Abstract
Mice are emerging as an important behavioral model for studies of auditory perception and acoustic communication. These mammals frequently produce ultrasonic vocalizations, although the details of how these vocalizations are used for communication are not entirely understood. An important step in determining how they might be differentiating their calls is to measure discrimination and identification of the dimensions of various acoustic stimuli. Here, behavioral operant conditioning methods were employed to assess frequency difference limens for pure tones. We found that their thresholds were similar to those in other rodents but higher than in humans. We also asked mice, in an identification paradigm, whether they would use frequency or duration differences to classify stimuli varying on those two dimensions. We found that the mice classified the stimuli based on frequency rather than duration.
Keywords: Cue trading, Frequency difference limens, CBA/CaJ mice, psychoacoustics, mouse ultrasonic vocalizations, operant conditioning
1. Introduction
Researchers have long known that mice produce various types of sonic and ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs; Whitney et al., 1973). However, the abilities of mice to discriminate vocalizations have only recently been investigated. Neilans et al. (2014) found that CBA/CaJ mice discriminate USVs well above chance, and that discrimination performance declines as spectrotemporal similarity between the vocalizations increases. The mice could have used frequency, duration, or any combination of auditory dimensions to distinguish the vocalizations. Therefore, we sought to add to the psychophysical literature on the CBA/CaJ mouse strain (May et al., 2006; Radziwon et al., 2009), a mouse known to retain its hearing into middle age, making it a good model for hearing and acoustic communication (Zheng et al., 1999).
We first measured simple frequency difference limens for pure tones. We used a modified go/no-go paradigm where the mice were required to discriminate a change in frequency between a repeating background reference tone and various target tones of higher frequencies. The purpose of this experiment was to determine whether the CBA/CaJ mouse could resolve high frequencies adequately enough to use frequency to discriminate between vocalizations.
In a second condition, we tested the mice in a novel cue trading study. The cue trading condition made use of an identification paradigm where the mice placed tones into two categories. For instance, if a mouse heard a 70 kHz tone with a 25 ms duration it made one response, and if it heard a 30 kHz tone with a 120 ms duration, it made a different response. Once the mouse was able to identify these two categories, a 70 kHz tone with a 120 ms duration was presented. If the mouse used frequency to identify the tones then it made the first response; however, if duration was more important, then it made the second response. Using this type of controlled paradigm, it was possible to determine whether frequency or duration was the more salient feature for mice when differentiating tones, potentially clarifying the feature(s) the mice use when identifying USVs.
2. Methods
2.1 Subjects
Three CBA/CaJ mice were used in the FDL condition, and four CBA/CaJ mice were used in the cue trading condition. Subjects began training at three months of age (sexually mature) and were tested until 12 months of age. They were bred at the University at Buffalo, SUNY and all procedures were approved by the University at Buffalo, SUNY’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. All mice were housed separately and were kept on a reverse day/night cycle. They were water restricted and kept at approximately 85% of their free-drinking weight.
2.2 Apparatus, procedure, and data analysis for FDL condition
2.2.1 Apparatus
The mice were tested in a cage containing an ultrasonic speaker, a response dipper, and nose poke holes surrounded by infrared sensors. TDT’s RPvds software and a MATLAB interface were used to control the hardware and monitor the animals’ responses (see Neilans et al., 2014 for more detail).
2.2.2 Procedure
The test stimuli used in this condition were 500 ms pure tones with cosine rise/fall times of 20 ms. The frequencies tested were 12, 16, 24, and 42 kHz presented at 10 and 30 dB sensation level (SL), as determined by a previously obtained audiogram (Radziwon et al., 2009), and roved by ±3 dB between presentations to eliminate level cues. The mice were trained using a go/no-go operant conditioning procedure to discriminate a higher-frequency comparison tone (target) from a reference tone (repeating background).
The mouse began a trial by nose poking the observation nose-poke hole, which initiated a variable waiting interval of 1 to 4 s. During this time, the reference tone played at a rate of 1 tone every 700 ms. After the waiting interval, a single target was presented. In the go condition, the target was the comparison tone. If the mouse detected a change between the reference and target, it poked the report nose-poke hole. In this trial type, a hit was recorded if the mouse correctly responded to the comparison tone within 2 s. The animal received a reinforcement of 0.01 ml of water (morning session) or Ensure® (afternoon). A miss was recorded if the mouse either failed to poke the report hole or if it did not respond within 2 s.
Thirty percent of all trials were catch trials, where the repeating background continued through the response interval. These constituted the no-go part of the procedure. If the mouse poked the report hole during a catch trial, a false alarm was recorded and the mouse received a 20 s timeout. However, if the subject continued to nose-poke to the observation nose-poke hole, a correct rejection was recorded. No reinforcement was given for a correct rejection but the next trial began immediately. Sessions were excluded from analysis if the percentage of false alarms was greater than 20%.
Targets were presented according to the psychophysical Method of Constant Stimuli, where stimuli both above and below the presumed threshold were presented to the subjects in a random order in blocks of ten trials (7 targets and 3 catch trials).
2.2.3 Data analysis
Mean hit and false alarm rates were used to calculate thresholds. Thresholds for each frequency were calculated using the last 200 trials out of at least 400 trials. If thresholds varied by more than 3% Weber fraction between the first 200 and last 200 trials, testing continued until thresholds stabilized.
2.3 Apparatus and procedure for cue trading condition
2.3.1 Apparatus
The apparatus for this study is identical to the first study except there are three nose pokes instead of two.
