Skip to main content
Patient preference and adherence logoLink to Patient preference and adherence
. 2014 Jul 17;8:1007–1018. doi: 10.2147/PPA.S64593

What puts heart failure patients at risk for poor medication adherence?

George J Knafl 1, Barbara Riegel 2,3,
PMCID: PMC4109641  PMID: 25114512

Abstract

Background

Medication nonadherence is a major cause of hospitalization in patients with heart failure (HF), which contributes enormously to health care costs. We previously found, using the World Health Organization adherence dimensions, that condition and patient level factors predicted nonadherence in HF. In this study, we assessed a wider variety of condition and patient factors and interactions to improve our ability to identify those at risk for hospitalization.

Materials and methods

Medication adherence was measured electronically over the course of 6 months, using the Medication Event Monitoring System (MEMS). A total of 242 HF patients completed the study, and usable MEMS data were available for 218 (90.1%). Participants were primarily white (68.3%), male (64.2%), and retired (44.5%). Education ranged from 8–29 years (mean, 14.0 years; standard deviation, 2.9 years). Ages ranged from 30–89 years (mean, 62.8 years; standard deviation, 11.6 years). Analyses used adaptive methods based on heuristic searches controlled by cross-validation scores. First, individual patient adherence patterns over time were used to categorize patients in poor versus better adherence types. Then, risk factors for poor adherence were identified. Finally, an effective model for predicting poor adherence was identified based on identified risk factors and possible pairwise interactions between them.

Results

A total of 63 (28.9%) patients had poor adherence. Three interaction risk factors for poor adherence were identified: a higher number of comorbid conditions with a higher total number of daily medicines, older age with poorer global sleep quality, and fewer months since diagnosis of HF with poorer global sleep quality. Patients had between zero and three risk factors. The odds for poor adherence increased by 2.6 times with a unit increase in the number of risk factors (odds ratio, 2.62; 95% confidence interval, 1.78–3.86; P<0.001).

Conclusion

Newly diagnosed, older HF patients with comorbid conditions, polypharmacy, and poor sleep are at risk for poor medication adherence. Interventions addressing these specific barriers are needed.

Keywords: heart failure, medication adherence, multiple chronic conditions, risk factors, self-care, sleep quality

Introduction

Heart failure (HF) affects more than five million adults (12% of older adults) in the United States.1 For patients, the symptoms of fatigue, shortness of breath, depression, poor memory, and impaired sleep make HF burdensome.2 Symptoms drive hospitalization: one in four HF patients is readmitted to a hospital within 30 days of hospital discharge, and almost half are readmitted within 6 months.3 These hospitalizations are the primary contributor to the staggering medical cost of HF: $30.7 billion annually.4 This cost is projected to increase more than twofold by 2030, making HF the most expensive condition billed to Medicare.5

A variety of reasons for HF hospitalization have been described, but as noted by Desai and Stevenson6 a robust and actionable model of risk factors for hospitalization is needed. In a prior study, we developed an effective model of risk factors for hospitalization and demonstrated that medication nonadherence was the best predictor of hospitalization in a sample of HF patients, after considering numerous demographic, support, clinical, symptom, cognitive, and self-care factors.7 This result is not surprising, in that most authors have found medication nonadherence rates between 40% and 60% in patients with HF.8 At this point, the urgent need is to identify modifiable factors associated with medication nonadherence.

In the prior study, we used recently developed adaptive modeling methods to analyze objectively measured medication adherence as collected prospectively, using the Medication Event Monitoring System (MEMS®; AARDEX Group Ltd, Sion, Switzerland), which records the date and time of each opening of the MEMS device, and presumably the taking of a dose of the associated medication. We identified individual patient medication adherence patterns over time and clustered these patterns into adherence types.7 These adherence types were used to identify the two categories of poor versus (vs) better adherence, an approach that is distinctly more sensitive than categorizations of adherence based on percentage prescribed doses taken (eg, <80% vs ≥80%). This nuanced approach contributed significantly to the predictive capability of the model for hospitalization, which had excellent discrimination characteristics.7 In this article, we build on previously identified individual patient adherence patterns and types and describe risk factors for poor adherence.

This is a second article exploring predictors of medication adherence in HF patients. In the first article, we used the World Health Organization9 dimensions of adherence (socioeconomic, condition, therapy, patient, and health care system) to focus our analyses.10 These dimensions reflect the types of variables found to predict treatment adherence in various populations.11,12 In our first study of HF patients, we determined that patient and condition characteristics contributed most to a steep decline in medication adherence. Patients with lapses in attention (odds ratio [OR], 2.65; P=0.023), excessive daytime sleepiness (OR, 2.51; P=0.037), and two or more medication dosing intervals per day (OR, 2.59; P=0.016) were more likely to have a steep decline in adherence over time than to have persistent adherence.10 However, as only a select group of possible predictors was tested in that study and no interactions were explored, in this current study, we build on our earlier work by assessing a wider variety of available conditions and patient-level risk factors for poor adherence. The purpose of this work is to identify risk factors associated with medication adherence problems in HF patients, with the ultimate goal of identifying and implementing interventions that address important barriers to adherence, and so reduce the chance of hospitalization. This purpose will be addressed in three steps: identify individual risk factors for poor adherence, identify a multiple risk factors model for poor adherence, and identify a multiple risk factors and interactions model for poor adherence.

Materials and methods

This was a secondary analysis of adherence data from a prospective cohort study of a consecutive sample of 280 adults with a confirmed diagnosis of HF enrolled from three outpatient sites in the northeastern United States; 242 (86.4%) of these patients completed the 6-month study.13 Institutional review board approval was obtained at the University of Pennsylvania, the participating sites where we enrolled participants, and the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and all participants gave informed consent. The primary objective of the parent study was to clarify the extent to which excessive daytime sleepiness influences HF self-care and clinical outcomes and the mechanism of the effect. Study methods have been described previously.13 Participants were preferentially sampled for variability in daytime sleepiness and cognition. Patients with severe depression, dementia (Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status [TICS] scores ≤2414), renal failure requiring dialysis, terminal illness, or history of serious drug or alcohol abuse within the past year were excluded. Because the parent study focused on sleepiness, patients with night-shift responsibilities were excluded. Study participants were followed-up for 6 months, with home visits at baseline, 3, and 6 months, where data were collected by research assistants. A list of all medications taken, including over-the-counter and as-needed medications, was made on the basis of visual assessment by the research assistant during the home visit. As almost half of all HF patients are readmitted to a hospital within 6 months, a 6-month interval was deemed adequate for follow-up.3,6,15 Nurses abstracted clinical information, including comorbid conditions, from medical records. All data were collected between 2007 and 2009.

