Summary
The role that genes play in human intelligence or IQ has remained a point of significant scientific debate dating back to the time of Galton [1]. It has now become increasingly clear that IQ is heritable in humans but these effects can be modified by non-genetic mechanisms [2-4]. In contrast to human IQ, until recently, views of learning and cognition in animals have largely been dominated by the behaviorist school of thought, originally championed by Watson [5] and Skinner [6]. A large body of research has now accumulated demonstrating a variety of cognitive abilities in nonhuman animals that challenge traditional behaviorist interpretations of performance [7, 8]. This, in turn, has lead to a renewed interest in the role that social and biological factors might play in explaining individual and phylogenetic differences in cognition[9]. Specifically, aside from early attempts to selectively breed for learning skills in rodents [10-12], studies examining the role that genetic factors might play in individual variation in cognitive abilities in nonhuman animals, particularly nonhuman primates, are scarce. Here, we utilized a modified Primate Cognitive Test Battery [13] in conjunction with quantitative genetic analyses to examine whether cognitive performance is heritable in chimpanzees. We found that some but not all cognitive traits were significantly heritable in chimpanzees. We further found significant genetic correlations between different dimensions of cognitive functioning, suggesting that the same genes may explain their variability.
Results and Discussion
Principal Components Analysis
Cognitive performance was assessed on 13 tasks from the Primate Cognition Test Battery (PCTB) task originally developed by Herrmann and colleagues [13, 14]. The 13 tasks are designed to assess a variety of cognitive abilities, broadly defined as non-social and social cognition. To assess the structure and heritability in cognitive performance in the chimpanzees, we performed principal component analysis (PCA) on their accuracy for individual PCTB tasks. PCA allowed us to derive unbiased component performance constructs based on item loadings of the different tasks. Component scores with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 was considered significant and item-component coefficients greater than .55 (absolute value) were judged as salient items. We also computed a single measure of cognitive performance by deriving a composite factor score using the first unrotated component from a separate PCA analysis (referred to as the “g” factor). Descriptive data and heritability analyses of the raw performance data can be found Table S1 and Figure S1.
The PCA with varimax rotation revealed four components with eigenvalues >1.0 and they accounted for 54.20 percent of variance (Table 1). Performance on the spatial memory, object permanence, rotation and transposition tasks loaded on component 1. Causality-visual and tool use loaded on component 2 while communication production, attention state and gaze following loaded on component 3. Finally, causality-noise was the single task to load on component 4. Each of the four significant component scores was saved and we compared these scores between sexes and rearing groups using MANOVA. No significant main effects or interactions were found between sex and rearing conditions on the component scores (Table S1).
Table 1.
Item | C1 | C2 | C3 | C4 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Eigenvalue | 3.066 | 1.513 | 1.404 | 1.063 |
Percent Variance | 23.59 | 11.68 | 10.80 | 8.18 |
Spatial Memory | .648 | −.473 | −.014 | .029 |
Object Permanence | .621 | .243 | .265 | .148 |
Rotation | .665 | .157 | −.088 | −.002 |
Transposition | .729 | .046 | −.027 | .102 |
Relative Numbers | .260 | .392 | .122 | .432 |
Causality-Noise | .038 | .084 | −.008 | .776 |
Causality-Visual | −.064 | .711 | −.062 | .114 |
Tool Use | .218 | .667 | .224 | .063 |
Tool Properties | .265 | .449 | −.479 | .040 |
Comprehension | .424 | .400 | .339 | −.356 |
Production | .458 | .095 | .587 | .150 |
Attention State | .173 | .084 | .669 | .397 |
Gaze Following | −.163 | .062 | .584 | −.239 |
Bolded items indicated significant item loading. C = component
Quantitative Genetics
As has been done in previous studies in primates [15, 16], we used the program SOLAR (Sequential Oligogenic Linkage Analysis Routines) to estimate heritability [17]. The overall “g” factor score as well the scores for each of the four components derived from the PCA served as the variables of interest in the heritability analyses. Age, sex and rearing history served as covariates. Significant heritability was found for the overall “g” factor score as well as components 1 and 3 but not 2 and 4 (see Figure 1). Recall that the four tasks spatial memory, object permanence, rotation and transposition loaded on component 1 while communication production, attention state and gaze following loaded on component 3 (see Table 1). None of the covariates accounted for a significant proportion of variance in the PCA components.
