Abstract
Musicians have been shown to better perceive pitch and timbre cues in speech and music, compared to non-musicians. It is unclear whether this “musician advantage” persists under conditions of spectro-temporal degradation, as experienced by cochlear-implant (CI) users. In this study, gender categorization was measured in normal-hearing musicians and non-musicians listening to acoustic CI simulations. Recordings of Dutch words were synthesized to systematically vary fundamental frequency, vocal-tract length, or both to create voices from the female source talker to a synthesized male talker. Results showed an overall musician effect, mainly due to musicians weighting fundamental frequency more than non-musicians in CI simulations.
I. Introduction
Identifying a talker's gender depends on two anatomically related vocal characteristics: (1) Fundamental frequency (F0), mainly related to vocal pitch, and (2) vocal-tract length (VTL), mainly related to the size of the speaker (Smith and Patterson, 2005). The ability to identify the voice of a talker is important to separate various talkers in a multi-talker environment and possibly improve speech intelligibility (Brungart, 2001). Recently Fuller et al. (2013) demonstrated that cochlear-implant (CI) users do not utilize both voice cues efficiently. Due to a diminished weighting of VTL cues and an over-reliance on F0 cues, CI users' gender categorization differs from that of normal hearing (NH) listeners, possibly leading to errors in categorization under certain conditions.
NH musicians have been shown to better understand speech in noise, better discriminate voices on the basis of timbre differences, and better perceive pitch in both speech and music, compared to non-musicians (e.g., Schon et al., 2004; Chartrand and Belin, 2006; Parbery-Clark et al., 2009). This “musician advantage” has been shown to enhance linguistic processing at brainstem, subcortical, and cortical levels, and is associated with better functional working memory and auditory attention (e.g., Besson et al., 2011). Some of the musician advantage for speech-related tasks has been attributed to a better perception of acoustical cues, such as timbre or prosody (e.g., Deguchi et al., 2012).
Based on these findings, musicians might be expected to better perceive both F0 and VTL cues compared to non-musicians. Fuller et al. (2013) showed CI users rely almost exclusively on F0 cues to categorize voice gender. NH non-musicians listening to acoustic CI simulations used both cues less efficiently, but relied more strongly on F0 cues in comparison to listening to normal acoustical stimuli. Under such conditions of spectro-temporal degradation, musicians may be better able to extract F0 and VTL information, compared to non-musicians. If so, past musical experience or active music training may benefit CI users' gender categorization that may help speech perception in noise. In this study, voice gender categorization was measured in NH musicians and non-musicians listening to unprocessed speech or to an acoustic CI simulation. Dutch words were synthesized to vary F0, VTL, or both, thereby systematically creating voices from the female source talker to a synthesized male talker. We hypothesized that musicians would be better able to utilize the F0 and VTL cues in a gender categorization task than non-musicians, especially with the CI simulation.
II. Materials and methods
A. Participants
Twenty-five NH musicians and 25 NH non-musicians were recruited for this study. “Musician” inclusion criteria were defined as: (1) Having begun musical training before or at the age of 7 yrs; (2) having had musical training for 10 yrs or more; and (3) having received some musical training within the last 3 yrs (Micheyl et al., 2006; Parbery-Clark et al., 2009). In addition to not meeting the musician criteria, non-musicians were defined as not having received musical training within the 7 yrs before the study. All participants had pure tone thresholds better than 20 dB HL at audiometric test frequencies between 250 and 4000 Hz, and all were native Dutch speakers with no neurological disorders.
The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center Groningen. Participants were given detailed information about the study and written informed consent was obtained. A financial reimbursement was provided.
B. Stimuli
Four meaningful Dutch words in consonant-vowel-consonant format (“bus,” “vaak,” “leeg,” and “pen,” meaning “bus,” “often,” “empty,” and “pencil,” respectively) were used as sources for subsequent speech synthesis. The source speech tokens were taken from the NVA corpus (Bosman and Smoorenburg, 1995) and produced by a single, female Dutch talker. The naturally spoken tokens were systematically manipulated to produce voices that ranged from the female to a male talker, using the STRAIGHT software (v40.006b), implemented in Matlab and developed by Kawahara et al. (1999). The F0 was decreased by an octave in five steps, 0, 3, 6, 9, or 12 semitones (st), and the VTL was increased by 23% (resulting in a downward spectral shift of 3.6 st) in six steps, 0.0, 0.7, 1.6, 2.4, 3.0, or 3.6 st, relative to the female voice. All combinations were generated, resulting in 30 synthesized “voices” and a total of 120 stimuli (4 words × 5 F0 values × 6 VTL values); note that all 120 stimuli were synthesized. Mm. 1 contains the word “bus” with four example voice manipulations.