Measures

Medication adherence was assessed using MEMS, a valid method of measuring medication-taking behavior.16,17 Methods used to collect these MEMS data have been described in detail elsewhere,10 but MEMS data were collected for a single selected medication scheduled to be taken one to three times daily. Our preference was that the MEMS be used with a medication taken in multiple daily doses, but sometimes negotiation was necessary when the medication to be used in the device was chosen. Only one medication was monitored, a practice that has been shown to be adequate because a single drug can be used to illustrate medication-taking patterns.18,19 Most patients (56.7%) put their beta-blocker in the device, but 15.2% put an angiotensin-converting-enzyme inhibitor in it. Putting a diuretic in the device was strongly discouraged, and only seven patients (3.2%) put their diuretic in the MEMS. MEMS data were downloaded for each patient at 3 and 6 months and were cleaned based on patient diaries, with clarification provided by telephone as needed. Specifically, patients were asked to note in their research diary whether something unusual happened that gave misleading data. An example would be that, after refilling a prescription, the device was opened an extra time that day to fill it with medicine. These types of incidents were captured in the diary and adjusted in the raw data. Participants were fully informed about the MEMS device, but telling patients that their medication dosing will be monitored is not sufficient to change behavior significantly.20

The World Health Organization9 dimensions of adherence (socioeconomic, condition, therapy, patient, and health care system) were used to focus the choice of additional indicators of the most promising dimensions and patient-related and condition-related factors. Patient-related variables were grouped into the categories of demographic (Table 1) and social support (Table 2). Condition-related factors were classified as clinical (Table 3), self-care (Table 4), symptom (Table 5), and cognition (Table 6) variables. Only baseline values for these variables were used in analyses. A variety of standard scales were considered; these scales and their psychometric properties are summarized in Table 7. Cognition was measured by a battery of neuropsychological tests, including the Digit Symbol Substitution Test, the Letter Number Sequencing subtest, the Probed Memory Recall Task, and the Trail Making Test: A and B.21 The non-scale-based variables of Tables 16 are self-explanatory.

Table 1.

Summary statistics for available demographic variables

Variable and observed range n (%)* Mean (SD; n)*
Employment status
 Retired 97 (44.5)
 Unemployed or disabled 62 (28.4)
 Employed (full or part time) 59 (27.1)
Sex
 Male 140 (64.2)
 Female 78 (35.8)
Income
 Do not have enough 35 (16.1)
 Have enough or more 183 (83.9)
Insurance
 Government or none 122 (56.0)
 Commercial or health maintenance organization 96 (44.0)
Race
 Nonwhite 69 (31.7)
 White 149 (68.3)
Age
 30–89 62.8 (11.6; 218)
ANART score
 0–49 31.0 (11.2; 218)
Years of education
 8–29 14.0 (2.9; 218)

Note:

*

Out of 218 patients with some Medication Event Monitoring System data.

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; ANART, American National Adult Reading Test.

Table 2.

Summary statistics for available social support variables

Variable and observed range n (%)* Mean (SD; n)*
Living alone
 Yes 48 (22.0)
 No 170 (78.0)
Marital status
 Single, divorced, separated, or widowed 88 (40.4)
 Married or partnered 130 (59.6)
Quality of support
 Satisfactory to good 75 (34.4)
 Very good 143 (65.6)
MSPSS score
 14–84 72.9 (11.3; 210)

Note:

*

Out of 218 patients with Medication Event Monitoring System data.

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; MSPSS, Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support.

Table 3.

Summary statistics for available clinical variables

Variable and observed range n (%)* Mean (SD; n)*
Exercise
 None 36 (16.5)
 Some 182 (83.5)
Body mass index, kg/m2
 15–67 30.8 (7.9; 218)
Blood urea nitrogen
 6–97 24.8 (13.8; 216)
Charlson total
 1–11 2.7 (1.8; 218)
Comorbidities
 0–9 3.0 (2.1; 218)
Creatinine
 0.5–3.4 1.3 (0.5; 215)
Diastolic blood pressure
 45–103 68.9 (110.8; 216)
Ejection fraction
 5–80 35.8 (17.4; 217)
Hemoglobin
 8.1–18.4 13.1 (1.8; 209)
Months since heart failure diagnosis
 0–508 76.2 (75.5; 203)
Pulse
 42–100 69.8 (11.5; 218)
Serum sodium
 131–146 139.1 (2.9; 213)
Systolic blood pressure
 80–176 116.0 (17.5; 217)

Note:

*

Out of 218 patients with some Medication Event Monitoring System data.

Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.

Table 4.

Summary statistics for available self-care variables

Variable and observed range n (%)* Mean (SD; n)*
Prescribed rate for medication controlled by MEMS
 2–3 133 (61.0)
 1 85 (39.0)
DHFKS score
 7–15 11.7 (1.6; 211)
SCHFI self-care confidence
 42–100 75.7 (14.4; 218)
SCHFI self-care maintenance
 32–92 66.8 (11.6; 215)
SCHFI self-care management
 29–100 66.1 (18.6; 94)**
Total medications
 1–25 9.9 (3.9; 218)

Notes:

*

Out of patients with available MEMS data;

**

patients with missing SCHFI self-care management values were those with no symptoms to manage.

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; MEMS, Medication Event Monitoring System; DHFKS, Dutch Heart Failure Knowledge Score; SCHFI, Self-Care of Heart Failure Index.

Table 5.

Summary statistics for available symptom variables

Variable and observed range n (%)* Mean (SD; n)*
General health perception
 Poor 24 (11.0)
 Fair to excellent 194 (89.0)
Health compared with a year ago
 Poor 17 (7.8)
 Fair to excellent 201 (92.2)
Trouble breathing or ankle swelling within past month
 Yes 94 (43.1)
 No 124 (56.9)
NYHA class
 IV 41 (18.8)
 I–III 177 (81.2)
Fatigue
 2–13 6.4 (3.0; 218)
SSS score
 1–6 2.2 (1.2; 218)
ESS score
 0–23 6.7 (4.5; 218)
PSQI global sleep score
 0–19 7.1 (4.0; 218)
PHQ total
 0–18 4.4 (3.6; 218)

Note:

*

Out of 218 patients with some Medication Event Monitoring System data.