Genetic Correlations
To determine the extent to which any two traits may have the same set of possible genes that account for their variation, we performed genetic correlations between the component 1 and 3 scores. This analysis revealed a significant genetic correlation between these two components (rg = .992, s.e. = .522, p < .05) suggesting that the same set of genes explains variability in performance on the tasks loading onto components 1 and 3 (see Table 1).
PCA analysis with varimax rotation of performance measures on 13 cognitive tasks revealed four factors. Two of these components (1 & 3), as well as the overall “g” factor were found to be significantly heritable. Significant genetic correlations were found between the factor 1 and factor 3, suggesting that common genes may possibly underlie their heritability. Lastly, neither sex nor rearing history accounted for a significant proportion of variance in cognitive performance.
A significant proportion of variance in overall chimpanzee cognitive performance was found to be heritable. The overall “g” factor and two (1 & 3) of the four components were significantly heritable, suggesting that genetic factors contribute to individual differences in chimpanzee cognition. The proportion of variance accounted for by genetic factors was moderate to large [18] whereas non-genetic factors including sex and rearing history were not significant. Furthermore, for the spatial cognition and communication components, significant genetic correlations were found, suggesting that common genes may explain individual differences in performance on these two measures.
Though we found significant genetic correlations between the communication and spatial cognition components, it is important to emphasize that the overall “g” factor score was also significantly heritable. Thus, even though some cognitive constructs were heritable (component 1 & 3) and others not (components 2 & 4), we believe the general findings reported here reflect heritability in overall cognitive performance rather than distinct separate, aptitudes for two reasons. First, though we used eigenvalues > 1.0 to derive specific components, examination of the spree plot does not show a sharp drop off between any of the components, which is more commonly observed when robust distinct components are evident from PCA. Further, it has been suggested that using eigenvalues > 1.0 as the criteria for determining relevance in PCA can lead to over-extraction [19]. Parallel analysis has been proposed as a solution to this problem [20], which evaluates what minimum eigenvalues are needed to reject the null hypothesis when adjusted for sample size and the number of tasks/items. When applied to this study, only component 1 had an eigenvalue that would be considered significant. Second, the composite scores for the individual components were positively and significantly correlated with the first unrotated factor (or “g” factor score). Thus, individual differences in the derived “g” factor score correlated with the component 1 (r = .771, p < .001), 2 (r = .457, p < .001), 3 (r = .363, p < .001) and 4 (r = .256, p < .02) scores, suggesting substantial overlap in the underlying or latent cognitive ability [21].
In terms of the structure of the performance measures on the PCTB tasks, the PCA findings are not entirely consistent with the a priori structure originally proposed by Herrmann et al. [13]. In previous studies in human children, chimpanzees and orangutans, the individual tasks comprising the PCTB were broadly defined into a two -construct structure including non-social and social cognition [13]. The non-social cognition construct was further broken down into three sub-components including spatial cognition, understanding causality and number discrimination whereas social cognition was divided into two constructs, communication and theory-of-mind [13]. The two-construct structure of PCTB performance in chimpanzees (i.e., non-social versus social) has been validated using confirmatory factor analysis [22] but the 5-construct structure has not found and was likewise not demonstrated in this study. Thus, the a priori structure of social and non-social cognition as measured by the PCTB task does not appear to be entirely valid, at least based on the data and PCA analysis used in this study.
