilated, and heart rate oscillations were entirely controlled by a cardiac pacemaker, such that the phase relationship between respiration and heart rate could be experimentally manipulated, in three phase relationships: 0, 90, and 180° (the last of these corresponding to a pattern where heart rate started increasing at the beginning of each exhalation, and started decreasing at the beginning of each exhalation). They measured gas exchange in the alveoli, and found that it was greatest at the 0° phase relationship, at an intermediate level at the 90° phase relationship, and lowest at the 180° phase relationship. Perhaps the function of a partially out-of-phase phase relationship is to allow the greatest degree of flexibility to the organism, such that greater efficiency can be achieved during greater metabolic need, and less during decreased need. Phase relationship studies at various levels of metabolic need have not yet been done, so this interpretation must remain speculative.
Respiratory sinus arrhythmia also can reflect aspects of autonomic function. It is controlled entirely by the vagus nerve, such that vagus nerve outputs to the sinoatrial node primarily occur only during exhalation. Greater vagus nerve traffic will therefore produce greater amplitudes of RSA, such that many scientists equate RSA (or HF HRV) with “cardiac vagal tone,” or parasympathetic influence on the heart (Berntson et al., 1997). However, longer exhalations (Strauss-Blasche et al., 2000) and slower respiration (Eckberg et al., 1985; Grossman et al., 1991; Song and Lehrer, 2003) also may increase RSA amplitude, possibly independently of vagus nerve traffic, since vagus nerve output occurs for relatively longer periods of time with each breath.
Indeed, it has long been known that amplitude of HRV is systematically related to breathing frequency, with higher amplitudes achievable with slower respiration (Eckberg et al., 1985; Brown et al., 1993; Badra et al., 2001; Eckberg, 2003; Song and Lehrer, 2003). However, most studies find that maximum effects usually are achieved when breathing at a rate of approximately 0.1 Hz (six breaths per minute). Working in St. Petersburg, Russia, Vaschillo systematically studied relationships between breathing and heart rate, using a “transfer function analysis,” whereby the interplay between two oscillations is studied at different frequencies – i.e., different respiration rates. The maximum heart rate oscillations at respiratory frequency occurred at approximately 0.1 Hz (six breaths per minute), the one frequency at which heart rate oscillates with breathing at a 0° phase relationship, i.e., exactly in phase. Thus breathing at this frequency produces both the highest amplitude of RSA and the most efficient gas exchange.
It should also be noted that respiratory-induced changes in HRV may function as a positive feedback loop, spiraling further increases in HRV, by feedback from the heart to the central nervous system, through the vagal afferent system, as described below.
These results also suggest that, where HRV biofeedback produces a 0° phase relationship between heart rate and breathing, conditions requiring better gas exchange efficiency could show improvement. Consistent with this, there is evidence for better athletic performance after training in HRV biofeedback (Strack, 2003; Shaw, 2011; Paul and Garg, 2012) and that HRV biofeedback may help improve breathing symptoms and quality of life among patients with emphysema (Giardino et al., 2004).
PHASE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN HEART RATE AND BLOOD PRESSURE
Vaschillo’s early studies also showed systematic changes in phase relationships between heart rate and blood pressure, when the system was stimulated at various frequencies. For each person, he found, there was a specific frequency where heart rate changes per unit change in blood pressure were greatest. This frequency varied from individual to individual, but was ∼0.1 Hz. (In later research we refined the average frequency for highest heart rate oscillations to be at about 0.09 Hz, or 5.5 breaths per minute, with breath duration of about 11 s; Vaschillo et al., 2002.) When he examined phase relationships between heart rate and blood pressure, he found that, at this frequency, heart rate and blood pressure oscillated in a 180° phase relationship: i.e., completely out of phase, such that blood pressure began falling as soon as heart rate began rising, and blood pressure began rising as soon as heart rate began falling. This phase relationship strongly suggested that the mechanism for the high-amplitude heart rate oscillations was the baroreflex.
THE BAROREFLEX
The baroreflex is a reflex mediated by blood pressure sensors in the aorta and carotid artery that help modulate blood pressure fluctuations (Eckberg and Sleight, 1992). Baroreceptors in the walls of these arteries detect stretching of the arteries as blood pressure increases. When blood pressure increases, the baroreflex causes immediate decreases in heart rate. As blood pressure falls, the baroreflex causes immediate increases in heart rate. Thus, when the system is stimulated at the specific frequency causing maximum heart rate oscillations and a 180° phase relationship between heart rate and blood pressure, effects of the stimulator are compounded by effects of the baroreflex. As external stimulation causes heart rate to rise, it also causes blood pressure to fall, thus causing an additional stimulus for heart rate to rise further; and as external stimulation causes heart rate to fall, it also causes blood pressure to rise, thus causing an additional stimulus for heart rate to fall further.
Because of the 0° phase relationship between heart rate and breathing at approximately the same frequency that external stimulation causes maximal stimulation to the baroreflex, breathing becomes a natural way to provide external stimulation to increase HRV. Conversely, each breath then stimulates the baroreflex. We have found large increases in baroreflex gain (number of beats per minute change in heart rate per 1 mm Hg change in blood pressure) during HRV biofeedback: i.e., the baroreflex operates more strongly (Lehrer et al., 2003). When HRV biofeedback is practiced twice daily at home over about a 3 month period, we also find increases in resting baroreflex gain (i.e., before people start practicing biofeedback in a given session; Lehrer et al., 2003). This demonstrated neuroplasticity in the baroreflex, and suggested that regular exercise of the reflex rendered it stronger. It also suggested that various conditions affected by blood pressure lability and baroreflex control may be affected by HRV biofeedback. Thus, there is a growing body of evidence that a course of HRV biofeedback can help hypertensive patients lower their blood pressures (Nolan et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2010; Lin et al., 2012).