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; NYHA, New York Heart Association; SSS, Stanford Sleepiness Scale; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire.

Table 6.

Summary statistics for available cognition variables

Variable and observed range Mean (SD; n)*
CAS
 0–23 9.4 (4.4; 218)
DSST score
 11–96 54.4 (16.7; 217)
LNS score
 1–20 8.9 (3.4; 214)
PMR score
 0–4 2.1 (1.2; 217)
PVT lapses
 0–79 7.8 (12.4; 214)
TICS score
 26–40 33.8 (2.9; 218)
TMTA score
 14–120 41.5 (17.3; 218)
TMTB score
 8–300 105.3 (53.3; 217)
Dimensions cognitively impaired
 0–5 1.6 (1.0; 218)
*

Out of 218 patients with some Medication Event Monitoring System data.

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; CAS, Compensatory Activities Score; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; LNS, Letter Number Sequencing; PMR, Probed Memory Recall; PVT, Psychomotor Vigilance Task; TICS, Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status; TMTA, Trail Making Test: A; TMTB, Trail Making Test: B.

Table 7.

Summary of each standard scale used

Scale name Description Psychometric properties
American National Adult Reading Test (ANART)53,54 Measure of premorbid, crystalized intellect. A list of 50 phonetically irregular words (eg, aisle) is read aloud. The number of words pronounced correctly is used as the score. Reliabilityby Cronbach’s alpha was 0.93. Validity was demonstrated by comparing with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) vocabulary test; coefficient was 0.75.
Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS)55,56 A 12-item measure assessing social support from family, friends, and a significant other. Responses range from 1 (very strongly disagree) to 7 (very strongly agree), with higher scores indicating more perceived support. Reliability coefficients range from 0.85–0.91. Factorial validity has been confirmed repeatedly.
Charlson Comorbidity Index57 Seventeen broad categories of conditions scored with 1–6 points. Scores range from 0–34 and can be classified as low, moderate, and high comorbidity. Established validity for predicting mortality, complications, health care resource use, length of hospital stay, discharge disposition, and cost.
Dutch Heart Failure Knowledge Score58 Fifteen items measuring general knowledge of HF and knowledge of HF treatments, HF symptoms, and symptom recognition. Items based on established patient education guidelines of the Netherlands Heart Foundation, which mirror those of the American Heart Association.
Self-Care of Heart Failure Index (SCHFI V6.2)59 Twenty-two items, measured using a four-point self-report response format, which form three scales: self-care maintenance, management, and confidence. Internal consistency tested by factor score determinacy, coefficients all >0.70. Moderate to high correlations over time in test–retest reliability testing. Construct validity has been demonstrated. The SCHFI is sensitive to subtle behavioral changes in a variety of HF samples.
Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS)60 A measure of global or typical sleepiness. Respondents rate the likelihood of falling asleep in eight soporific situations using a four-point Likert scale ranging from never dozing (0) to high chance of dozing (3). Test–retest reliability (r=0.82) and internal consistency (α=0.88) have been established. Single factor structure. ESS correlates significantly with the frequency of apneas and has a sensitivity of 93.5% and a specificity of 100% for distinguishing pathological from normal sleepiness.
Stanford Sleepiness Scale61 The Stanford Sleepiness Scale provides a rating of sleepiness at a particular moment in time. Current degree of sleepiness is rated 1 (vital, alert, or wide-awake) to 7 (feeling that sleep onset is soon). Sensitive to both sleep deprivation and time of day. Reliability tested as the correlation between alternative forms was adequate (0.88).
Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9)27 The PHQ-9 is a measure of depression. Scores of 5, 10, 15, and 20 represent mild, moderate, moderately severe, and severe depression, respectively. Reliable and valid in numerous populations including HF.
Pittsburgh Sleezp Quality Index (PSQI)62 A self-report measure of the perception of habitual sleep quality measuring seven domains for the prior month: 1) sleep quality; 2) latency; 3) duration; 4) habitual sleep efficiency; 5) use of sleep medications; 6) disturbance; and 7) daytime dysfunction. A global score (0–21 points) is obtained by summing scale domain scores. Higher scores indicate poorer global sleep quality. Internal consistency reliability is in the range of 0.77–0.83. In test–retest reliability testing, scores were not significantly different. PSQI scores have been validated by comparison with polysomnography and shown to discriminate among known groups.
Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT)63 Measure of simple attention. Subjects press a button in response to a series of red digits “000” in an automated light-emitting diode counter window of a small, portable device. Signals are presented at random intervals over a 10-minute period. Metrics involving response speed and lapses are the best primary outcomes for the 10-min PVT. Highly sensitive measure of sleep deprivation.

Abbreviation: HF, heart failure.

Some variables were derived from investigator-generated lists such as the Compensatory Activities Score. Participants were presented with a list of behaviors used by patients (eg, lists) and support persons (eg, reminders) to compensate for memory problems. The number endorsed was used to compute the Compensatory Activities Score. Fatigue was measured as the sum of two items from the Kansas City Cardiomyopathy Questionnaire.22 These items ask how many times fatigue has limited the ability to do activities and how bothersome fatigue has been. Each item is scored 1 to 7. Fatigue scores ranging from 2 to 14 were reversed so that higher scores indicate more fatigue. The alpha coefficient of the fatigue measure was 0.90.

Data analysis

In our prior study using adaptive methods23 to model the effect of medication adherence on hospitalization for the same adherence data, we identified seven adherence types.7 First, individual adherence patterns were generated for each patient, consisting of possibly nonlinear mean adherence and adherence variability curves over time. These adherence patterns were adjusted for prescribed medication rates so that the ideal adherence pattern had mean adherence 1 at each time, with no variability. Then, these patterns were clustered into seven adherence types consisting of patients with similar adherence patterns. The best combination of these seven adherence types for predicting hospitalization was the dichotomous adherence type of poor adherence (ie, with very low adherence some time during study participation) versus better adherence (ie, primarily moderate or better levels of adherence throughout study participation). This dichotomous poor adherence type is modeled in reported analyses.