C. CI simulations
Eight-channel, sine-wave vocoded acoustic CI simulations were generated using Angelsound™ software (Emily Shannon Fu Foundation, http://www.angelsound.tigerspeech.com/). The acoustical input was first band limited to a frequency range of 200 to 7000 Hz, and then bandpass-filtered into 8 frequency analysis bands [fourth order Butterworth filters with band cutoff frequencies according to Greenwood (1990) frequency-place formula]. For each channel, the temporal envelope was extracted using half-wave rectification and low-pass filtering (fourth order Butterworth filter with cutoff frequency = 160 Hz). These envelopes modulated a sinusoidal carrier that was equal to the center frequency of the analysis filter. The modulated carriers were summed to produce the final stimulus and the overall level was adjusted to be the same level as the original signal. Figure 1 shows the spectrums for the word “bus”. The middle row shows the original stimulus resynthesized in STRAIGHT, with the original parameters of the recorded female voice. In the top row, only the F0 was changed, by an octave down. In the bottom row, only the VTL was changed to be made 23% longer, which results in shifting all the formants down by 3.6 st. The left panels show the non-simulated stimulus and the right panel shows the CI-simulated stimulus.
D. Procedure
The stimuli were presented using AngelSound™ software (Emily Shannon Fu Foundation, http://www.angelsound.tigerspeech.com/) and were played from a PC with an Asus Virtuoso Audio Device soundcard (ASUSTeK Computer Inc., Fremont, CA). Participants were seated in an anechoic chamber facing the speaker (Tannoy Precision 8D; Tannoy Ltd., North Lanarkshire, United Kingdom) at 1 m distance. After conversion to an analog signal via a DA10 digital-to-analog converter of Lavry Engineering Inc. (Washington, USA), the stimuli were played at 65 dB sound pressure level in the free field. All stimuli were randomly selected from the stimulus set (without replacement) and played to the subject once. The subject indicated whether the talker was a man or woman by selecting one of two response buttons shown on an A1 AOD 1908 touch screen (GPEG International, Woolwich, United Kingdom) and labeled “man” or “vrouw” (i.e., “man” and “woman”). Subject responses were recorded by the testing software. No feedback was provided. All of the NH listeners were familiar with CI simulations as they had participated in similar experiments before, but otherwise no specific training for the gender recognition task was provided. The gender categorization task lasted for 10 min, resulting in a total testing time of approximately 20 min for all participants.
E. Cue weighting
To quantify how efficiently musicians and non-musicians used voice cues, perceptual weighting of F0 and VTL was calculated using a generalized linear mixed model based on a binomial distribution (logit link function). F0 and VTL were fixed factors and subject was the random intercept. The model was applied to normalized dimensions defined as F0 = −ΔF0/12 and VTL = ΔVTL/3.6, where ΔF0 and ΔVTL represent the F0 or VTL difference in st relative to the source talker. With these normalized dimensions, the point (0,0) represents the synthesized female talker and the point (1,1) represents the synthesized male talker. The cue weights were then expressed as a and b in the equation logit (score) = a F0 + b VTL + ε, where ε is the random intercept that is subject-dependent.
III. Results
Figure 2 shows the mean voice gender categorization with unprocessed (but synthesized) stimuli for non-musicians (left panels) and musicians (right panels), as a function of F0 difference (top plots) or VTL difference (bottom plots) relative to the female source talker. The percentage of male responses was averaged across the four words and across subjects. Both musicians and non-musicians seemed to utilize both F0 and VTL cues similarly, as confirmed by the cue weighting analysis (musicians: F0 = 3.77, VTL = 6.99; non-musicians: F0 = 3.72, VTL = 5.56).
Figure 3 shows similar data for musicians and non-musicians as in Fig. 2, but with the acoustic CI simulation. In general, both subject groups seemed to use both F0 and VTL less efficiently compared to the unprocessed condition (Fig. 2). This is shown by the flatter performance lines and by the lower cue weighting (musicians: F0 = 2.03, VTL = 0.31; non-musicians: F0 = 1.76, VTL = 0.99). The pattern of results with the F0 cue was more diffuse for non-musicians than for musicians, who scored similarly regardless of the VTL cue. The cue weighting analysis suggests that musicians utilized F0 cues more and VTL cues less when compared to non-musicians.
