Abstract
Humans form impressions of others by associating persons (faces) with negative or positive social outcomes. This learning process has been referred to as social conditioning. In everyday life, affective nonverbal gestures may constitute important social signals cueing threat or safety, which therefore may support aforementioned learning processes. In conventional aversive conditioning, studies using electroencephalography to investigate visuocortical processing of visual stimuli paired with danger cues such as aversive noise have demonstrated facilitated processing and enhanced sensory gain in visual cortex. The present study aimed at extending this line of research to the field of social conditioning by pairing neutral face stimuli with affective nonverbal gestures. To this end, electro-cortical processing of faces serving as different conditioned stimuli was investigated in a differential social conditioning paradigm. Behavioral ratings and visually evoked steady-state potentials (ssVEP) were recorded in twenty healthy human participants, who underwent a differential conditioning procedure in which three neutral faces were paired with pictures of negative (raised middle finger), neutral (pointing), or positive (thumbs-up) gestures. As expected, faces associated with the aversive hand gesture (raised middle finger) elicited larger ssVEP amplitudes during conditioning. Moreover, theses faces were rated as to be more arousing and unpleasant. These results suggest that cortical engagement in response to faces aversively conditioned with nonverbal gestures is facilitated in order to establish persistent vigilance for social threat-related cues. This form of social conditioning allows to establish a predictive relationship between social stimuli and motivationally relevant outcomes.
Introduction
Traditionally, in classical aversive conditioning either highly aversive electric stimuli [1], [2], [3], [4], [5] or loud aversive bursts of (white) noise [6], [7], [8], [9], [10] have been used as aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), which have been proven to elicit strong fear reactions and enhanced amygdala activity in response to the conditioned stimulus (CS). Comparable effects were found for other types of US, such as odor stimuli [11] and negative emotional pictures [12]. From a social neuroscience perspective however, one has to note that affective and social learning processes outside the laboratory are rarely happening with these types of US stimuli. In contrast, one may consider these types of stimuli as ecologically less valid because humans seldom encounter such stimuli in everyday life. Admittedly, social stimuli (verbal or non-verbal) are much more likely to function as US in everyday social learning situations, and thus contribute to impression formation and social and affective learning. Particularly, the ability to identify individual faces based on the social consequences they have predicted in the past constitutes an essential form of associative learning in humans. This learning mechanism has been coined social conditioning, defined as process whereby an individual learns to identify other individuals that have predicted threats or rewards in the past [13].
Only recently researchers have started using social and hence ecologically more valid US such as verbal descriptions (sentences), affective prosody, and facial expressions [13], [14], [15], [16] to investigate the effects and neural correlates of social conditioning. Using verbal feedback sentences as US (e.g., “He says you're stupid”), it was shown that faces associated with pleasant and unpleasant social outcomes elicited larger activations in the human amygdala compared to when subjects learned that a face predicted neutral social outcomes [13]. Consistent with these findings, pairing faces with aversive audiovisual US (negative faces combined with a male voice saying “Stupid”) also led to efficient social aversive learning and concurrent amygdala activation to the fear-associated CS face [14]. These studies suggests that social US, although less intense than conventional US, are sufficient to cause conditioning and modulate amygdala responses to previously neutral stimuli. In a further study in which social conditioning was investigated in social anxiety disorder, participants underwent differential social conditioning incorporating socially stressful US such as critical facial expressions combined with derogatory verbal feedback [15]. Interestingly, only socially anxious subjects demonstrated fear conditioning, as a potentiated startle blink reflex to the CS face predictive of a negative compared to both CS predictive of a neutral or a positive social outcome indicated. The latter study points at the notion that socially relevant US may especially disseminate their anxiogenic effect in individuals with social anxiety disorder. This notion was also recently supported by findings of enhanced amygdala activity in socially anxious individuals in response to neutral faces which have been previously associated with videos of negative feedback [16].