In this study, adaptive methods23 were used to identify risk factors, individually and in combination, for poor adherence, as previously identified from MEMS adherence data.7 Adaptive methods have been used previously for modeling adherence for HIV-positive patients,24,25 hypertensive patients,26 and HF patients.7 These methods use k-fold likelihood cross-validation (LCV) scores for model selection. The data are randomly partitioned into k distinct subsets, called folds. Likelihoods are computed for each fold, using parameter estimates for the data in the other folds. These deleted fold likelihoods are then combined into a LCV score, with larger scores indicating better models for the data. LCV scores provide for an objective evaluation of models, independent of the size of estimated parameter values (such as ORs for logistic regression models) and of P-values. Reported analyses used 10-fold LCV scores computed from likelihoods for logistic regression models.

The model with the largest LCV score is not always the best choice. A less-complex model may be preferable if the reduction in the LCV score is insubstantial (nonsignificant or indistinct). LCV ratio tests, analogous to likelihood ratio tests, can be used to make such assessments. Although these are χ2-based tests, they are expressed in terms of a cutoff for a substantial (significant or distinct) percentage decrease in the LCV score. A model generating a percentage decrease in the LCV score greater than this cutoff is substantially improved on by the model with the larger LCV score. In contrast, a model generating a percentage decrease in the LCV score less than or equal to the cutoff is a competitive alternative to the model with the larger LCV score. If the model with the lower score is also less complex, then it is a parsimonious, competitive alternative and, thus, preferable. For example, if the constant model for predicting adherence generates an insubstantial percentage decrease in the LCV score in comparison with the model based on a specific risk factor, then the constant model is preferable, and so the risk factor is not a substantive predictor of adherence. The cutoff changes with the sample size.

Patients with poor adherence were characterized by identifying risk factors for poor adherence from among the variables of Tables 16. Categorical variables were used as reported. For each continuous and ordinal variable, observed values were categorized into lower versus higher values, using each observed value as a cutoff. The cutoff with the best (largest) LCV score was used to determine the associated potential risk factor. The risk factor category corresponded to the range of values generating an OR greater than 1 for poor adherence. When a predictor had missing values, those observations were conservatively assigned to the non-risk factor category. In this way, all patients having an identified risk factor had non-missing values for the associated predictor. Cutoffs with less than 10% of the observations in either the lower or higher categories were excluded to avoid sparse cases.

Categorizing continuous/ordinal variables into dichotomous risk factors has the advantage of allowing for missing values without loss of data and of having a practical clinical interpretation. However, the disadvantage is possible loss of information. Whether substantial information has been lost or not can be assessed by comparing LCV scores for a dichotomous risk factor model with the model based on the associated continuous/ordinal variable. This is only possible if that variable has no missing values, as LCV scores are only comparable when based on the same set of data.

Bivariate models were generated for all potential risk factors. Then a multiple risk factors model was generated, using the adaptive modeling process of Knafl et al23 considering only the risk factors with significant (P<0.05) bivariate effects. This adaptive modeling process has been described elsewhere.7,26 In this case, the process first adds risk factors systematically to the model and then contracts the expanded model to remove extraneous risk factors, if any, using a LCV ratio test to decide when to stop the contraction.

Next, an adaptive model was generated with this same process, but considering the same set of risk factors as well as possible pairwise interactions between any two of them to obtain an assessment of the effect of interactions between risk factors. Pairwise interactions holding for less than 10% of the observations were excluded to avoid sparse cases. Finally, this latter model was used to compute a risk index for poor adherence for patients as the count of that model’s risk factors and risk factor interactions.

Results

Sample

Usable MEMS data were available for 218 (90.1%) of the 242 parent study subjects who completed that study. Summary statistics for the patients with usable MEMS data are presented in Tables 16 for available variables within the six categories described earlier. For example, patients were primarily white (68.3%), male (64.2%), and retired (44.5%). Education ranged from 8–29 years, with a mean of 14.0 years (standard deviation, 2.9 years), whereas ages ranged from 30–89 years, with a mean of 62.8 years (standard deviation, 11.6 years). A total of 63 (28.9%) of the patients with usable MEMS data had poor medication adherence.

Risk factors for poor adherence

Table 8 presents results for characterizing poor adherence, considering the variables of Tables 16, one at a time. Individual risk factor analyses identified 12 significant (P<0.05) individual risk factors for poor adherence, including one demographic, zero social support, three clinical, one self-care, four symptom, and three cognition risk factors. The cutoff for a substantial percentage decrease in LCV scores for these analyses is 0.88%. The percentage decrease for the constant model exceeds this cutoff for only two (16.7%) of the twelve variables (LCV scores not reported), indicating that LCV ratio tests are more conservative than tests for zero coefficients, and thus are similar in effect to multiple comparisons procedures.

Table 8.

Significant individual risk factor models of poor versus better adherence

Variable Factor n (%)* P-value OR 95% CI
Demographics
 Age, years ≥61 vs <61 132 (60.6) 0.018 2.17 1.15–4.12
Social support
 –
Clinical
 Comorbidities ≥4 vs <4 87 (39.9) 0.008 2.26 1.24–4.10
 Months since heart failure ≤21 vs >21 or missing 50 (22.9) 0.008 2.43 1.26–4.71
 Diastolic blood pressure ≥82 vs <82 or missing 22 (10.1) 0.026 2.77 1.13–6.77
Self-care
 Total medications ≥9 vs <9 132 (60.6) 0.038 1.95 1.04–3.68
Symptoms
 General health perception Poor vs fair to excellent 24 (11.0) 0.019 2.80 1.19–6.64
 Trouble breathing or ankle swelling within past month Yes vs no 94 (43.1) 0.040 1.86 1.03–3.35
 PHQ total ≥10 vs <10 22 (10.1) 0.026 2.80 1.13–6.77
 PSQI global sleep score ≥3 vs <3 or missing 195 (89.4) 0.039 4.78 1.09–21.0
Cognition
 DSST score ≤42 vs >42 or missing 55 (25.2) 0.016 2.21 1.16–4.21
 TICS score ≤30 vs >30 36 (16.5) 0.027 2.30 1.10–4.80
 TMTB score ≥148 vs <148 or missing 36 (16.5) 0.001 3.51 1.68–7.33

Note:

*

Out of 218 patients with some Medication Event Monitoring System data.

Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval; PHQ, Patient Health Questionnaire; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; TICS, Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status; TMTB, Trail Making Test: B; vs, versus.

There are five continuous/ordinal variables with no missing values generating individual risk factors in Table 8. For two of these variables (number of comorbidities and TICS score), the LCV scores were better for the individual linear models, but with insubstantial percentage decreases for the associated individual risk factor model (0.55357 vs 0.55543 and 0.54778 vs 0.54826, with percentage decreases of 0.33% and 0.09%, respectively). For two other of these variables (age and total medications), the LCV scores were worse for the individual linear models, but with insubstantial percentage decreases compared with the associated individual risk factor model (0.54820 vs 0.55160 and 0.54622 vs 0.54894, with percentage decreases 0.62% and 0.50%, respectively). For the fifth variable (Patient Health Questionnaire27 total score), the LCV score was worse for the individual linear model and with a substantial percentage decrease compared with the associated individual risk factor model (0.54308 vs 0.54857, with percentage decrease 1.00%). These results indicate that, in this case, consideration of dichotomous risk factors does not result in loss of predictive capability over associated linear models. Further, sometimes dichotomous factors even provide distinct improvements in predictive capability.

The adaptive multiple risk factors model, generated considering the twelve significant risk factors of Table 8, had three risk factors: higher Trail Making Test: B, a measure of complex attention (P=0.002; OR, 3.36; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.56–7.25); higher number of comorbid conditions (P=0.025; OR, 2.04; 95% CI, 1.09–7.25); and lower HF duration (P=0.007; OR, 2.61; 95% CI, 1.30–5.22). The LCV score was 0.56824. In contrast, the best individual risk factor model based on a higher Trail Making Test: B score had an LCV score of 0.55545, with a substantial percentage decrease of 2.25%, indicating that the multiple risk factors model substantially improved on each of the individual risk factor models.

The adaptive modeling process is based on LCV scores, so individual risk factors with large ORs need not be included in the adaptively generated multiple risk factor model, unless those risk factors also generate large LCV scores. For example, the risk factor based on a larger Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index global sleep score generated the largest OR of 4.78 in Table 8. However, it had the fourth largest LCV score among individual risk factors (scores not reported in Table 8) and was not included in the adaptively generated multiple risk model. In contrast, the risk factor based on a larger Trail Making Test: B score had the second largest OR of 3.51 in Table 8. However, it also had the largest LCV score among individual risk factor models and was included in the adaptively generated multiple risk factor model.

The adaptive model also considering pairwise risk factor interactions is described in Table 9. This model included three risk factor interactions (and no noninteraction risk factors): a higher number of comorbid conditions with a higher total number of medications, older age with poorer global sleep quality, and fewer months since diagnosis of heart failure (ie, less experience with the illness) with poorer global sleep quality. The c-index (also called the c-statistic; the same as the area under the receiver-operating characteristics curve) was 0.72, which is considered acceptable discrimination.28 The LCV score for this model was 0.57665. The noninteraction multiple risk factors model generated a substantial percentage decrease in the LCV score of 1.46%, indicating that consideration of interactions provided a substantial improvement over only using noninteraction risk factors for predicting poor adherence.

Table 9.

Multiple Risk Factor Interactions Model for poor versus better adherence

Description Interaction term 1
Interaction term 2
At risk group, n (%)* P-value OR 95% CI
Variable Risk factor Variable Risk factor
Higher number of comorbidities with higher total medications Comorbidities ≥4 vs <4 Total medications ≥9 vs <9 67 (30.7) 0.2 2.89 1.18–7.06
Older age with poorer global sleep quality Age ≥61 vs <61 PSQI global sleep score ≥3 vs <3 or missing 117 (53.7) 0.004 3.20 1.45–7.07
Fewer months since diagnosis of heart failure with poorer global sleep quality Months since heart failure ≤21 vs >21 or missing PSQI global sleep score ≥3 vs <3 or missing 44 (20.2) 0.006 2.82 1.35–5.85
*

Out of 218 patients with some Medication Event Monitoring System data.

Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; vs, versus.

To assess the possibility of collinearity between these three interactions, we computed logistic regression models predicting each of these three dichotomous interactions as a function of the other two. The largest Nagelkerke R2 value for these models was 4.5%, indicating that collinearity was not a problem for the risk factor interactions model.

Patients had zero to three of the three interaction risk factors, with 69 (31.7%) patients having none of the interaction risk factors, 78 (35.8%) patients having one, 63 (28.9%) patients having two, and eight (3.7%) patients having three factors. For patients with poor adherence, percentages increased from 10.1% to 26.9%, 47.6%, and 62.5% of the 69, 78, 63, and eight patients with zero to three interaction risk factors, respectively. The risk index model based on the number of risk factor interactions as the only predictor of poor adherence had an LCV score 0.57954. The risk factor interaction model was a parsimonious, competitive alternative with an insubstantial percentage decrease in the LCV score of 0.50%. The c-index for the risk index model was acceptable, at 0.71, and the estimated OR for a unit increase in the risk index variable was 2.62 (95% CI, 1.78–3.86; P<0.001).

Discussion

In this study, we characterized poor medication adherence, as determined from our prior assessment of electronically monitored patient adherence.7 Our major finding was that three pairs of interaction risk factors successfully predicted having poor versus better medication adherence levels: a higher number of comorbid conditions with a higher total number of medications, older age with poorer global sleep quality, and fewer months since diagnosis of heart failure with poorer global sleep quality. The addition of even one interaction risk factor increased the odds of poor adherence by about 2.6 times. Some of these risk factors are modifiable and provide direction for intervention.

These results differ in important ways from our prior analysis of predictors of MEMS-based adherence data.10 First, in that analysis, using growth mixture modeling, we identified two distinct patterns, and 22% of HF patients were in the “steep decline” or poorest medication-adherence group. In the current analysis, when the seven distinct types, accounting for both means adherence and adherence variability over time, as identified earlier,7 were collapsed into two types, 28.9% were in the poor medication adherence type. Second, having focused the analysis of contributors to a steep decline in adherence on a select group of variables suggested by the World Health Organization model, we identified only three contributors: lapses in attention, excessive daytime sleepiness, and two or more medication doses per day. Moreover, interactions were not considered. In this analysis, we tested more potential contributors in the promising dimensions of patient- and condition-related factors and assessed potential interactions. This approach revealed three specific pairs of interacting risk factors likely to increase the odds of medication nonadherence. Interestingly, the number of daily medication doses for the drug used in the MEMS device did not predict adherence, although this factor has been found repeatedly in other studies.29,30 Perhaps this was a result of considering adherence variability along with mean adherence, rather than just mean adherence, in forming adherence types.