Pathways of baroreflex neural control suggest other possible HRV biofeedback applications. The baroreflex is mediated through the nucleus tractus solitarius, located in the brain stem (Raven et al., 1997; Rogers et al., 2000; Polson et al., 2007; Arnold et al., 2009). This center communicates directly with the amygdala, a center for emotional control, in a pathway extending through the insula (Volz et al., 1990; Henderson et al., 2004). Perhaps it is for this reason that various studies have shown positive HRV biofeedback effects for treating anxiety and depression (Karavidas et al., 2007; Reiner, 2008; Siepmann et al., 2008; McCraty et al., 2009; Nada, 2009; Zucker et al., 2009; Henriques et al., 2011; Tan et al., 2011; Patron et al., 2013).
THE BAROREFLEX, HRV, AND RESILIENCE
There is a large amount of evidence that people are more resilient – physically and emotionally – when HRV oscillation amplitudes are higher and more complex. Greater complexity, as measured by various calculations of fractal entropy, suggest the operation of multiple regulatory feedback loops. One can think of this as the system having multiple backup systems to regulate the body, and finely tune it to environmental and internal need. Thus individuals with low HRV have generally impaired function: i.e., they are physically (Volz et al., 1990; Henderson et al., 2004) or emotionally (Friedman and Thayer, 1998; Gorman and Sloan, 2000; Carney and Freedland, 2009; Kemp et al., 2010) sick, are older (Fukusaki et al., 2000; Valentini and Parati, 2009; Nunan et al., 2010; McNarry and Lewis, 2012), are less aerobically fit (De Meersman, 1993; Hautala et al., 2003; McNarry and Lewis, 2012; Boutcher et al., 2013), and, when greatly physically compromised, at greater risk of dying (Kudaiberdieva et al., 2007; Laitio et al., 2007; Chan, 2008; Politano et al., 2008; Ranpuria et al., 2008; Stein, 2008; Ahmad et al., 2009; Thayer et al., 2010; Christensen, 2012; Handa et al., 2012; Huikuri and Stein, 2013). Total HRV in these studies generally is measured by the standard deviation of normal beat-to-beat intervals, i.e., intervals controlled by central nervous system input to the sinoatrial node of the heart, rather than by abnormal cardiac function). People with simpler patterns of HRV appear to be similarly compromised (Otsuka et al., 1997; Srinivasan et al., 2002; Yeragani et al., 2002; Skinner et al., 2008a,b, 2009; Huikuri et al., 2009). For this reason, HRV is often seen as a measure of physical and emotional resilience. We have found that HRV biofeedback restores autonomic function that is suppressed when people are exposed experimentally to inflammatory cytokines (Lehrer et al., 2010). All frequencies are suppressed by these cytokines, much as happens when we catch the flu or are subjected to another inflammatory condition.
RESONANCE
It is a physical principle that all oscillating feedback systems with a constant delay have the characteristic of resonance. A resonant system is one that, when stimulated, produces high-amplitude oscillations at a single frequency, recruiting or overshadowing other frequencies, to produce a sine wave oscillation with very high-amplitude (Başar, 1998). An example of this from everyday life is the so-called Larsen effect, where a high pitched squeal at a single frequency results from placing a microphone in front of a speaker, and stimulating the system with sound (Weaver and Lobkis, 2006).
The same thing appears to happen in the cardiovascular system. The constant delay appears to be caused by amount of blood in the system, although, theoretically, flexibility, and diameter of the blood vessels also should play a role. We have found that taller people and men, who have a greater blood supply than, respectively, shorter people and women, have lower resonance frequencies (Vaschillo et al., 2006). That is, independently, stimulation at lower frequencies causes heart rate oscillations with the highest amplitude in taller people and men. Independently of height, age, and weight have no effect on resonance frequency, nor does experience with repeated system stimulation by HRV biofeedback.
If resonance occurs in the cardiovascular system at approximately 0.1 Hz, it should not occur exclusively in response to breathing. Rather, any source of rhythmic stimulation that affects the cardiovascular system should produce the same effect. This has, in fact, been found for rhythmic muscle tension (Lehrer et al., 2009; Vaschillo et al., 2011), and rhythmical presentation of emotion-inducing pictures (Vaschillo et al., 2008).
There also is evidence that resonance may occur at lower frequencies than 0.1 Hz. There is some evidence for resonance at about 0.02–0.03 Hz. While the source of this resonance is not completely known, it is known that the highest amplitudes of blood pressure oscillations are achieved when the system is stimulated at approximately this frequency (Vaschillo et al., 2002). Oscillations in this range are thought to be controlled primarily by the alpha sympathetic system, and related to oscillations in vascular tone, which also is affected by the baroreflex (Vaschillo et al., 2012). Thus, when blood pressure increases, the blood vessels dilate; when blood pressure falls, blood vessels constrict. This causes an oscillation in both blood pressure and vascular tone, but at a lower frequency than for the heart rate limb of the baroreflex, because dilation and constriction of blood vessels is a slower process than speeding or slowing the heart. Little is known about biofeedback training to stimulate the system in this frequency range. However, it is known that experienced Zen monks tend to breathe in this very slow range (Lehrer et al., 1999). Oscillations in this range are also thought to be involved in reflexes controlling thermoregulation (Fleisher et al., 1996; Thayer et al., 1997; Matsumoto et al., 1999).