The aim of the current study was to examine the electrocortical correlates of social conditioning, i.e. how the visual brain responds to socially conditioned faces. The conditioned stimuli consisted of three neutral faces which were paired with unpleasant, neutral, or pleasant hand gestures during the acquisition phase. Symbolic hand gestures carrying affective meaning appear well-suited as social US, as they have been shown to be preferentially processed by the brain [17], [18], [19]. Steady-state visually evoked potentials (ssVEPs) in response to faces were used to quantify the degree of visuocortical engagement to the different CS cues. The ssVEP is an oscillatory response of the visual cortex elicited by luminance- or contrast-modulated stimuli in which the frequency of the electrocortical response recorded from the scalp equals that of the driving [20], [21]. Here, the frequency of the cortical response is precisely known and can therefore be reliably separated from noise and quantified in the frequency domain [22]. Moreover and of significant advantage in conditioning paradigms where the trial number is usually limited, ssVEPs possess excellent signal-to-noise ratios compared to traditional ERP components [23]. An amplitude enhancement of the ssVEP reflects heightened visuocortical activation in response to a stimulus, which has been demonstrated to be modulated both by bottom-up sources of signal salience [24] and top-down, task-related processes [25], [26]. The neural sources of the ssVEP have been localized to the primary and extended visual cortex [27], [28], with strong contributions from retinotopic areas, but also from cortices higher in the visual hierarchy [29]. In experiments on differential aversive conditioning, ssVEP and ssVEF responses (its magnetic relative measured by magnetencephalography) were found to be reliably enhanced for CS+ compared to CS- cues [9], [10], [30], [31], [32].
Based on the literature as reviewed above, we examined the hypothesis that affective CS cues elicit larger sensory responses compared to neutral CS cues, following differential social conditioning with pictures of affective and neutral gestures as US. Based on differential amygdala activity findings in social conditioning and larger motivational relevance of negative gestures of insult, we further explored whether visual cortex activity was also higher for CS cues paired with negative compared to CS cues paired with positive gestures.
Methods
Participants
Twenty undergraduate students (11 females, mean age M = 20.8, SD = 2.6 years) from the University of Würzburg with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in this study for course credits. All participants were screened for personal and family history of photic epilepsy. Nineteen participants were right-handed, one participant left handed. The institutional review board at the University of Würzburg approved all experimental procedures; all participants provided written informed consent.
Stimuli
The conditioned stimuli (CS) consisted of pictures of 3 male faces taken from the Radboud Faces database [33], which were converted to grey-scale, adjusted for brightness, luminance and contrast, and presented using Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, CA, USA). Only male faces were used as it has been shown that male faces seem to be more efficient in fear conditioning and to elicit stronger responses in both men and women, for a review see [34]. The CS cues were delivered for 5000 ms in a flickering mode in front of a uniform gray background at a frequency of 12 Hz in order to elicit the ssVEP. The unconditioned stimuli (US) were pictures of unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant hand gestures [17], [19], which were presented in the conditioning phase only, immediately at offset of the CS faces for 500 ms. Pictures used as CS and US are given in Figure 1.
Design and Procedure
The experiment contained three blocks (habituation, acquisition, extinction), each consisting of 60 trials (three faces, each presented 20 times) resulting in 180 total trials. In the habituation and extinction phase, faces were presented without any pairings with the US. In the acquisition phase, each face was paired with one of the three hand gestures such that the picture of the respective hand gesture immediately followed the 5000 ms presentation of the face stimulus. The combination of faces and hand gestures was counter-balanced across participants. The order of the stimuli within each block was pseudo-randomized such that no more than two of the same faces ever occurred consecutively during the different phases. After providing written informed consent and initial screening to rule out photic epilepsy/seizures, participants were seated in a sound-attenuated, dimly lit testing room where the electroencephalogram (EEG) sensor net was applied. Participants were instructed that they would view flickering faces of three different individuals, which would at some point during the experiment be combined with pictures of hand gestures. Participants were not informed of a specific relation between CSs and the US. Each picture was displayed centrally on a 19-inch computer monitor (resolution = 1280×1024 pixel) with a vertical refresh rate of 60 Hz, located approximately 80 cm in front of the participant, resulting in a picture presentation with a visual angle of 4.2° horizontally and 5.9° vertically. Each CS was presented on the screen for 5000 ms, with inter-trial intervals varying between 2000 and 3000 ms. Participants were asked to rate each CS stimulus for hedonic valence and arousal after each phase (Habituation, Acquisition, Extinction) using a computer-based version of the Self-Assessment Manikin Scale SAM [35]. The SAM is a language-free instrument for rating hedonic valence and consists of a graphic figure representing nine levels of pleasure/displeasure. Contingency awareness was also assessed using an online analogue scale, in which participants were to indicate the probability of the face to be paired with one of the three US. The purpose of the contingency rating was to determine whether participants successfully learned the CS-US pairing rule. The contingency ratings were obtained immediately after the conditioning phase. After the three experimental phases, participants were asked to rate the US stimuli for affective valence and arousal using the SAM scales.