2.3.2 Procedure
The test stimuli were 30 or 70 kHz pure tones, with 25 or 120 ms durations. These features were chosen because they were within the range of naturally-produced USVs (Neilans et al., 2014) and because they were easily discriminable (Ehret, 1975; Klink and Klump, 2004). All tones had cosine rise/fall times of 5 ms, were presented at 30 dB SL, and were roved ±3 dB to eliminate level cues (rise/fall times were shorter in this study because the tone durations were much shorter). After the mice reached 85% correct identification performance for at least 100 trials, untrained probe stimuli were added. The probe stimuli tested whether the mice were using frequency or duration to classify the endpoints. For instance, if the mice were trained with the 70 kHz/25 ms and 30 kHz/120 ms endpoints, then the probes were the 70 kHz/120 ms and 30 kHz/25 ms tones.
As in the first study, the mice were trained to nose poke to initiate trials and make responses to the stimuli. If the mouse made a correct response, a hit was recorded and the animal received either water or Ensure®. A miss was recorded if the mouse poked to the incorrect hole, resulting in a 25 s timeout. During testing, the untrained probe stimuli were randomly interspersed with the endpoint stimuli on 20% of trials. The responses to the probe stimuli were always rewarded regardless of how the mouse responded. The mice received 20 presentations of each probe stimulus, and their responses were counted to determine whether they were categorizing the probes based on frequency or duration.
3. Results and Discussion
3.1 Frequency difference limens
The FDLs (Figure 1) for these mice were similar to those found in other rodents, but were higher than those found in humans and cats (~200 Hz higher at lowest frequencies; Heffner et al., 1971). In general, thresholds increased with increasing frequency. Their FDLs, though unsurprising given the known physiological (Saunders and Crumling, 2001) and psychophysical (Ehret, 1975; May et al., 2006) data, were useful in determining which features the mice can use when discriminating conspecific USVs. Since their thresholds fell within the frequency boundaries of pup and adult USVs (Grimsley et al., 2011), our results suggest that frequency can be used to identify pups from adults. However, more research is needed to determine whether mice actually differentiate calls based upon frequency alone.
3.2 Cue trading
For the first time here, mice were trained on an auditory identification task. The mice identified the two endpoint stimuli with at least 87% accuracy. Further, every mouse classified the untrained probes based on frequency instead of duration (Figure 2). Responses to the probes were not different than the responses to the endpoint stimuli [t(6)=1.17, p=0.29, Cohen’s d (effect size)=0.83], suggesting that the mice were only using frequency to classify the stimuli.
Mouse USVs, like other complex stimuli, differ by more than frequency, and to understand mouse communication, researchers must take into account their perceptual strategies. Therefore, cue trading experiments like this one are useful in examining interactions among auditory dimensions (Melara and Marks, 1990). Given that duration and frequency differences were well above threshold, the mice could have used either dimension to classify the probes (Klink and Klump, 2004) but they mainly chose frequency. These results highlight the reliance the mice have on frequency, and suggest that frequency can be the dominant feature mice use to discriminate among various auditory stimuli, including, possibly, vocalizations.
HIGHLIGHTS.
Frequency difference limens were measured in CBA/CaJ mice
Frequency difference limens were similar to those in other rodents
Mice were also trained, for the first time, on an auditory identification cue trading task
Mice overwhelmingly chose frequency over duration during identification
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Ehret G. Frequency and intensity difference limens and nonlinearities in the ear of the housemouse (Mus musculus) Journal of Comparative Physiology. 1975;102:321–336. [Google Scholar]
- Grimsley JMS, Monaghan JJM, Wenstrup JJ. Development of social vocalizations in mice. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(3):e17460. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heffner R, Heffner H, Masterson B. Behavioral measurements of absolute and frequency-difference thresholds in guinea pig. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1971;49:1888–1895. doi: 10.1121/1.1912596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klink KB, Klump GM. Duration discrimination in the mouse (Mus musculus) Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2004;190:1039–1046. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0561-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May BJ, Kimar S, Prosen CA. Auditory filter shapes of CBA/CaJ mice: Behavioral assessments. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2006;120:321–330. doi: 10.1121/1.2203593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melara RD, Marks LE. Interaction among auditory dimensions: Timbre, pitch, and loudness. Perception & Psychophysics. 1990;48:169–178. doi: 10.3758/bf03207084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neilans EG, Holfoth DP, Radziwon KE, Portfors CV, Dent ML. Discrimination of ultrasonic vocalizations by CBA/CaJ mice (Mus musculus) is related to spectrotemporal dissimilarity of vocalizations. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e85405. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0085405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radziwon KE, June KM, Stolzberg DJ, Xu-Friedman MA, Salvi RJ, Dent ML. Behaviorally measured audiograms and gap detection thresholds in CBA/CaJ mice. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 2009;195:961–969. doi: 10.1007/s00359-009-0472-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders JC, Crumling MA. The outer and middle ear. In: Willott JF, editor. Handbook of mouse auditory research. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2001. pp. 99–115. [Google Scholar]
- Whitney G, Coble JR, Stockton MD, Tilson EF. Ultrasonic emissions: Do they facilitate courtship of mice? Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1973;84:445–452. doi: 10.1037/h0034899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng QY, Johnson KR, Erway LC. Assessment of hearing in 80 inbred strains of mice by ABR threshold analyses. Hearing Research. 1999;130:94–107. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(99)00003-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]