THE VAGAL AFFERENT PATHWAY
Several studies have reported that HRVB might be effective in reducing symptoms of depression and/or anxiety (Karavidas et al., 2007; Reiner, 2008; Siepmann et al., 2008; McCraty et al., 2009; Nada, 2009; Zucker et al., 2009; Henriques et al., 2011; Tan et al., 2011; Patron et al., 2013). These results led to speculation that some other mechanism might be at work beyond the baroreflex gains. One possible clue came from the recent research using vagal nerve stimulation for severe depression (and seizure disorders; Sackeim et al., 2001a,b; Nahas et al., 2005; Daban et al., 2008; George et al., 2008; Cristancho et al., 2011). An implanted electrical stimulation device is used to stimulate the vagal afferent pathways resulting in reduction of depressive symptoms. While this technique has not been subjected to larger random controlled trials, the preliminary pilot studies do raise interesting possibilities. It is known that the vagal afferent pathways affect brain areas known to be involved in affect regulation and mood (locus coeruleus, orbitofrontal cortex, insula, hippocampus, and amygdala; Grundy, 2002). In addition, there has been speculation that stimulation especially of the sub-diaphragmatic pathways through slow deeper breathing techniques might be stimulating these same pathways and thus having an effect on depressive/anxiety symptoms (Brown and Gerbarg, 2005a; Porges, 2011; Brown et al., 2013). We discovered some literature that offered the possibility that these pathways could be investigated using a technique called heart period evoked potentials (HEPs). Schandry and his colleagues had used this procedure to study interoception some years ago (Schandry, 1981, 2003; Schandry et al., 1986; Montoya et al., 1993; Critchley et al., 2004; Pollatos et al., 2005a,b; Gray et al., 2007). The HEP is a unique version of the usual evoked response (ERP) in that the R-wave of the ECG is used as a signal rather than being filtered out. Each heart beat then creates a large electrical signal to the brain which can be measured with surface electrodes. The above mentioned studies discovered that those subjects that had better interoception (ability to perceive their heart rates) produced a larger evoked potential presumably by way of the vagal afferent system. So if HRVB is, in fact, stimulating beneficial brain structures, we might see it reflected in the HEP. Thus far, two studies have supported this idea. In the first (MacKinnon et al., 2013), we examined the HEP waveform (it’s called an N250 because it produces a negative deflection at about 250 ms) at baseline in a resting state, during a negative emotion induction, during a positive emotion induction, and during a slow resonance breathing period. Figure 2 shows the results.
As can be seen, the slow breathing condition was significantly more negative than the other conditions.
In a second study (Huang et al., 2014), we trained a group of 12 participants in HRVB over four sessions and compared them to a group of 13 participants who received EMG/relaxation training again over four sessions. As expected the HRVB group improved their HRV substantailly whereas the EMG comparison group did not (see Figure 3).
More importantly, only the HRVB group showed changes in their HEP (see Figure 4).
These results support our speculations and those of Brown and Gerbarg:
“… voluntary control of breath patterns can affect ANS functions via vagal afferents to brainstem nuclei (nucleus tractus solitarius, parabrachial nucleus, locus coeruleus)…Our neurophysiologic model postulates that vagal afferents activate hypothalamic vigilance areas and enhance and enhance attention and alertness, whereas pathways through the thalamus quiet frontal cortical activity and reduce anxious worrying” (Brown and Gerbarg, 2005a,b, p. 713).
Of course, there are many other possible explanations for why the complex process that occurs with HRVB might reduce depression and/or anxiety (distraction, self-efficacy, etc.). However, HEP method might give us a tool to disentangle possible mechanisms of the intervention.
OTHER POTENTIAL MECHANISMS FOR THERAPEUTIC EFFECTS
Because HRV biofeedback is apparently helpful to conditions involving various physiological systems (pain, anxiety, depression, COPD, blood pressure control, athletic performance, etc.), it is probable that a number of mechanisms are involved in various effects, in addition to baroreflex stimulation and effects of vagal afferents. Most of them have received little empirical attention, but they all deserve investigation at this point. Possible mechanisms include:
EFFECTS OF VAGAL EFFERENTS
Parasympathetic activity is usually a component in the “relaxation response.” Stimulation of parasympathetic reflexes by HRV biofeedback may produce body autonomic activity characteristic of relaxation, and thus directly counter stress effects. One way this may occur is a mechanism that has been labeled “Accentuated Antagonism.” “Vagal ‘tone’ predominates over sympathetic tone at rest. Under normal physiological conditions, abrupt parasympathetic stimulation will inhibit tonic sympathetic activation and its effects at rest and during exercise. This response is known as ‘accentuated antagonism’ (Olshansky et al., 2008, p. 863)”. Presumably this aspect of the parasympathetic efferent system is strengthened with HRVB training. This may be at play in inhibiting sympathetic output to myofascial trigger points (Hubbard and Berkoff, 1993; Gevirtz et al., 1996; Hubbard, 1998). The work of the Aziz group in London (Hobson et al., 2008) has also demonstrated that slow breathing almost immediately prevents esophageal pain thresholds from dropping dramatically when acid is introduced to the stomach.