There are at least two limitations of this study. First, although this is one of the largest studies of cognition performed on chimpanzees, compared to other quantitative genetic studies in humans, it was relatively small. A replication of this study in a larger cohort of chimpanzees would be useful and allow for increased statistical power. One advantage of the PCTB is that it is relatively simple to administer and therefore data could be obtained from a larger sample of apes without too much effort and expense. Nonetheless, we do believe the findings presented here are stable and valid. For instance, over a two-year period, we re-tested 86 of the original 99 chimpanzees in this study on the 13 PCTB tasks. For the most part, performance was stable over time though the chimpanzees did significantly better on the re-test of the object permanence, rotation, transposition and relative number tasks whereas performance was significantly lower for gaze following (Table S2). A PCA on the re-test data constraining the number of components to 4 revealed a similar pattern of item loadings as was found in the original analysis (Table S3). The only differences in the item loadings for the components between tests were that gaze following loaded on component 4 instead of 3 and the visual-causality task failed to load on any component. Importantly, heritability for the “g-factor” based on the re-test data was significant (h2 = .624, s.e. = .242, p < .005) and quite similar to the values from the original analysis (see Figure 1). Thus, within our sample, the structure and heritability in cognitive performance was consistent over time.
Second, it is important to recognize that the findings of this study are limited to a discussion of heritability in cognition for a specific set of tasks assessed at a given point in time. We did not measure the acquisition and learning of the tasks comprising the PCTB; therefore we are not estimating heritability in chimpanzee learning abilities per se. To estimate heritability in learning ability would require assessment in the acquisition of novel problem solving tasks, as has been reported in mice [23]. This could be a novel and alternative approach to comparative heritability studies of cognition in human and nonhuman primates in the future.
Finally, from an evolutionary standpoint, the results reported here suggest that genetic factors play a significant role in determining individual variation in cognitive abilities, particularly for spatial cognition and communication skills. Presumably these attributes would have conferred advantages to some individuals potentially in the way of enhanced foraging skills or increase social skills, leading to increased opportunities for access to food or mating [24, 25]. These individuals would have then potentially had increased survival and fitness, traits that would have become increasingly selected upon during primate evolution, as has been postulated by a number of theorist going all the way back to Darwin [26-30].
Experimental Procedures
Subjects
There were 99 chimpanzees in the study including 29 males and 70 females. Subjects ranged in age from 9 to 54 years (Mean = 24.55, sd = 10.67). Ninety-five of the subjects were residing at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center (YNPRC) of Emory University and four were housed at the Language Research Center (LRC) of Georgia State University. Within the sample, there were 44 mother-reared (35 female, 9 male), 43 human-reared (24 female 19 male) and 12 wild- caught individuals (11 female, 1 male). The 40 human-reared YNPRC chimpanzees were separated from their mothers within the first 30 days of life, due to unresponsive care, injury, or illness [31, 32]. These chimpanzees were placed in incubators, fed standard human infant formula (non-supplemented), and cared for by humans until they could sufficiently care for themselves, at which time they were placed with other infants of the same age until they were 3 years of age [31, 32]. At 3 years of age, human-reared chimpanzees were integrated into larger social groups of adult and sub-adult chimpanzees. The rearing of the three human-raised LRC chimpanzees has been described extensively elsewhere [33-37]. Mother-reared chimpanzees were not separated from their mother for at least 2.5 years of life and were raised in nuclear family groups ranging from 4 to 20 individuals. Wild-born chimpanzees were individuals who had been captured in the wild and subsequently brought to research facilities within the United States prior to 1974, when the importation of chimpanzees was banned. Within the mother-reared cohort of 44 chimpanzees, offspring from 29 different females were represented while the 43 human-reared offspring were born to 30 different females. Thus, the range in genetic variation, at least from the standpoint of the dams, was comparable between the cohorts. The average related coefficient of the sample was .0178 and did not differ between mother- (Mean = .017) and human-reared (Mean = .0185) individuals t(97)=-1.18, p = .240. All procedures used with the chimpanzees were approved by the local Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Procedures
Subjects were tested on a modified version of the primate cognition test battery (PCTB) originally developed by Hermann et al. [13, 14] and described elsewhere [38]. The PCTB attempts to assess subjects’ abilities in various areas of non-social and social cognition. The previously published procedures were followed as closely as possible but some tasks were modified to better address the questions at hand given the past experience and environmental constraints of our subjects. The nine non-social and four social cognition tasks are described in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures (S1) with notes made when procedures were altered from those described by Herrmann et al. [13]. Testing was completed over 3 to 5 testing sessions, depending on the motivation and attention of the subject.