We found that a higher number of comorbid conditions plus more medications taken daily or polypharmacy, conventionally defined as the chronic use of five or more medications,31 predicted poor medication adherence. These results are consistent with those of others who have found that more comorbid conditions and more pills taken each day predicted poor medication adherence.32 We found previously that HF patients with multiple comorbid conditions find that differentiating the symptoms of multiple conditions is one of the most challenging self-care skills.33 Having multiple conditions also decreases self-efficacy or confidence in one’s ability to perform specific self-care tasks such as medication taking.34 When intervention studies were examined in a systematic review, medication adherence increased most consistently with behavioral interventions that reduced medication dosing demands,35 illustrating that polypharmacy adds a level of complexity to life with multiple chronic conditions that predisposes patients to poor medication adherence. In fact, the essence of medication reconciliation, a popular approach for patients with multiple chronic conditions, involves analyzing and resolving medication discrepancies and typically decreases the number of pills taken daily.36

Another pair of risk factors for medication nonadherence was the interaction of older age and poorer sleep quality. We are not the first investigators to identify older age as a factor in nonadherence.5,15,37 Poor sleep quality is also known to impair the ability to pay attention and make good decisions.38,39 However, the interaction of older age and poor sleep quality may be best explained by the compelling mechanistic explanation described by Neupert et al37 who examined how daily fluctuations in cognition and busyness are related to daily fluctuations in forgetting to take medications and whether these within-person relationships differed for younger and older adults. On days when the older adults in their study were relatively less busy, they were at lower risk for forgetting to take their medicines, but only if they were also performing well on the everyday cognition assessments. This observation is consistent with our findings that poor sleep quality contributes to forgetting to take medications.10,13 Together these results reinforce the salience of daily routines and lifestyle factors such as sleep routines, as they influence memory in older adults.

Finally, patients with a shorter duration of HF or less experience with the diagnosis and poor sleep quality were at higher risk for nonadherence. Others have demonstrated previously that patients who are newly diagnosed with HF struggle with self-care.40,41 Dickson et al described a typology in which novices lacked experience and skill in caring for their HF diagnosis.42 Knowing that attention and decision-making are impaired by poor sleep and that better decisions are made by people with illness experience, the interaction between shorter duration of HF and poor sleep quality in predicting nonadherence is not surprising.

Together, these results suggest that older age, multiple comorbid conditions, polypharmacy, lack of experience, and poor sleep quality put HF patients at risk for poor medication adherence. Fortunately, interventions addressing some of these predictors are available. Systematic evaluation and modification of the medication regimen (including over-the-counter medicines) in all HF patients could address many of the problems caused by polypharmacy. Multidrug combinations or “polypills” have been advocated as a solution to drug–drug interactions and poor treatment adherence.43 Lack of experience is best addressed with education and support. We found previously that about 2 months after being diagnosed with HF, patients improve in their abilities to adhere to the treatment program, detect symptoms, and make good decisions about those symptoms.41 Steering newly diagnosed patients toward a sustainable routine during that 2-month period may decrease problems with medication adherence.

Surprisingly, interventions for poor sleep quality are the most challenging. For people with sleep apnea, continuous positive airway pressure is effective, but adherence to treatment is problematic.44 For people with insomnia, the most common treatments used are over-the-counter antihistamines, alcohol, and prescription medications such as benzodiazepine receptor agonists.45 These prescription hypnotics have been shown to have good short-term efficacy46 and good durability over time frames of up to 12 months,47,48 but clinical outcomes do not persist after treatment discontinuation, and issues such as rebound insomnia, dependence, abuse potential and respiratory depression49,50 make providers and patients hesitant to use them. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia is effective,51 but it is initially time-intensive and costly, and not all patients are willing or able to engage in this form of psychotherapy. Research identifying other treatment options for the general population with poor sleep quality may have an added benefit of improving medication adherence.

Limitations of this study include possible selection bias, as the data were taken from a prospective cohort study in which patients were selected for variability in daytime sleepiness and cognitive function. In addition, as a group, these patients were younger and better educated than some community samples of HF patients. Future research is needed to test these results in more general populations. Because the analyses were exploratory, further research is needed to confirm these results. Further research is needed to investigate longitudinally both the effects of prior risk factors on adherence and of that adherence on subsequent risk factors. Adherence was measured with MEMS for a single HF medication. There is no guarantee that a dose of the medication was taken every time the MEMS device was opened. Moreover, adherence for medications not controlled by the MEMS has not been accounted for. These limitations are offset by several strengths. The statistical approach took into account patterns over time for both mean adherence and adherence variability, allowing a more nuanced understanding of medication adherence than prior studies. Other strengths include the prospective design and the objective measurement of medication adherence.

In conclusion, medication adherence is a continuing problem for which solutions are urgently needed. In this study, we identified three pairs of variables associated with nonadherence. Together, these three pairs suggest that clinicians caring for HF patients who are of older age, those with multiple comorbid conditions and taking numerous medications, and those who are newly diagnosed should anticipate problems with medication adherence and discuss ways to assist patients to avoid adherence problems. Asking patients about their sleep quality should be a routine element of all clinical encounters.52 Future research is needed to identify interventions that adequately address these predictors of nonadherence.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge Megan Patey, RN, BSN, for her assistance with preparation of the data for analysis and Dr Harleah Buck for her insightful review of a prior draft of this article. This work was funded in part by a grant from the National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute (R01 HL084394-01A1) and by the Philadelphia Veterans Affairs Medical Center, VISN 4 Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center.

