Abstract
Background
It is known that enhancement of sympathetic nerve activity based on a decrease in parasympathetic nerve activity is associated with fatigue induced by mental tasks lasting more than 30 min. However, to measure autonomic nerve function and assess fatigue levels in both clinical and industrial settings, shorter experimental durations and more sensitive measurement methods are needed. The aim of the present study was to establish an improved method for inducing fatigue and evaluating the association between it and autonomic nerve activity.
Methods
Twenty-eight healthy female college students participated in the study. We used a kana pick-out test (KPT) as a brief verbal cognitive task and recorded electrocardiography (ECG) to measure autonomic nerve activity. The experimental design consisted of a 16-min period of ECG: A pre-task resting state with eyes open for 3 min and eyes closed for 3 min, the 4-min KPT, and a post-task resting state with eyes open for 3 min and eyes closed for 3 min.
Results
Baseline fatigue sensation, measured by a visual analogue scale before the experiment, was associated with the decrease in parasympathetic sinus modulation, as indicated the by ratio of low-frequency component power (LF) to high-frequency component power (HF), during the KPT. The LF/HF ratio during the post-KPT rest with eyes open tended to be greater than the ratio during the KPT and correlated with fatigue sensation. Fatigue sensation was correlated negatively with log-transformed HF, which is an index of parasympathetic sinus modulation, during the post-KPT rest with eyes open.
Conclusions
The methods described here are useful for assessing the association between fatigue sensation and autonomic nerve activity using a brief cognitive test in healthy females.
Background
Fatigue, defined as a difficulty in initiating or sustaining voluntary activities [1], is experienced by many people during or after a prolonged period of activity. Large community surveys have reported that up to half of the general adult population report fatigue [2,3]. This is also true in Japan, where more than one third of the population report chronic fatigue [4]. Epidemiological studies have reported that the female/male ratio of people with chronic fatigue in the general population is approximately 2:1 [5,6], and Pikó et al. reported that fatigue level was higher in female university students than in male university students [7]. Acute fatigue is a normal phenomenon that disappears after a period of rest. By contrast, chronic fatigue is sometimes irreversible and compensatory mechanisms that are useful in reducing acute fatigue are not effective [8]. Chronic fatigue is caused by the prolonged accumulation of acute fatigue. Thus, in order to avoid chronic fatigue, it is important to develop objective measures of fatigue and effective strategies to recover from and avoid the accumulation of acute fatigue.
Alterations of autonomic functions caused by fatigue in healthy people have been measured by electrocardiogram (ECG) and accelerated plethysmography and can provide objective biomarkers of fatigue [4]. Decreased parasympathetic sinus modulation and increased sympathetic sinus modulation were induced in healthy volunteers following a 30-min series of fatigue-inducing mental tasks [9,10]. After a prolonged cognitive load for 8 h, corresponding to a normal work day, we found that sympathetic hyperactivity based on decreased parasympathetic sinus modulation was positively correlated with subjective fatigue level [11]. This suggests that enhancement of sympathetic sinus modulation is closely related to fatigue induced by lengthy mental tasks of 30 min to 8 h. However, to measure autonomic nerve function and fatigue levels, both in clinical and industrial settings, better experimental designs, including briefer mental tasks and more sensitive measures for detecting changes in sympathetico-vagal balance, are needed.
In previous studies, for fatigue-inducing and fatigue-evaluating tests, we have used 30 min or more of the 2-back test [9,10] and an advanced trail making test [9-11], both of which require working memory or selective attention, to investigate the relation between fatigue and autonomic nerve functions. The kana pick-out test (KPT) is a divided-attention test (dual task) that has also been used as a fatigue-inducing and fatigue-evaluating mental task [11-13]. The KPT demands parallel processing during a 4-min task. Participants must select a subset of letters contained within a story while reading the story for comprehension for 2 min, and must then answer 10 questions about the contents of the story for 2 min. In the present study, we used the KPT as a brief but difficult mental task.