EEG Data Recording
EEG was recorded continuously from 129 electrodes using an Electrical Geodesics (EGI) high-density EEG system and digitized at a rate of 250 Hz, using Cz as a recording reference. Impedances were kept below 50 kΩ, as recommended for the Electrical Geodesics high input-impedance amplifiers. All channels were filtered on-line with 0.1 and 100-Hz and 50 Hz notch filter.
EEG Data Reduction and Data Analysis
Offline EEG analyses were implemented using the ElectroMagnetoEncephalography toolbox for MATLAB [36]. Epochs of 600 ms pre-stimulus and 5600 ms post-stimulus onset were extracted offline. Data were filtered using a 40-Hz low-pass (45 dB/octave, 12th order Butterworth) filter. Artifact rejection was performed following the procedure proposed by Junghöfer, Elbert, Tucker, and Rockstroh [37]. This procedure creates distributions of statistical indices of data quality and allows to identify bad channels and trials, with the latter being discarded and the former being interpolated from the full channel set. In a subsequent step, data was re-referenced to average reference, and artifact-free trials were averaged for each subject according to experimental conditions. Trials were rejected when more than 20 channels out of 129 were outlying as per the statistical parameters used for artifact identification: the mean absolute (rectified) amplitude; the variability over time points; and the maximum first order derivate (gradient). Using this method, 74% of the trials were retained. A minimum number of 3 trials per condition were retained. The number of artifact-free trials did not differ between conditions per phase.
The artifact-free ssVEP epochs were averaged, and the time-varying amplitude of the ssVEP signal was then extracted by means of Hilbert transform on the time-domain averaged ssVEP data [9]. To this end, data were first bandpass-filtered with a 12th order Butterworth filter having a width of .5-Hz (48 dB/octave), around the target frequency of 12 Hz. To achieve high time resolution, instantaneous amplitudes of the band-pass filtered signal were computed using the Hilbert function implemented in MATLAB. The Hilbert transformation possesses high temporal resolution for indexing rapid changes in ssVEP amplitude. The absolute value of Hilbert transform corresponds to the envelope of the averaged waveform [38]. Figure 2 depicts the steady-state visually evoked potential (averaged across conditions and participants) in the time domain, demonstrating the onset of the oscillatory visuocortical response at the driving frequency (12 Hz) and its frequency spectrum as derived from FFT.
Statistical Analysis
As was seen in previous work with centrally presented stimuli [9], [28], [39], [40], [41], [42], amplitudes of the ssVEPs were most pronounced at electrode locations near the medial occipital electrode Oz, over the occipital pole. Thus, to test conditioning-induced changes in visuo-cortical responses to the different CS, the ssVEP activity was averaged across 8 medial occipital sensors including Oz in the International 10/20 System (EGI sensors 70, 71, 72, 74, 75, 76, 82, 83; see Figure 3).
Mean ssVEP amplitudes (100–4900 ms) were analyzed by means of repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVAs). The ANOVA contained the following within-subjects factors: Phase (Habituation, Acquisition, Extinction), and CS-Type (CSneg vs. CSneu vs. CSpos). To investigate whether cortical activation differed across picture presentation time [9], an additional ANOVA analysis was carried out using two time windows of the ssVEP amplitudes (100–2500 ms and 2501–4900 ms), consequently including the factor time (early vs. late) as an additional within-subject factor. SAM ratings for valence and arousal were averaged for each stimulus and phase, and submitted to separate mixed-model ANOVAs, containing the within-subjects factors Phase and CS type. The Mauchly's test of sphericity was used to test for violations of this assumption and wherever relevant, the Greenhouse-Geisser corrected results are provided with uncorrected degrees of freedom, corrected F and p values [43].