INCREASED GAS EXCHANGE EFFICIENCY
Because of the 0° phase relationship between breathing and heart rate during resonance frequency breathing, improved gas exchange may help people with respiratory diseases, and may even decrease respiratory drive in people with stress-induced hyperventilatory reactions.
MEDITATION EFFECT
Heart rate variability biofeedback involves paying close attention to nuances in breathing. This is very similar to what is done in mindfulness meditation exercises. The pathway here would be primarily mental: i.e., one cannot simultaneously worry about various concerns of the day while concentrating on relaxed breathing.
MECHANICAL STRETCHING OF AIRWAYS
Effects on asthma may occur indirectly through effects of stretching epithelial tissue in the lung by deep breathing. It is known that only a single deep inhalation during a methacholine challenge can decrease airway reactivity to methacholine in asthma patients (Pellegrino et al., 1996; Marchal et al., 2002).
ANTI-INFLAMMATORY EFFECTS
It is known that the vagal system interacts closely with the inflammatory system, such that increases in vagus nerve traffic (usually produced by electrical vagal stimulation) are associated with decreases in serum levels of various inflammatory cytokines (Borovikova et al., 2000; Tracey, 2002). One study did find a decrease in C-reactive proteins among hypertensive patients treated with HRV biofeedback (Nolan et al., 2012). In another study, we experimentally exposed healthy subjects to an inflammatory cytokine, lipopolysaccharide (Lehrer et al., 2010). Usually both sympathetic and parasympathetic activity is blocked by lipopolysaccharide. Although no biofeedback-induced decreases in inflammatory cytokines were found, the autonomic effects of inflammation were greatly modulated, indicating that a greater resiliency was preserved among individuals given HRV biofeedback.
CONCLUSION
In this paper we have tried to summarize possible mechanisms for the effectiveness of HRVB. We have been working under the assumption that increases baroreflex represented a viable marker of improved autonomic homeostasis or increased complexity. We have reviewed the evidence that led us to this conclusion, which now are somewhat established by our labs and others. We now speculate that in conjunction with this class of mechanisms, stimulation of the vagal afferent system may also play a role, especially in disorders of negative affectivity.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
See Gevirtz (2013) for citations.
REFERENCES
- Ahmad S., Tejuja A., Newman K. D., Zarychanski R., Seely A. J. (2009). Clinical review: a review and analysis of heart rate variability and the diagnosis and prognosis of infection. Crit. Care 13:232 10.1186/cc8132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold A. C., Shaltout H. A., Gallagher P. E., Diz D. I. (2009). Leptin impairs cardiovagal baroreflex function at the level of the solitary tract nucleus. Hypertension 54 1001–1008 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.138065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badra L. J., Cooke W. H., Hoag J. B., Crossman A. A., Kuusela T. A., Tahvanainen K. U., et al. (2001). Respiratory modulation of human autonomic rhythms. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 280 H2674–H2688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Başar E. (1998). “Resonance phenomena in the brain, physical systems, and nature,” in Brain Functions and Oscillations ed.Başar E. (Berlin: Springer Verlag; ). 10.1007/978-3-642-72192-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson G. G., Bigger J. T., Jr., Eckberg D. L., Grossman P., Kaufmann P. G., Malik M., et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 34 623–648 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borovikova L. V., Ivanova S., Zhang M., Yang H., Botchkina G. I., Watkins L. R., et al. (2000). Vagus nerve stimulation attenuates the systemic inflammatory response to endotoxin. Nature 405 458–462 10.1038/35013070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boutcher S. H., Park Y., Dunn S. L., Boutcher Y. N. (2013). The relationship between cardiac autonomic function and maximal oxygen uptake response to high-intensity intermittent-exercise training. J. Sports Sci. 31 1024–1029 10.1080/02640414.2012.762984 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown R. P., Gerbarg P. L. (2005a). Sudarshan Kriya Yogic breathing in the treatment of stress, anxiety, and depression. Part II – clinical applications and guidelines. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 11 711–717 10.1089/acm.2005.11.711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown R. P., Gerbarg P. L. (2005b). Sudarshan Kriya yogic breathing in the treatment of stress, anxiety, and depression: part I-neurophysiologic model. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 11 189–201 10.1089/acm.2005.11.189 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown R. P., Gerbarg P. L., Muench F. (2013). Breathing practices for treatment of psychiatric and stress-related medical conditions. Psychiatr. Cliin. North Am. 36 121–140 10.1016/j.psc.2013.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown T. E., Beightol L. A., Koh J., Eckberg D. L. (1993). Important influence of respiration on human R-R interval power spectra is largely ignored. J. Appl. Physiol. 75 2310–2317 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carney R. M., Freedland K. E. (2009). Depression and heart rate variability in patients with coronary heart disease. Cleve. Cliin. J. Med. 76(Suppl. 2), S13–S17 10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.03 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan C. T. (2008). Heart rate variability in patients with end-stage renal disease: an emerging predictive tool for sudden cardiac death? Nephrol. Dial. Transplant. 23 3061–3062 10.1093/ndt/gfn280 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen J. H. (2012). Cardiac autonomic dysfunction in hemodialysis patients assessed by heart rate variability. Minerva Urol. Nefrol. 64 191–198 10.1111/j.1542-4758.2011.00529.