A three-way repeated measures, split-plot analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on all data using a Greenhouser-Geisser correction to ensure sphericity assumption (Table I). The within-subject factors were F0 (five levels), VTL (six levels), and listening condition (two levels: Unprocessed, CI simulated); the between-subject factor was musical experience (two levels: Musician, non-musician). Results confirm a significant overall musician effect on gender categorization. There were significant interactions between F0 and VTL, the listening condition and VTL, and the listening condition, F0 and VTL. However, there were no significant interactions between musical experience and any other factors.
TABLE I.
Factors | F-ratio, p-value |
---|---|
Musical experience (musician, non-musician) | F (1,1) = 4.47, p = 0.040* |
F0 | F (2.23,107.17) = 148.05, p < 0.001 ** |
VTL | F (2.68,128.72) = 197.73, p < 0.001 ** |
Listening condition (unprocessed, CI simulation) | F (1,1) = 41.09, p < 0.001 ** |
F0 × Musical experience | F (2.23,107.17) = 0.240, p = 0.811 |
VTL × Musical experience | F (2.68,128.72) = 1.81. p = 0.154 |
Listening condition × Musical experience | F (1,1) = 0.02, p = 0.889 |
F0 × VTL | F (13.11,629.47) = 6.70, p < 0.001** |
F0 × VTL × Musical experience | F (13.11,629.47) = 1.67, p = 0.062 |
Listening condition × F0 | F (1.82,87.53) = 0.58. p = 0.549 |
Listening condition × VTL | F (2.52,120.92) = 108.12, p < 0.001** |
Listening condition × VTL × Musical experience | F (1.82,120.92) = 2.36, p = 0.086 |
Listening condition × F0 × Musical experience | F (1.82,87.53) = 1.23, p = 0.296 |
Listening condition × F0 × VTL | F (13.46,645.93) = 7.30, p < 0.001** |
Listening condition × F0 × VTL × Musical experience | F (13.46,645.93) = 1.26, p = 0.23 |
IV. Discussion
Based on previous studies in which a positive musician effect in NH listeners had been observed in speech and music-related tasks (Kraus and Chandrasekaran, 2010; Besson et al., 2011; Patel, 2014), we hypothesized that musicians would utilize the voice cues for gender categorization more effectively than non-musicians, especially in spectrally degraded conditions like the CI simulation. This study showed an overall musician effect, mainly in the CI simulation, and that the perceptual weighting of the two voice cues differed between musicians and non-musicians. Musicians perceptually weighted F0 more, but VTL less, than non-musicians in the CI simulation. It is possible that the CI simulation delivered F0 cues more reliably than VTL cues, and musicians made better use of the more reliable cue. Alternatively, musicians may have been more sensitive to F0 cues, and therefore relied on F0 cues more strongly than on VTL cues. If this is the case, musicians would appear to perform similarly to CI users, who have been shown to rely almost exclusively on F0 cues for gender categorization (Fu et al., 2005). This may be a coincidence, as CI users generally do not have extensive musical experience due to hearing impairment (Fuller et al., 2012). On the other hand, in CI simulations, as well as in actual CIs, VTL cues are likely less reliable. This is perhaps the reason for the overall low weighting of VTL in CI simulations, by musicians and non-musicians, as well as CI users. Hence, it may be more advantageous to rely on the more robust cue of F0.
While an overall musician effect was observed, note that the perceptual weighting of F0 and VTL cues was similar for musicians and non-musicians with unprocessed speech. Indeed, performance differences were quite small between subject groups with unprocessed speech (Fig. 2). Previous studies have shown better voice timbre recognition and pitch perception in both speech and music by musicians listening to unprocessed acoustic stimuli (Chartrand and Belin, 2006; Parbery-Clark et al., 2009). The present gender categorization task may have been too easy with unprocessed stimuli, compared to a voice discrimination task (Chartrand and Belin, 2006). As such, gender categorization with unprocessed speech may not have been as sensitive to musical experience. Furthermore, the F0 steps used to synthesize the present “talkers” may have been too large to elicit differences in performance between musicians and non-musicians observed in previous studies (Chartrand and Belin, 2006; Micheyl et al., 2006; Parbery-Clark et al., 2009). Nevertheless, the musician effect on VTL perception or on combined VTL and F0 perception has not been studied previously. The present data do not show a difference in these between musicians and non-musicians under normal listening situations.
The overall findings add to the previous reports of cross-domain effect of musical experience to speech-related tasks, such as voice timbre recognition and voice discrimination (Chartrand and Belin, 2006). In general, musical experience has been shown to enhance performance in a number of listening tasks. Music training has also been shown to improve CI users' music perception (e.g., Galvin et al., 2007). Based on these observations, music training may benefit CI users' speech perception, especially when pitch cues are important, for example, for separating foreground speech from masking speech and better understanding speech in multi-talker environments (Brungart, 2001).