Results
Electrocortical activity (ssVEPs)
The ANOVA on the mean amplitudes across the whole viewing time revealed a significant interaction of phase and CS type, F(4,76) = 2.88, GG-ε = .63, p = .045, ηp 2 = .13. (see Figure 4). Separate ANOVAs per phase revealed significant modulations of the ssVEP amplitude for the conditioning phase, only, F(2,38) = 3.82, p = .031, ηp 2 = .17. Planned contrasts showed that CSneg faces evoked larger ssVEP amplitudes compared to CSneu faces, t(19) = 2.73, p = .013 (Bonferroni-corrected p = .017), but CSpos faces compared to CSneu faces elicited only marginally larger amplitudes, t(19) = 1.93, p = .069 (Bonferroni-corrected p = .017). No differences emerged between CSneg and CSpos faces (Figure 5).
The analysis of the time course of the ssVEP amplitude including earlier and later time windows (100–2500 ms and 2501–4900 ms) did not find any interaction including the factor time, but a significant main effect of time, F(1,19) = 8.91, p = .008, ηp 2 = .32, with higher amplitudes in the first compared to the second time window. Additionally, the Phase x CS type interaction was significant, F(4,76) = 2.88, GG-ε = .61, p = .045, ηp 2 = .13.
Affective Ratings
As expected, arousal and valence ratings changed across the three phases of the experiment depending on the CS type, as the interaction of Phase X CS Type indicated, F(4,76) = 3.35, p = .014, ηp 2 = .15, and F(4,76) = 2.81, p = .031, ηp 2 = .13, respectively (see Figure 6).
For both ratings, also a significant main effect of CS type was observed: arousal ratings, F(2,38) = 4.60, GG-€ = .77, p = .026, ηp 2 = .20; valence ratings: F(2,38) = 7.96, p = .001, ηp 2 = .30. To follow up on the interaction, separate ANOVAS per phase were run. For arousal ratings it turned out that differences were only significant in the conditioning phase, F(2,38) = 7.60, p = .002, ηp 2 = .29. This was due to CSneg face cues were rated as to be more arousing compared to CSneu faces, t(19) = 4.09, p = .001, whereas the comparison of CSpos and CSneu just missed significance, t(19) = 2.24, p = .037 (Bonferroni-corrected p = .017). For valence ratings, separate ANOVAS per phase revealed significant differences between CS types after the conditioning phase, F(2,38) = 12.78, p<.001, ηp 2 = .40, and the extinction phase, F(2,38) = 6.32, GG-€ = .78, p = .009, ηp 2 = .25. Post-hoc t-tests showed that after conditioning, CSneg cues were rated as to be more unpleasant compared to CSneu and CSpos, t(19) = 3.67, p = .002, and t(19) = 4.56, p<.001 (Bonferroni-corrected p = .017). After extinction, only the difference between CSneg and CSpos cues was still significant, t(19) = 4.76, p<.001 (Bonferroni-corrected p = .017).
US ratings
The analysis of the US ratings revealed that the different gestures which served as US during conditioning were rated as differentially arousing as expected, F(2,38) = 6.01, p = .005, ηp 2 = .24. The middle finger gesture (M = 5.80, SD = 1.74) was rated as more arousing than the point gesture (M = 4.15, SD = 1.76), t(19) = 3.38, p = .003, whereas the arousal rating of the thumbs-up gesture (M = 5.40, SD = 1.54) was only marginally higher than the neutral point gesture, t(19) = 5.93, p = .024 (Bonferroni-corrected p = .017). No difference emerged between middle finger and thumbs-up gesture, t(19) = 1.05, p = .31. With regard to valence, ratings were also modulated by type of pictures, F(2,38) = 85.21, p<.001, ηp 2 = .82. As expected, the insult gesture (M = 2.90, SD = 1.12) was rated more unpleasant, whereas thumbs-up gesture was rated as more pleasant (M = 7.30, SD = 0.80) than the neutral point gesture (M = 4.55, SD = 0.95), t(19) = 4.44, p<.001, and t(19) = 9.23, p<.001.