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cristancho P., Cristancho M. A., Baltuch G. H., Thase M. E., O’Reardon J. P. (2011). Effectiveness and safety of vagus nerve stimulation for severe treatment-resistant major depression in Clinical practice after FDA approval: outcomes at 1 year. J. Clin. Psychiatry 72 1376–1382 10.4088/JCP.09m05888blu [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley H. D., Wiens S., Rotshtein P., Ohman A., Dolan R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 7 189–195 10.1038/nn1176 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daban C., Martinez-Aran A., Cruz N., Vieta E. (2008). Safety and efficacy of Vagus Nerve Stimulation in treatment-resistant depression. A systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 110 1–15 10.1016/j.jad.2008.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Meersman R. E. (1993). Heart rate variability and aerobic fitness. Am. Heart. J. 125 726–731 10.1016/0002-8703(93)90164-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eckberg D. L. (2003). The human respiratory gate. J. Physiol. 548(Pt 2), 339–352 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.037192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eckberg D. L., Nerhed C., Wallin B. G. (1985). Respiratory modulation of muscle sympathetic and vagal cardiac outflow in man. J. Physiol. 365181–196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eckberg D. L., Sleight P. (1992). Human Baroreflexes in Health and Disease. Oxford: Clarendon Press [Google Scholar]
- Fleisher L. A., Frank S. M., Sessler D. I., Cheng C., Matsukawa T., Vannier C. A. (1996). Thermoregulation and heart rate variability. Clin. Sci. 90 97–103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman B. H., Thayer J. F. (1998). Autonomic balance revisited: panic anxiety and heart rate variability. J. Psychosom. Res. 44 133–151 10.1016/S0022-3999(97)00202-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukusaki C., Kawakubo K., Yamamoto Y. (2000). Assessment of the primary effect of aging on heart rate variability in humans. Clin. Auton. Res. 10 123–130 10.1007/BF02278016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- George M. S., Ward H. E., Jr., Ninan P. T., Pollack M., Nahas Z., Anderson B., et al. (2008). A pilot study of vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatment-resistant anxiety disorders. Brain Stimul. 1 112–121 10.1016/j.brs.2008.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gevirtz R. (2013). The promise of heart rate variability biofeedback: evidence-based applications. Biofeedback 41 110–120 10.5298/1081-5937-41.3.01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gevirtz R., Hubbard D., Harpin E. (1996). Psychophysiologic treatment of chronic low back pain. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 27 561–566 10.1037/0735-7028.27.6.561 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Giardino N. D., Chan L., Borson S. (2004). Combined heart rate variability and pulse oximetry biofeedback for chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: preliminary findings. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 29 121–133 10.1023/B:APBI.0000026638.64386.89 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorman J. M., Sloan R. P. (2000). Heart rate variability in depressive and anxiety disorders. Am. Heart. J. 140(4 Suppl.), 77–83 10.1067/mhj.2000.109981 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray M. A., Taggart P., Sutton P. M., Groves D., Holdright D. R., Bradbury D., et al. (2007). A cortical potential reflecting cardiac function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104 6818–6823 10.1073/pnas.0609509104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman P., Karemaker J., Wieling W. (1991). Prediction of tonic parasympathetic cardiac control using respiratory sinus arrhythmia: the need for respiratory control. Psychophysiology 28 201–216 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb00412.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grundy D. (2002). Neuroanatomy of visceral nociception: vagal and splanchnic afferent. Gut 51(Suppl. 1), i2–i5 10.1136/gut.51.suppl_1.i2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handa R., Poanta L., Rusu D., Albu A. (2012). The role of heart rate variability in assessing the evolution of patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Rom. J. Intern. Med. 50 83–88 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hautala A. J., Makikallio T. H., Kiviniemi A., Laukkanen R. T., Nissila S., Huikuri H. V., et al. (2003). Cardiovascular autonomic function correlates with the response to aerobic training in healthy sedentary subjects. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 285 H1747–H1752 10.1152/ajpheart.00202.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayano J., Yasuma F., Okada A., Mukai S., Fujinami T. (1996). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: a phenomenon improving pulmonary gas exchange and circulatory efficiency. Circulation 94 842–847 10.1161/01.CIR.94.4.842 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson L. A., Richard C. A., Macey P. M., Runquist M. L., Yu P. L., Galons J. P., et al. (2004). Functional magnetic resonance signal changes in neural structures to baroreceptor reflex activation. J. Appl. Physiol. 96 693–703 10.1152/japplphysiol.00852.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henriques G., Keffer S., Abrahamson C., Horst S. J. (2011). Exploring the effectiveness of a computer-based heart rate variability biofeedback program in reducing anxiety in college students. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 36 101–112 10.1007/s10484-011-9151-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang C., Gevirtz R., Onton J., Criado J. (2014). Investigation of vagal afferent functioning using heartbeat event related potentials. Paper Presented at the 45th Annual Meeting of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback Savannah, GA [Google Scholar]
- Hobson A. R., Furlong P. L., Aziz Q. (2008). Oesophageal afferent pathway sensitivity in non-erosive reflux disease. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 20 877–883 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2008.01122.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubbard D. (1998). Persistent muscular pain: approaches to relieving trigger points. J. Musculoskelat. Med. 15 16–26 [Google Scholar]