Footnotes

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • 1.Go AS, Mozaffarian D, Roger VL, et al. American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee Heart disease and stroke statistics – 2013 update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2013;127(1):e6–e245. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e31828124ad. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Herr JK, Salyer J, Lyon DE, Goodloe L, Schubert C, Clement DG. Heart Failure Symptom Relationships: A Systematic Review. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2013 Jul 8; doi: 10.1097/JCN.0b013e31829b675e. Epub. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Krumholz HM, Merrill AR, Schone EM, et al. Patterns of hospital performance in acute myocardial infarction and heart failure 30-day mortality and readmission. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2009;2(5):407–413. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.109.883256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Heidenreich PA, Albert NM, Allen LA, et al. American Heart Association Advocacy Coordinating Committee. Council on Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis and Vascular Biology. Council on Cardiovascular Radiology and Intervention. Council on Clinical Cardiology. Council on Epidemiology and Prevention. Stroke Council Forecasting the impact of heart failure in the United States: a policy statement from the American Heart Association. Circ Heart Fail. 2013;6(3):606–619. doi: 10.1161/HHF.0b013e318291329a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Andrews RM, Elixhauser A. The national hospital bill: growth trends and 2005 update on the most expensive conditions by payer. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; 2007. [Accessed April 18, 2014]. Available from: http://www.hcup-us.ahrq.gov/reports/statbriefs/sb42.pdf. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Desai AS, Stevenson LW. Rehospitalization for heart failure: predict or prevent? Circulation. 2012;126(4):501–506. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.112.125435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Riegel B, Knafl GJ. Electronically monitored medication adherence predicts hospitalization in heart failure patients. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2013;8:1–13. doi: 10.2147/PPA.S54520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wu JR, Moser DK, Lennie TA, Burkhart PV. Medication adherence in patients who have heart failure: a review of the literature. Nurs Clin North Am. 2008;43(1):133–153. doi: 10.1016/j.cnur.2007.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.World Health Organisation . Overview: Medication Adherence – Where Are We Today? World Health Organization; 2006. [Accessed April 18, 2014]. Available from: http://www.adultmeducation.com/downloads/Adult_Med_Overview.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Riegel B, Lee CS, Ratcliffe SJ, et al. Predictors of objectively measured medication nonadherence in adults with heart failure. Circ Heart Fail. 2012;5(4):430–436. doi: 10.1161/CIRCHEARTFAILURE.111.965152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.AlGhurair SA, Hughes CA, Simpson SH, Guirguis LM. A systematic review of patient self-reported barriers of adherence to antihypertensive medications using the world health organization multidimensional adherence model. J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich) 2012;14(12):877–886. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7176.2012.00699.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Bender BG, Rand C. Medication non-adherence and asthma treatment cost. Curr Opin Allergy Clin Immunol. 2004;4(3):191–195. doi: 10.1097/00130832-200406000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Riegel B, Moelter ST, Ratcliffe SJ, et al. Excessive daytime sleepiness is associated with poor medication adherence in adults with heart failure. J Card Fail. 2011;17(4):340–348. doi: 10.1016/j.cardfail.2010.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Brandt J, Folstein MF. Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jencks SF, Williams MV, Coleman EA. Rehospitalizations among patients in the Medicare fee-for-service program. N Engl J Med. 2009;360(14):1418–1428. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa0803563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Vrijens B, Tousset E, Rode R, Bertz R, Mayer S, Urquhart J. Successful projection of the time course of drug concentration in plasma during a 1-year period from electronically compiled dosing-time data used as input to individually parameterized pharmacokinetic models. J Clin Pharmacol. 2005;45(4):461–467. doi: 10.1177/0091270004274433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Savic RM, Barrail-Tran A, Duval X, et al. ANRS 134–COPHAR 3 Study Group Effect of adherence as measured by MEMS, ritonavir boosting, and CYP3A5 genotype on atazanavir pharmacokinetics in treatment-naive HIV-infected patients. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2012;92(5):575–583. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2012.137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.McNabb JJ, Nicolau DP, Stoner JA, Ross J. Patterns of adherence to antiretroviral medications: the value of electronic monitoring. AIDS. 2003;17(12):1763–1767. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200308150-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cramer J, Vachon L, Desforges C, Sussman NM. Dose frequency and dose interval compliance with multiple antiepileptic medications during a controlled clinical trial. Epilepsia. 1995;36(11):1111–1117. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1995.tb00469.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sutton S, Kinmonth AL, Hardeman W, et al. Does Electronic Monitoring Influence Adherence to Medication? Randomized Controlled Trial of Measurement Reactivity. Ann Behav Med. 2014 Feb 27; doi: 10.1007/s12160-014-9595-x. Epub. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lezak M, Howieson D, Lorig D. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Green CP, Porter CB, Bresnahan DR, Spertus JA. Development and evaluation of the Kansas City Cardiomyopathy Questionnaire: a new health status measure for heart failure. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2000;35(5):1245–1255. doi: 10.1016/s0735-1097(00)00531-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Knafl GJ, Delucchi KL, Bova CA, Fennie KP, Ding K, Williams AB. A systematic approach for analyzing electronically monitored adherence data. In: Ekwall B, Cronquist M, editors. Micro Electro Mechanical Systems (MEMS) Technology, Fabrication Processes and Applications. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers; 2010. pp. 1–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Knafl GJ, Bova CA, Fennie KP, O’Malley JP, Dieckhaus KD, Williams AB. An analysis of electronically monitored adherence to antiretroviral medications. AIDS Behav. 2010;14(4):755–768. doi: 10.1007/s10461-008-9512-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Delucchi KL, Knafl GJ, Haug N, Sorensen J. Adaptive Poisson modeling of medication adherence among HIV-positive methadone patients provided greater understanding of behavior. In: Ekwall B, Cronquist M, editors. Micro Electro Mechanical Systems (MEMS) Technology, Fabrication Processes and Applications. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers; 2010. pp. 259–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Knafl GJ, Schoenthaler A, Ogedegbe G. Secondary analysis of electronically monitored medication adherence data for a cohort of hypertensive African-Americans. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2016:207–219. doi: 10.2147/PPA.S30582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kroenke K, Spitzer R, Williams J. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2001;16(9):606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Applied Logistic Regression. 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Claxton AJ, Cramer J, Pierce C. A systematic review of the associations between dose regimens and medication compliance. Clin Ther. 2001;23(8):1296–1310. doi: 10.1016/s0149-2918(01)80109-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kardas P. Comparison of patient compliance with once-daily and twice-daily antibiotic regimens in respiratory tract infections: results of a randomized trial. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2007;59(3):531–536. doi: 10.1093/jac/dkl528. Epub 2007 Feb 8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mastromarino V, Casenghi M, Testa M, Gabriele E, Coluccia R, Rubattu S, Volpe M. Polypharmacy in heart failure patients. Curr Heart Fail Rep. 2014;11(2):212–219. doi: 10.1007/s11897-014-0186-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jansà M, Hernández C, Vidal M, et al. Multidimensional analysis of treatment adherence in patients with multiple chronic conditions. A cross-sectional study in a tertiary hospital. Patient Educ Couns. 2010;81(2):161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2009.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dickson VV, Buck H, Riegel B. A qualitative meta-analysis of heart failure self-care practices among individuals with multiple comorbid conditions. J Card Fail. 2011;17(5):413–419. doi: 10.1016/j.cardfail.2010.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Dickson VV, Buck H, Riegel B. Multiple comorbid conditions challenge heart failure self-care by decreasing self-efficacy. Nurs Res. 2013;62(1):2–9. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0b013e31827337b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kripalani S, Yao X, Haynes RB. Interventions to enhance medication adherence in chronic medical conditions: a systematic review. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167(6):540–550. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Alfaro Lara ER, Vega Coca MD, Galván Banqueri M, et al. Selection of tools for reconciliation, compliance and appropriateness of treatment in patients with multiple chronic conditions. Eur J Intern Med. 2012;23(6):506–512. doi: 10.1016/j.ejim.2012.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Neupert SD, Patterson TR, Davis AA, Allaire JC. Age differences in daily predictors of forgetting to take medication: the importance of context and cognition. Exp Aging Res. 2011;37(4):435–448. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2011.590757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Quan SF, Chan CS, Dement WC, et al. The association between obstructive sleep apnea and neurocognitive performance – the Apnea Positive Pressure Long-term Efficacy Study (APPLES) Sleep. 2011;34(3):303B–314B. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.3.303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.McKenna BS, Dickinson DL, Orff HJ, Drummond SP. The effects of one night of sleep deprivation on known-risk and ambiguous-risk decisions. J Sleep Res. 2007;16(3):245–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00591.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cameron J, Worrall-Carter L, Page K, Stewart S. Self-care behaviours and heart failure: does experience with symptoms really make a difference? Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2010;9(2):92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.ejcnurse.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Francque-Frontiero L, Riegel B, Bennett J, et al. Self-care of persons with heart failure: Does experience make a difference? Clin Excel Nurse Pract. 2002;6:23–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dickson VV, Deatrick JA, Riegel B. A typology of heart failure self-care management in non-elders. Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2008;7(3):171–181. doi: 10.1016/j.ejcnurse.2007.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Vaduganathan M, Gheorghiade M, Butler J. Expanding the scope of the “polypill” to heart failure. J Card Fail. 2013;19(8):540–541. doi: 10.1016/j.cardfail.2013.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Weaver TE, Chasens ER. Continuous positive airway pressure treatment for sleep apnea in older adults. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(2):99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2006.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.NIH State of the Science Conference statement on Manifestations and Management of Chronic Insomnia in Adults statement. NIH Consens State Sci Statements. 2005;22(2):1–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Nowell PD, Mazumdar S, Buysse DJ, Dew MA, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Kupfer DJ. Benzodiazepines and zolpidem for chronic insomnia: a meta-analysis of treatment efficacy. JAMA. 1997;278(24):2170–2177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Krystal AD, Walsh JK, Laska E, et al. Sustained efficacy of eszopiclone over 6 months of nightly treatment: results of a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in adults with chronic insomnia. Sleep. 2003;26(7):793–799. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.7.793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Roth T, Walsh JK, Krystal A, Wessel T, Roehrs TA. An evaluation of the efficacy and safety of eszopiclone over 12 months in patients with chronic primary insomnia. Sleep Med. 2005;6(6):487–495. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Clarfield AM. Review: sedative-hypnotics increase adverse effects more than they improve sleep quality in older persons with insomnia. ACP J Club. 2006;145(1):14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Curran HV, Collins R, Fletcher S, Kee SC, Woods B, Iliffe S. Older adults and withdrawal from benzodiazepine hypnotics in general practice: effects on cognitive function, sleep, mood and quality of life. Psychol Med. 2003;33(7):1223–1237. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703008213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Mitchell MD, Gehrman P, Perlis M, Umscheid CA. Comparative effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia: a systematic review. BMC Fam Pract. 2012;13:40. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-13-40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Riegel B, Hanlon AL, Zhang X, et al. What is the best measure of daytime sleepiness in adults with heart failure? J Am Assoc Nurse Pract. 2013;25(5):272–279. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7599.2012.00784.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Gladsjo JA, Heaton RK, Palmer BW, Taylor MJ, Jeste DV. Use of oral reading to estimate premorbid intellectual and neuropsychological functioning. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1999;5(3):247–254. doi: 10.1017/s1355617799533079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Uttl B. North American Adult Reading Test: age norms, reliability, and validity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24(8):1123–1137. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.8.1123.8375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Dahlem NW, Zimet GD, Walker RR. The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support: a confirmation study. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1991;47(6):756–761. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199111)47:6<756::aid-jclp2270470605>3.0.co;2-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Zimet GD, Powell SS, Farley GK, Werkman S, Berkoff KA. Psychometric characteristics of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. J Pers Assess. Winter. 1990;55(3–4):610–617. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Charlson M, Pompei P, Ales K, MacKenzie C. A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: Development and validation. J Chronic Dis. 1987;40:373–383. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(87)90171-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.van der Wal M, Jaarsma T, Moser D, van Veldhuisen D. Development and testing of the Dutch Heart Failure Knowledge Scale. European Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing. 2005 Aug; doi: 10.1016/j.ejcnurse.2005.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Riegel B, Lee CS, Dickson VV, Carlson B. An Update on the Self-Care of Heart Failure Index. Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing. 2009;24(6):485–497. doi: 10.1097/JCN.0b013e3181b4baa0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1992;15(4):376–381. doi: 10.1093/sleep/15.4.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, Phillips R, Dement WC. Quantification of sleepiness: a new approach. Psychophysiology. 1973;10(4):431–436. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1973.tb00801.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Buysse D, Reynolds CF, 3d, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatr Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011;34(5):581–591. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Patient preference and adherence are provided here courtesy of Dove Press

RESOURCES