Contingency ratings
The analysis of correctly identified contingencies per category did not reveal any differences between CS types, F(2,38) = 1.48, p = .241, ηp 2 = .07. Faces were correctly identified as CSneg in 97.5%, as CSneu in 98.0%, and as CSpos in 99.5% of cases.
Discussion
Faces associated with negative social cues (raised middle fingers) elicited stronger mass neuronal responses within the visual cortex compared to faces associated with neutral social signals. No differences were found between face-evoked cortical activity in response to faces that indicated negative compared to positive social consequences, however, the difference between neutrally and positively associated faces was only small. Affective ratings confirm these findings, but also demonstrate longer-lasting effects in explicit ratings, as differences were still observable after the extinction phase. Altogether, the findings suggest that response gain in local cortical population activity is modulated by the acquired social and motivational significance of the faces.
The enhanced electrocortical activation in response to faces predictive of negative social signals indicates that social conditioning alters visuocortical processing in a similar manner as more conventional aversive conditioning in which gratings were associated with aversive sounds or electrical shocks [9], [10], [30], [31], [32], [41]. These adaptive changes in function of early visual cortices leads to augmented sensory gain and consequently enhanced processing of CS+ related features [44]. This change in sensory processing during social fear acquisition may be due to transient plasticity of sensory cortical networks [45]. Most likely, this short-term plasticity related to the individual learning history is due to re-entrant modulations of visual areas both by sub-cortical areas such as the amygdala as well as top-down influences of the fronto-parietal attention network. This corroborates findings which demonstrated that the amygdala shows elevated responses to socially conditioned stimuli [13], [14], [16]. Findings of conditioned responses in the lateral amygdala [46] and thalamus [47] preceding those that are observed in the primary sensory cortices support the notion that subcortical centers are necessary for the induction of sustained fear-related plasticity in the cortex. The amygdala can serve to enhance visual cortex activity given extensive bidirectional connectivity between amygdalar nuclei and multiple stages of visual hierarchy known to exist in the brains of human and non-human animals [48]. However, endogenous processes within sensory cortices may also underlie some transient forms of plasticity [49] in sensory cortical areas.
Notably, the amplification of sensory processing in response to socially conditioned faces bears striking similarities to the enhanced processing of visual cues which are inherently threatening such as aversive pictures or threatening faces [50], [51]. Thus, sensory cortices seem to preferentially react to threatening information regardless of their threat values to be acquired by associated learning processes or inherent due to preparedness mechanisms of phylogenetic origin [52]. This observation is also in line with assumptions that sensory cortical networks are rather characterized as being highly adaptive and continuously shaped by the organism's learning history than just holding invariant representations of the external world [53], [54]. Thus, features that are especially predictive of negative outcomes due to a learning history lead to enhanced sensory gain [41]. Future research may compare differences in the processing of inherent and acquired threat cues directly to further shed light on the nature of the development of anxiety and anxiety disorders [55].
The nature of the US in the present study (affective symbolic gestures) points at the notion that nonverbal socio-communicative signals may serve as cues in social learning experience. Given the high emotional significance of particularly the aversive raised middle finger gesture [17], [19], it seems plausible to regard the current paradigm as an excellent model for real-life situations in which subjects are exposed to social stress and form their impressions based on the social consequences they experienced with the very person. The result of the strongest learning effect with the negative gesture (raised middle finger) is in line with enhanced early cortical activity observed in the processing of this gesture of insult [19], which is most likely due to its immediate association with social threat and need of urgent action [17]. In line with other studies [13], [14], [16] using more ecologically valid US such as human voices, faces, verbal feedback, our results confirm that an emotional nonverbal gesture is sufficient to cause conditioning and modulate responses in the visual cortex. It has to be noted that this effect is observed although the US (picture) clearly is much less intense than conventional US such as electrical stimuli. Altogether, the present results make the paradigm of social conditioning with nonverbal gestures an interesting avenue for research on social learning and altered social conditioning in social anxiety, where enhanced sensitivity to social conditioning is assumed [15]. It has to be noted that the current paradigm is also a particular form of evaluative conditioning, in which pairings of positive or negative stimuli (US) with neutral stimuli (CS) induce the learning of evaluative reactions to the target stimuli [56]. Thus, originally neutral faces adopt the evaluative color of the US gestures with which they have been paired previously. Further research needs to clarify whether social conditioning is a different phenomenon such that social CS and US lead to stronger associative learning compared to evaluative conditioning by the easier association of two stimuli social in nature, i.e. a face and nonverbal gesture versus a face and aversive picture of a snake, for example. One may assume that social conditioning as presented here leads to stronger effects for implicit measures (such as visuocortical responses), which have been found to be rather weak in more conventional evaluative conditioning paradigms [57].