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the participants of this study. Furthermore, we would like to thank the research assistants for their help with collecting the data and Etienne Gaudrain for the help with the figures. We thank Qian-jie Fu and the Emily Fu Foundation for software support. J.J.G. is supported by Grant No. NIH R01-DC004792. R.H.F. is supported by an otological/neurotological stipendium from the Heinsius-Houbolt Foundation. D.B. is supported by a Rosalind Franklin Fellowship from the University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, and the VIDI Grant No. 016.096.397 from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) and the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw). The study is part of the research program of our department: Healthy Aging and Communication. There is no conflict of interest regarding this manuscript.
REFERENCES AND LINKS
- 1.Besson, M., Chobert, J., and Marie, C. (2011). “ Transfer of training between music and speech: Common processing, attention, and memory,” Front. Psychol. 2, 94. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bosman, A. J. , and Smoorenburg, G. F. (1995). “ Intelligibility of Dutch CVC syllables and sentences for listeners with normal hearing and with three types of hearing impairment,” Audiol. 5, 260–284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Brungart, D. S. (2001). “ Informational and energetic masking effects in the perception of two simultaneous talkers,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109, 1101–1109 10.1121/1.1345696 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Chartrand, J. P. , and Belin, P. (2006). “ Superior voice timbre processing in musicians,” Neurosci. Lett. 3, 164–167 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Deguchi, C., Boureux, M., Sarlo, M., Besson, M., Grassi, M., Schon, D., and Colombo, L. (2012). “ Sentence pitch change detection in the native and unfamiliar language in musicians and non-musicians: behavioral, electrophysiological and psychoacoustic study,” Brain Res. 1455, 75–89 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fu, Q. J. , Chinchilla, S., Nogaki, G., and Galvin, J. J. , III (2005). “ Voice gender identification by cochlear implant users: The role of spectral and temporal resolution,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 118(3), 1711–1718 10.1121/1.1985024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fuller, C., Gaudrain, E., Clarke, J., Galvin, J. J. , Fu, Q. J. , Free, R., and Başkent, D. (2013). “Little Red Riding Hood was a Cochlear Implant User!!,” Poster session presented at the 16th Conference on Implantable Auditory Prostheses, July 14–19 (2013), Lake Tahoe, California. Accessible via: http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.871408.
- 17.Fuller, C., Maat, A., Free, R., and Başkent, D. (2012). “Musical background not associated with self-perceived hearing performance or speech perception in postlingual cochlear-implant users,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 132, 1009–1016 10.1121/1.4730910 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Galvin, J. J. , III, Fu, Q. J. , and Nogaki, G. (2007). “ Melodic contour identification by cochlear implant listeners,” Ear Hear. 3, 302–319 10.1097/01.aud.0000261689.35445.20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Greenwood, D. D. (1990). “ A cochlear frequency position function for several species—29 years later,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 87(6), 2592–2605 10.1121/1.399052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kawahara, H., Masuda-Katsuse, I., and de Cheveigné, A. (1999). “ Restructuring speech representations using a pitch-adaptive time–frequency smoothing and an instantaneous-frequency-based F0 extraction: Possible role of a repetitive structure in sounds,” Speech Commun. 3, 187–207 10.1016/S0167-6393(98)00085-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kraus, N., and Chandrasekaran, B. (2010). “ Music training for the development of auditory skills,” Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 599–605 10.1038/nrn2882 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Micheyl, C., Delhommeau, K., Perrot, X., and Oxenham, A. J. (2006). “ Influence of musical and psychoacoustical training on pitch discrimination,” Hear. Res. 1–2, 36–47 10.1016/j.heares.2006.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., and Kraus, N. (2009). “ Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise,” Ear Hear. 6, 653–661 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181b412e9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Patel, A. D. (2014). “ Can nonlinguistic musical training change the way the brain processes speech? The expanded OPERA hypothesis,” Hear. Res. 308(2), 98–108 10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schon, D., Magne, C., and Besson, M. (2004). “ The music of speech: music training facilitates pitch processing in both music and language,” Psychophysiology 3, 341–349 10.1111/1469-8986.00172.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Smith, D. R. , and Patterson, R. D. (2005). “ The interaction of glottal-pulse rate and vocal-tract length in judgments of speaker size, sex, and age,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 5, 3177–3186 10.1121/1.2047107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]