- Hubbard D. R., Berkoff G. M. (1993). Myofascial trigger points show spontaneous needle EMG activity. Spine 18 1803–1807 10.1097/00007632-199310000-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huikuri H. V., Perkiomaki J. S., Maestri R., Pinna G. D. (2009). Clinical impact of evaluation of cardiovascular control by novel methods of heart rate dynamics. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 367 1223–1238 10.1098/rsta.2008.0294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huikuri H. V., Stein P. K. (2013). Heart rate variability in risk stratification of cardiac patients. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 56 153–159 10.1016/j.pcad.2013.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanov P. C., Amaral L. A., Goldberger A. L., Havlin S., Rosenblum M. G., Struzik Z. R., et al. (1999). Multifractality in human heartbeat dynamics. Nature 399 461–465 10.1038/20924 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karavidas M. K., Lehrer P. M., Vaschillo E., Vaschillo B., Marin H., Buyske S., et al. (2007). Preliminary results of an open label study of heart rate variability biofeedback for the treatment of major depression. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 32 19–30 10.1007/s10484-006-9029-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp A. H., Quintana D. S., Gray M. A., Felmingham K. L., Brown K., Gatt J. M. (2010). Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: a review and meta-analysis. Biol. Psychiatry 67 1067–1074 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudaiberdieva G., Gorenek B., Timuralp B. (2007). Heart rate variability as a predictor of sudden cardiac death. Anadolu. Kardiyol. Derg. 7(Suppl. 1),68–70 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laitio T., Jalonen J., Kuusela T., Scheinin H. (2007). The role of heart rate variability in risk stratification for adverse postoperative cardiac events. Anesth. Analg. 105 1548–1560 10.1213/01.ane.0000287654.49358.3a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer P., Eddie D. (2013). Dynamic processes in regulation and some implications for biofeedback and biobehavioral interventions. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 38 143–155 10.1007/s10484-013-9217-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer P., Karavidas M. K., Lu S.-E., Coyle S. M., Oikawa L. O., Macor M., et al. (2010). Voluntarily produced increases in heart rate variability modulate autonomic effects of endotoxin induced systemic inflammation: an exploratory study. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 35 303–315 10.1007/s10484-010-9139-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer P. M., Vaschillo E., Vaschillo B. (2000). Resonant frequency biofeedback training to increase cardiac variability: rationale and manual for training. Appl. Psychophyisol. Biofeedback 25 177–191 10.1023/A:1009554825745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer P. M., Vaschillo E., Vaschillo B., Lu S. E., Eckberg D. L., Edelberg R., et al. (2003). Heart rate variability biofeedback increases baroreflex gain and peak expiratory flow. Psychosom. Med. 65 796–805 10.1097/01.PSY.0000089200.81962.19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer P., Sasaki Y., Saito Y. (1999). Zazen and cardiac variability. Psychosom. Med. 61 812–821 10.1097/00006842-199911000-00014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer P., Vaschillo E., Trost Z., France C. R. (2009). Effects of rhythmical muscle tension at 0.1 Hz on cardiovascular resonance and the baroreflex. Biol. Psychol. 81 24–30 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin G., Xiang Q., Fu X., Wang S., Wang S., Chen S., et al. (2012). Heart rate variability biofeedback decreases blood pressure in prehypertensive subjects by improving autonomic function and baroreflex. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 18 143–152 10.1089/acm.2010.0607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon S., Gevirtz R., McCraty R., Brown M. (2013). Utilizing heartbeat evoked potentials to identify cardiac regulation of vagal afferents during emotion and resonant breathing. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 38 241–255 10.1007/s10484-013-9226-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marchal F., Schweitzer C., Moreau-Colson C. (2002). Respiratory impedance response to a deep inhalation in children with history of cough or asthma. Pediatr. Pulmonol. 33 411–418 10.1002/ppul.10093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto T., Miyawaki T., Ue H., Kanda T., Zenji C., Moritani T. (1999). Autonomic responsiveness to acute cold exposure in obese and non-obese young women. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 23 793–800 10.1038/sj.ijo.0800928 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCraty R., Atkinson M., Lipsenthal L., Arguelles L. (2009). New hope for correctional officers: an innovative program for reducing stress and health risks. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 34 251–272 10.1007/s10484-009-9087-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNarry M. A., Lewis M. J. (2012). Interaction between age and aerobic fitness in determining heart rate dynamics. Physiol. Meas. 33 901–914 10.1088/0967-3334/33/6/901 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montoya P., Schandry R., Muller A. (1993). Heartbeat evoked potentials (HEP): topography and influence of cardiac awareness and focus of attention. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 88 163–172 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90001-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nada P. J. (2009). Heart rate variability in the assessment and biofeedback training of common mental health problems in children. Med. Arh. 63 244–248 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nahas Z., Marangell L. B., Husain M. M., Rush A. J., Sackeim H. A., Lisanby S. H., et al. (2005). Two-year outcome of vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatment of major depressive episodes. J. Cliin. Psychiatry 66 1097–1104 10.4088/JCP.v66n0902 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolan R. P., Floras J. S., Ahmed L., Harvey P. J., Hiscock N., Hendrickx H., et al. (2012). Behavioural modification of the cholinergic anti-inflammatory response to c-reactive protein in patients with hypertension. J. Intern. Med. 272 161–169 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2012.02523.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolan R. P., Floras J. S., Harvey P. J., Kamath M. V., Picton P. E., Chessex C., et al. (2010). Behavioral neurocardiac training in hypertension: a randomized, controlled trial. Hypertension 55 1033–1039 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.146233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunan D., Sandercock G. R., Brodie D. A. (2010). A quantitative systematic review of normal values for short-term heart rate variability in healthy adults. Pacing Clin. Electrophysiol. 