Quantitative Genetic Analysis
Heritability (h2) is the proportion of total phenotypic variance that is attributable to all genetic sources. Total phenotypic variance is constrained to a value of 1; therefore, all non-genetic contributions to the phenotype are equal to 1 - h2. The analytic approach we took takes into consideration all relationships within a sample and allows for an analysis of heritability using quantitative genetics based on the entire pedigree. To estimate heritability in PCTB performance, we used the software package SOLAR [17] which uses a variance components approach to estimate the polygenic component of variance when considering the entire pedigree. SOLAR has been previously used to estimate heritability in various behavioral and temperament traits as well as different aspects of cortical organization in extended pedigrees of baboons, vervet and rhesus monkeys [see 39, 40-44].
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Individual differences in chimpanzee cognitive performance are heritable
Cognitive traits found to be heritable show significant genetic correlations
Sex and rearing history do not significantly influence cognitive performance
Acknowledgement
This research was supported by NIH grants MH-92923, NS-42867, NS- 73134 and HD-60563 to WDH and National Center for Research Resources P51RR165 to YNPRC, which is currently supported by the Office of Research Infrastructure Programs/OD P51OD11132). We appreciate the helpful comments of the four reviewers and Dr. Alex Weiss for bringing to our attention the use of parallel analysis in determining the number of components to extract using PCA. Inquiries regarding this paper may be sent to: William D. Hopkins, Neuroscience Institute and Language Research Center, Georgia State University, P.O. Box 5030, Atlanta, Georgia 30302-5030. whopkins4@gsu.edu or whopkin@emory.edu
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Galton F. Hereditary Genius. Macmillan; London: 1869. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D’Onofrio B, Gottesman II. Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science. 2003;14:623–628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bouchard TJ. Genetic influences on human psychological traits: A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2004;13:148–151. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bates TC, Lwewis G, Weiss A. High childhood socioeconomic status amplifies genetic effects on adult intelligence. Psychological Science. 2013;24:2111–2116. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Watson JB. Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review. 1913;20:158–177. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Skinner BF. About Behaviorism. Vol. 1974. Random House; New York: [Google Scholar]
- 7.Roitblat HL, Bever TG, Terrace HS, editors. Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Nillsdale, N.J.: 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tomasello M, Call J. Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Leavens DA, Bard KA. Environmental influences on joint attention in great apes: Implications for human cognition Jounrnal of Cognitive Education and Psychology. 2011;10:9–31. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Tolman EC. The inheritance of maze-learning ability in rats. Journal of Compartive Psychology. 1924;4:1–18. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Heron WT. The inheritance of maze learning in rats. J. Comp. Psychol. 1935;19:77–89. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Tyron RC. Genetic differences in maze learning ability of rats. Yearboook of the National Society for the Study of Education. 1940;39 [Google Scholar]
- 13.Herrmann E, Call J, Hernandez-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M. Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis Science. 2007;317:1360–1366. doi: 10.1126/science.1146282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Herrmann E, Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M. Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. PLos One. 2010;5:e12438. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Phillips KA, Rogers J, Barrett EA, Glahn DC, Kochunov PV. Genetic contributions to the midsagittal area of the corpus callosum Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2012;15:315–323. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kochunov PV, Glahn DC, Fox PT, Lancaster JL, Saleem KS, Shelledy W, Zilles K, Thompson PM, Coulon O, Mangin JF, et al. Genetics of primary cerebral gyrification: Heritability of length, depth and area of primary sulci in an extended pedigree of Papio baboons. Neuroimage. 2010;53:1126–1134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Almasy L, Blangero J. Multipoint quantitative-trait linkage analysis in general pedigrees. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 1998;62:1198–1211. doi: 10.1086/301844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Falconer DS. Introduction ot Quantitative Genetics. Wiley and Sons; New York: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gorsuch RL. Factor analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Horn JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometricka. 1965;30:179–185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Deary IJ, Penke L, Johnson W. The neuroscience of human intellligence differences. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:201–211. doi: 10.1038/nrn2793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Herrmann E, Hernandez-Lloreda MV, Call J, Hare B, Tomasello M. The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science. 2010;21:102–110. doi: 10.1177/0956797609356511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kolata S, Light K, Matzel LD. Domain-specific and domain-general learing factors are expressed in genetically hterogeneous CD-1 mice. Intelligence. 2008;36:619–629. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.King BJ. Extractive foraging and the evolution of primate intelligence. Human Evolution. 1986;1:361–372. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hopkins WD, Keebaugh AC, Reamer LA, Schaeffer J, Schapiro SJ, Young LJ. Genetic influences on receptive joint attention in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Scientific Reports. 2014;4:1–7. doi: 10.1038/srep03774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Dunbar RIM, Schultz S. Evolution of the social brain Science. 2007;317:1344–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.1145463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fisher SE, Marcus GF. The eloquent ape: genes, brains and the evolution of language. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2006;7:9–20. doi: 10.1038/nrg1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Deaner RO, Isler K, Burkart J, Van Schaik CP. Overall brain size, and not encephalization quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2007;70:115–124. doi: 10.1159/000102973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Reader SM, Laland KN. Social intelligence, innnovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2002;99:4436–4441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.062041299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Darwin C. Origin of Species. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1859. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bard KA, Platzman KA, Lester BM, Suomi SJ. Orientation to social and nonsocial stimuli in neonatal chimpanzees and humans. Infant Behavior and Development. 1992;15:43–56. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bard KA. Evolutionary roots of intuitive parenting: Maternal competence in chimpanzees. Early Development and Parenting. 1994;3:19–28. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Brakke KE, Savage-Rumbaugh ES. The development of language skills in bonobo and chimpanzee - i. comprehension. Language and Communication. 1995;15:121–148. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brakke KE, Savage-Rumbaugh ES. The development of language skills in Pan - ii. production. Language and Communication. 1996;16:361–380. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Savage-Rumbaugh ES. Ape language: From conditioned response to symbol. Columbia University Press; New York: 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Murphy J, Sevcik RA, Brakke KE, Williams SL, Rumbaugh DM. Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1993;58:1–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Lewin R. Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human Mind. John Wiley; New York: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Russell JL, Lyn H, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD. The role of socio-communicative rearing environments in the development of social and physical cognition in apes. Developmental Science. 2011;14:1459–1470. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01090.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fears SC, Melega WP, Service SK, Lee C, Chen K, Tu Z, Jorgensen MJ, Fairbanks LA, Cantor RM, Freimer NB, et al. Identifying heritable brain phenotypes in an extended pedigree of vervet monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:2867–2875. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5153-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Fears SC, Scheibel K, Abaryan Z, Lee C, Service SK, Jorgensen MJ, Fairbanks LA, Cantor RM, Freimer NB, Woods RP. Anatomic brain asymmetry in vervet monkeys. PlosOne. 2011;6:e28243. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Rogers J, Kochunov PV, Lancaster JL, Sheeledy W, Glahn D, Blangero J, Fox PT. Heritability of brain volume, surface area and shape: An MRI study in an extended pedigree of baboons. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2007;28:576–583. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Williamson DE, Coleman K, Bacanu SA, Devlin BJ, Rogers J, Ryan ND, Cameron JL. Heritability of fearful-anxious endophenotypes in infant rhesus macaques: a preliminary report. Biol. Psychiatry. 2003;53:284–291. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01601-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Fairbanks LA, Bailey JN, Breidenthal SE, Laudenslager ML, Kaplan JR, Jorgensen MJ. Environmental stress alters genetic regulation of novelty seeking in vervet monkeys. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2011;10:683–688. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2011.00707.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rogers SE, Shelton SE, Shelledy W, Garcia R, Kalin NK. Genetic influences on behavioral inhibition and anxiety in juvenile rhesus macaques. Genes, Brain and Behavior. 2008;7:463–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00381.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.