The present findings also add to the notion that the perception and evaluation of faces is critically dependent on the context in which faces appear [58], [59]. For example, it has been shown in several studies where faces were combined with emotional bodies that a congruent affective value of the body helps the identification of the facial expression [60], [61]. An important difference between these studies and the present study is that in our study the face gains affective value in a learning procedure through repeated association with affective gestures, whereas in the studies mentioned above the faces themselves were inherently affective (e.g., angry or happy facial expressions), but this recognition of this affective value was facilitated by congruent affective body postures. Nevertheless, both lines of research point to the notion that the affective value of a face is influenced by contextual factors such as concurrent affective body postures or learned associations between faces and affective gestures.
It also has to be noted that the faces paired with positive affective gestures also showed slightly higher electrocortical signal amplitudes compared to neutral faces, albeit non-significant. This effect which may have missed statistical significance due to statistical power, possibly indicates that the observed contextual modulation of face perception may not be entirely exclusive to negative USs, but that faces associated with positive outcomes may also attract more attention compared to neutrally associated faces. In accordance with this notion, the positive hand gesture has consistently been shown to also receive priority processing which, however, is considerably reduced as compared to the higher arousing negative middle finger gesture [17], [19].
Conclusion
The current study introduces a social conditioning paradigm incorporating socially relevant US of nonverbal affective gestures. The ssVEP in response to the CS faces as well as subjective ratings indicate that faces combined with aversive hand gestures (raised middle finger) are perceived as more negative and arousing, which is also accompanied by elevated visuocortical processing. Such results highlight the importance of using ecologically valid US in conditioning when social learning processes in impression formation are the main area of interest. Moreover, the current paradigm offers a potential means for the study of social learning and its modulation in psychiatric disorders with deficits in social information processing such as in social anxiety disorder and autism.
Data Availability
The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, FOR 605, Wi2714/3-2; Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, SFB-TRR 58, project B05; This publication was funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the University of Wuerzburg in the funding programme Open Access Publishing. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Alvarez RP, Chen G, Bodurka J, Kaplan R, Grillon C (2011) Phasic and sustained fear in humans elicits distinct patterns of brain activity. Neuroimage 55: 389–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Andreatta M, Fendt M, Muhlberger A, Wieser MJ, Imobersteg S, et al. (2012) Onset and offset of aversive events establish distinct memories requiring fear and reward networks. Learn Mem 19: 518–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Kalisch R, Korenfeld E, Stephan KE, Weiskopf N, Seymour B, et al. (2006) Context-Dependent Human Extinction Memory is Mediated by a Ventromedial Prefrontal and Hippocampal Network. The Journal of Neuroscience 26: 9503–9511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Knight DC, Cheng DT, Smith CN, Stein EA, Helmstetter FJ (2004) Neural Substrates Mediating Human Delay and Trace Fear Conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience 24: 218–228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Phelps EA, Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE (2004) Extinction learning in humans: role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron 43: 897–905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Büchel C, Dolan RJ (2000) Classical fear conditioning in functional neuroimaging. Current opinion in neurobiology 10: 219–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Büchel C, Dolan RJ, Armony JL, Friston KJ (1999) Amygdala-hippocampal involvement in human aversive trace conditioning revealed through event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience 19: 10869–10876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Dunsmoor JE, Bandettini PA, Knight DC (2008) Neural correlates of unconditioned response diminution during Pavlovian conditioning. Neuroimage 40: 811–817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Miskovic V, Keil A (2013) Visuocortical changes during delay and trace aversive conditioning: Evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials. Emotion 13: 554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Miskovic V, Keil A (2013) Perceiving Threat In the Face of Safety: Excitation and Inhibition of Conditioned Fear in Human Visual Cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience 33: 72–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Gottfried JA, Dolan RJ (2004) Human orbitofrontal cortex mediates extinction learning while accessing conditioned representations of value. Nature neuroscience 7: 1144–1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Nitschke JB, Sarinopoulos I, Mackiewicz KL, Schaefer HS, Davidson RJ (2006) Functional neuroanatomy of aversion and its anticipation. Neuroimage 29: 106–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Davis FC, Johnstone T, Mazzulla EC, Oler JA, Whalen PJ (2010) Regional response differences across the human amygdaloid complex during social conditioning. Cerebral Cortex 20: 612–621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Iidaka T, Saito DN, Komeda H, Mano Y, Kanayama N, et al. (2010) Transient Neural Activation in Human Amygdala Involved in Aversive Conditioning of Face and Voice. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22: 2074–2085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Lissek S, Levenson J, Biggs AL, Johnson LL, Ameli R, et al. (2008) Elevated fear conditioning to socially relevant unconditioned stimuli in social anxiety disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 165: 124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Pejic T, Hermann A, Vaitl D, Stark R (2013) Social anxiety modulates amygdala activation during social conditioning. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 8: 267–276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Flaisch T, Hacker F, Renner B, Schupp HT (2011) Emotion and the processing of symbolic gestures: An event-related brain potential study. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 6: 109–118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Flaisch T, Schupp HT (2013) Tracing the time course of emotion perception: the impact of stimulus physics and semantics on gesture processing. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 8: 820–827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Flaisch T, Schupp HT, Renner B, Junghofer M (2009) Neural systems of visual attention responding to emotional gestures. Neuroimage 45: 1339–1346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Regan D (1989) Human Brain Electrophysiology: Evoked Potentials and Evoked Magnetic Fields in Science and Medicine. New York: Elsevier.
- 21.Vialatte F-B, Maurice M, Dauwels J, Cichocki A (2010) Steady-state visually evoked potentials: Focus on essential paradigms and future perspectives. Progress in Neurobiology In Press, Corrected Proof. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 22. Wang J, Clementz BA, Keil A (2007) The neural correlates of feature-based selective attention when viewing spatially and temporally overlapping images. Neuropsychologia 45: 1393–1399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nunez PL, Srinivasan R (2006) Electric fields of the brain: The neurophysics of EEG. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- 24. Keil A, Gruber T, Müller MM, Moratti S, Stolarova M, et al. (2003) Early modulation of visual perception by emotional arousal: evidence from steady-state visual evoked brain potentials. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 3: 195–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Andersen SK, Müller MM (2010) Behavioral performance follows the time course of neural facilitation and suppression during cued shifts of feature-selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107: 13878–13882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Müller MM, Teder-Salejarvi W, Hillyard SA (1998) The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience 1: 631–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Müller MM, Teder W, Hillyard SA (1997) Magnetoencephalographic recording of steady-state visual evoked cortical activity. Brain Topography 9: 163–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Wieser MJ, Keil A (2011) Temporal Trade-Off Effects in Sustained Attention: Dynamics in Visual Cortex Predict the Target Detection Performance during Distraction. The Journal of Neuroscience 31: 7784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Di Russo F, Pitzalis S, Aprile T, Spitoni G, Patria F, et al. (2007) Spatiotemporal analysis of the cortical sources of the steady-state visual evoked potential. Human Brain Mapping 28: 323–334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Keil A, Miskovic V, Gray MJ, Martinovic J (2013) Luminance, but not chromatic visual pathways, mediate amplification of conditioned danger signals in human visual cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience 38: 3356–3362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Moratti S, Keil A (2005) Cortical activation during Pavlovian fear conditioning depends on heart rate response patterns: an MEG study. Cognitive Brain Research 25: 459–471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Moratti S, Keil A, Miller GA (2006) Fear but not awareness predicts enhanced sensory processing in fear conditioning. Psychophysiology 43: 216–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Langner O, Dotsch R, Bijlstra G, Wigboldus DHJ, Hawk ST, et al. (2010) Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database. Cognition and Emotion 24: 1377–1388. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Kret ME, De Gelder B (2012) A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia 50: 1211–1221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1994) Measuring emotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 25: 49–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Peyk P, De Cesarei A, Junghöfer M (2011) Electro Magneto Encephalography Software: overview and integration with other EEG/MEG toolboxes. Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2011: Article ID 861705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 37. Junghöfer M, Elbert T, Tucker DM, Rockstroh B (2000) Statistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. Psychophysiology 37: 523–532. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Kiebel SJ, Tallon-Baudry C, Friston KJ (2005) Parametric analysis of oscillatory activity as measured with EEG/MEG. Human Brain Mapping 26: 170–177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gruss LF, Wieser MJ, Schweinberger S, Keil A (2012) Face-evoked steady-state visual potentials: effects of presentation rate and face inversion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. McTeague LM, Shumen JR, Wieser MJ, Lang PJ, Keil A (2011) Social vision: Sustained perceptual enhancement of affective facial cues in social anxiety. Neuroimage 54: 1615–1624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Miskovic V, Keil A (2014) Escape from harm: Linking affective vision and motor responses during active avoidance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience: doi:10.1093/scan/nsu1013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 42. Wieser MJ, McTeague LM, Keil A (2012) Competition effects of threatening faces in social anxiety. Emotion 12: 1050–1060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Picton TW, Bentin S, Berg P, Donchin E, Hillyard SA, et al. (2000) Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology 37: 127–152. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Miskovic V, Keil A (2012) Acquired fears reflected in cortical sensory processing: A review of electrophysiological studies of human classical conditioning. Psychophysiology 49: 1230–1241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Keil A, Stolarova M, Moratti S, Ray WJ (2007) Adaptation in human visual cortex as a mechanism for rapid discrimination of aversive stimuli. Neuroimage 36: 472–479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Quirk GJ, Armony JL, LeDoux JE (1997) Fear conditioning enhances different temporal components of tone-evoked spike trains in auditory cortex and lateral amygdala. Neuron 19: 613–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Weinberger NM (2011) The medial geniculate, not the amygdala, as the root of auditory fear conditioning. Hear Res 274: 61–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Freese JL, Amaral DG (2009) Neuroanatomy of the primate amygdala. In: Whalen PJ, Phelps EA, editors. The human amygdala. New York, NY: Guilford Press. pp. 3–42. [Google Scholar]
- 49. Armony JL, Quirk GJ, LeDoux JE (1998) Differential effects of amygdala lesions on early and late plastic components of auditory cortex spike trains during fear conditioning. J Neurosci 18: 2592–2601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Lang PJ, Bradley MM (2010) Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological psychology 84: 437–450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Vuilleumier P (2005) How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 585–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Öhman A (2009) Of snakes and faces: An evolutionary perspective on the psychology of fear. Scandinavian journal of psychology 50: 543–552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W (2001) Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 704–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Gilbert CD, Sigman M (2007) Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron 54: 677–696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Mineka S, Oehlberg K (2008) The relevance of recent developments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Acta psychologica 127: 567–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. De Houwer J, Thomas S, Baeyens F (2001) Association learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin 127: 853–869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Hofmann W, De Houwer J, Perugini M, Baeyens F, Crombez G (2010) Evaluative conditioning in humans: a meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin 136: 390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Hassin RR, Aviezer H, Bentin S (2013) Inherently ambiguous: Facial expressions of emotions, in context. Emotion Review 5: 60–65. [Google Scholar]
- 59. Wieser MJ, Brosch T (2012) Faces in context: A review and systematization of contextual influences on affective face processing. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Aviezer H, Trope Y, Todorov A (2012) Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science 338: 1225–1229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kret ME, Stekelenburg JJ, Roelofs K, De Gelder B (2013) Perception of face and body expressions using electromyography, pupillometry and gaze measures. Frontiers in psychology 4.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.