33 1407–1417 10.1111/j.1540-8159.2010.02841.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olshansky B., Sabbah H. N., Hauptman P. J., Colucci W. S. (2008). Parasympathetic nervous system and heart failure: pathophysiology and potential implications for therapy. Circulation 118 863–871 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.760405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otsuka K., Cornelissen G., Halberg F. (1997). Circadian rhythmic fractal scaling of heart rate variability in health and coronary artery disease. Clin. Cardiol. 20 631–638 10.1002/clc.4960200710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Passino C., Sleight P., Valle F., Spadacini G., Leuzzi S., Bernardi L. (1997). Lack of peripheral modulation of cardiovascular central oscillatory autonomic activity during apnea in humans. Am. J. Physiol. 272(1Pt 2), H123–H129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patron E., Messerotti Benvenuti S., Favretto G., Valfre C., Bonfa C., Gasparotto R., et al. (2013). Biofeedback assisted control of respiratory sinus arrhythmia as a biobehavioral intervention for depressive symptoms in patients after cardiac surgery: a preliminary study. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 38 1–9 10.1007/s10484-012-9202-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paul M., Garg K. (2012). The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback on performance psychology of basketball players. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 37 131–144 10.1007/s10484-012-9185-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellegrino R., Violante B., Brusasco V. (1996). Maximal bronchoconstriction in humans. Relationship to deep inhalation and airway sensitivity. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 153 115–121 10.1164/ajrccm.153.1.8542103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pikkujamsa S. M., Makikallio T. H., Sourander L. B., Raiha I. J., Puukka P., Skytta J., et al. (1999). Cardiac interbeat interval dynamics from childhood to senescence : comparison of conventional and new measures based on fractals and chaos theory. Circulation 100 393–399 10.1161/01.CIR.100.4.393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Politano L., Palladino A., Nigro G., Scutifero M., Cozza V. (2008). Usefulness of heart rate variability as a predictor of sudden cardiac death in muscular dystrophies. Acta Myol. 27 114–122 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollatos O., Kirsch W., Schandry R. (2005a). Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26 54–64 10.1002/hbm.20121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollatos O., Kirsch W., Schandry R. (2005b). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness, emotional experience, and brain processes. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 25 948–962 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polson J. W., Dampney R. A., Boscan P., Pickering A. E., Paton J. F. (2007). Differential baroreflex control of sympathetic drive by angiotensin II in the nucleus tractus solitarii. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 293 R1954–R1960 10.1152/ajpregu.00041.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porges S. (2011). The Polyvagal theory. New York: W. W. Norton & Company [Google Scholar]
- Ranpuria R., Hall M., Chan C. T., Unruh M. (2008). Heart rate variability (HRV) in kidney failure: measurement and consequences of reduced HRV. Nephrol. Dial. Transplant. 23 444–449 10.1093/ndt/gfm634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raven P. B., Potts J. T., Shi X. (1997). Baroreflex regulation of blood pressure during dynamic exercise. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 25 365–389 10.1249/00003677-199700250-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiner R. (2008). Integrating a portable biofeedback device into Clinical practice for patients with anxiety disorders: results of a pilot study. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 33 55–61 10.1007/s10484-007-9046-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers R. F., Rybak I. A., Schwaber J. S. (2000). Computational modeling of the baroreflex arc: nucleus tractus solitarius. Brain Res. Bull. 51 139–150 10.1016/S0361-9230(99)00242-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sackeim H. A., Keilp J. G., Rush A. J., George M. S., Marangell L. B., Dormer J. S., et al. (2001a). The effects of vagus nerve stimulation on cognitive performance in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 14 53–62 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sackeim H. A., Rush A. J., George M. S., Marangell L. B., Husain M. M., Nahas Z., et al. (2001b). Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatment-resistant depression: efficacy, side effects, and predictors of outcome. Neuropsychopharmacology 25 713–728 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00271-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schandry R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18 483–488 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schandry R. (2003). [From the heart to the brain]. Deutsch. Med. Wochenschr. 128 2707–2711 10.1055/s-2003-812550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schandry R., Sparrer B., Weitkunat R. (1986). From the heart to the brain: a study of heartbeat contingent scalp potentials. Int. J. Neurosci. 30 261–275 10.3109/00207458608985677 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw L. (2011). Setting the balance: assessment of a biofeedback intervention for improving competitive performance with a Division 1 gymnastics beam team. Diss. Abstr. Int. B Sci. Eng. 71:4496 [Google Scholar]
- Siepmann M., Aykac V., Unterdorfer J., Petrowski K., Mueck-Weymann M. (2008). A pilot study on the effects of heart rate variability biofeedback in patients with depression and in healthy subjects. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 33 195–201 10.1007/s10484-008-9064-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner J. E., Anchin J. M., Weiss D. N. (2008a). Nonlinear analysis of the heartbeats in public patient ECGs using an automated PD2i algorithm for risk stratification of arrhythmic death. Ther. Cliin. Risk Manag. 4549–557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner J. E., Meyer M., Dalsey W. C., Nester B. A., Ramalanjaona G., O’Neil B. J., et al. (2008b). Risk stratification for arrhythmic death in an emergency department cohort: a new method of nonlinear PD2i analysis of the ECG. Ther. Clin. Risk Manag. 4 689–697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner J. E., Meyer M., Nester B. A., Geary U., Taggart P., Mangione A., et al. (2009). Comparison of linear-stochastic and nonlinear-deterministic algorithms in the analysis of 15-minute Clinical ECGs to predict risk of arrhythmic death. Ther. Clin. Risk Manag. 5 671–682 10.2147/TCRM.S5568 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song H. S., Lehrer P. M. (2003). The effects of specific respiratory rates on heart rate and heart rate variability. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 28 13–24 10.1023/A:1022312815649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srinivasan K., Ashok M., Vaz M., Yeragani V. K. (2002). Decreased chaos of heart rate time series in children of patients with panic disorder. Depress. Anxiety 15 159–167 10.1002/da.10046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein K. M. (2008). Noninvasive risk stratification for sudden death: signal-averaged electrocardiography, nonsustained ventricular tachycardia, heart rate variability, baroreflex sensitivity, and QRS duration. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 51 106–117 10.1016/j.pcad.2007.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strack B. W. (2003). Effect of heart rate variability (hrv) biofeedback on batting performance in baseball. Diss. Abstr. Int. B Sci. Eng. 64:1540 [Google Scholar]
- Strauss-Blasche G., Moser M., Voica M., McLeod D. R., Klammer N., Marktl W. (2000). Relative timing of inspiration and expiration affects respiratory sinus arrhythmia. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 27 601–606 10.1046/j.1440-1681.2000.03306.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan G., Dao T. K., Farmer L., Sutherland R. J., Gevirtz R. (2011). Heart rate variability (HRV) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): a pilot study. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 36 27–35 10.1007/s10484-010-9141-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer J. F., Nabors-Oberg R., Sollers J. J., III (1997). Thermoregulation, and cardiac variability: a time-frequency analysis. Biomed. Sci. Instrum. 34 252–256 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer J. F., Yamamoto S. S., Brosschot J. F. (2010). The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Int. J. Cardiol. 141 122–131 10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tracey K. J. (2002). The inflammatory reflex. Nature 420 853–859 10.1038/nature01321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentini M., Parati G. (2009). Variables influencing heart rate. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 52 11–19 10.1016/j.pcad.2009.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van de Louw A., Médigue C., Papelier Y., Landrain M., Cottin F. (2010). Role of brainstem centers in cardiorespiratory phase difference during mechanical ventilation. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 174 119–127 10.1016/j.resp.2010.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaschillo E. G., Bates M. E., Vaschillo B., Lehrer P., Udo T., Mun E. Y., et al. (2008). Heart rate variability response to alcohol, placebo, and emotional picture cue challenges: effects of 0.1-Hz stimulation. Psychophysiology 45 847–858 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00673.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaschillo E. G., Vaschillo B., Buckman J. F., Pandina R. J., Bates M. E. (2012). Measurement of vascular tone and stroke volume baroreflex gain. Psychophysiology 49 193–197 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01305.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaschillo E. G., Vaschillo B., Lehrer P. M. (2006). Characteristics of resonance in heart rate variability stimulated by biofeedback. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 31 129–142 10.1007/s10484-006-9009-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaschillo E. G., Vaschillo B., Pandina R. J., Bates M. E. (2011). Resonances in the cardiovascular system caused by rhythmical muscle tension. Psychophysiology 48 927–936 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01156.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaschillo E., Lehrer P., Rishe N., Konstantinov M. (2002). Heart rate variability biofeedback as a method for assessing baroreflex function: a preliminary study of resonance in the cardiovascular system. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 27 1–27 10.1023/A:1014587304314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volz H. P., Rehbein G., Triepel J., Knuepfer M. M., Stumpf H., Stock G. (1990). Afferent connections of the nucleus centralis amygdalae. A horseradish peroxidase study and literature survey. Anat. Embryol. 181 177–194 10.1007/BF00198957 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang S. Z., Li S., Xu X. Y., Lin G. P., Shao L., Zhao Y., et al. (2010). Effect of slow abdominal breathing combined with biofeedback on blood pressure and heart rate variability in prehypertension. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 16 1039–1045 10.1089/acm.2009.0577 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weaver R. L., Lobkis O. I. (2006). On the linewidth of the ultrasonic Larsen effect in a reverberant body. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120 102–109 10.1121/1.2205128 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yeragani V. K., Rao K., Smitha M., Pohl R. B., Balon R., Srinivasan K. (2002). Diminished chaos of heart rate time series in patients with major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 51 733–744 10.1016/S0006-3223%2801%2901347-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zucker T. L., Samuelson K. W., Muench F., Greenberg M. A., Gevirtz R. N. (2009). The effects of respiratory sinus arrhythmia biofeedback on heart rate variability and posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms: a pilot study. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 34 135–143 10.1007/s10484-009-9085-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]