Autonomic nerve function during mental fatigue was assessed during the KPT and while participants were sitting quietly with their eyes closed or open for few minutes rest before and after the KPT. We have previously recorded ECG and accelerated plethysmography with participants’ eyes open [14] or closed [10,11] during rest, but with eyes open only during the fatigue-inducing task. Attention levels are different between eyes open and eyes closed conditions, therefore the sympathetico-vagal balance is also considered to be different, suggesting that control of sympathetico-vagal balance could be evaluated by comparing these conditions [15]. Therefore, in the present study we measured ECG with both eyes closed and open to investigate the sensitivity of our measurement method for detecting changes in autonomic nerve activity produced by fatigue.The aim of the present study was to establish an improved fatigue measurement method for assessing the association between fatigue sensation and autonomic nerve activity in healthy female volunteers. We measured a 16-min period of ECG that covered a pre-KPT resting state with eyes open (3 min) and eyes closed (3 min), the 4-min KPT performed with eyes open, and a post-KPT resting state with eyes open (3 min) and eyes closed (3 min; Figure 1), and investigated the correlation between baseline fatigue sensation and alterations in autonomic nerve activity induced by the KPT.
Methods
Participants
The study group comprised 28 healthy female college students [age, 20.8 ± 0.5 years (mean ± SD)]. Individuals with a history of medical illness or taking chronic medications or supplemental vitamins were excluded. Current smokers were also excluded because smoking is closely associated with fatigue [16], sleep [17], and attentional task performance [18] and it has been reported that female cigarette smokers have abnormal sympathetic nerve activity [19]. These findings indicate that smoking affects fatigue and fatigue-related factors (sleep, task performance and autonomic nerve function). The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Kansai University of Welfare Sciences, and all participants provided written informed consent for participation in the study.
Experimental design
The time schedule for the experiments is shown in Figure 1. On the day before the experiment and the day of the experimental, participants were instructed to avoid intensive physical and mental activities. We conducted the experiment for around 1 h within the period from 9:00 a.m. to noon in each participant. Before measurement of ECG, participants recorded their subjective sensation of fatigue on a visual analogue scale (VAS) from 0 (no fatigue) to 100 (complete exhaustion). To evaluate the autonomic nerve function during the resting state before the KPT, ECG was recorded while participants sat in a chair for 6 min: 3 min with their eyes open and 3 min with their eyes closed. Participants then performed the KPT for 4 min, and ECG was recorded throughout. ECG was again recorded during the resting state after the KPT: 3 min with eyes open and 3 min with eyes closed.
Electrocardiogram
ECG was recorded continuously during the experiment using RF-ECG (GM3, Tokyo, Japan) and was analyzed using MemCalc/Bonaly Light (SuwaTrust/GMS, Tokyo, Japan). Frequency analyses for R-R interval variation were analyzed with the maximum entropy method, which is capable of estimating the power spectrum density from short time series data, and is adequate to examine changes in heart rate variability in different conditions of short duration [20,21]. The validity of the power spectral density of the maximum entropy method has been confirmed by comparing it to the autoregressive model [22]. The power spectrum resolution was 256 Hz. For the frequency analyses, the total power (TP) was calculated as the power within the frequency range 0 – 0.4 Hz. The very-low-frequency component power (VLF) was calculated as the power within the frequency range 0 – 0.05 Hz, the low-frequency component power (LF) was calculated as the power within the frequency range 0.04 – 0.15 Hz, and the high-frequency component power (HF) as the power within the frequency range 0.15 – 0.4 Hz. Absolute LF and HF values were transformed to normalized units (nu) as follows: [LF nu = LF/(TP – VLF) × 100] and [HF nu = HF/(TP – VLF) × 100] [23,24]. LF is simultaneously a marker of sympathetic sinus modulation increase and a marker of parasympathetic sinus modulation decrease, LF nu is a marker of parasympathetic sinus modulation decrease, HF nu is a marker of parasympathetic sinus modulation increase, and the LF/HF ratio expressed in nu is a marker of parasympathetic sinus modulation decrease [24].
Kana pick-out test
The KPT requires parallel processing of reading and selection of letters, and also requires appropriate allocation of attentional resources to the two activities. Participants are shown a short story written in Japanese kana characters. They are required to find as many vowel symbols as possible within 2 min, while understanding the meaning of the story (dual-task session). Two min after the start of the test, they are asked 10 questions about the contents of the story over a 2 min period (answer session). Japanese kana characters consist of 66 phonetic symbols that include five vowels and the story consisted of 406 symbols with 61 vowels. The full score for this test is therefore 61, and the full comprehension score is 10.
Statistical analyses
Log-transformed LF (ln LF), HF (ln HF) and LF/HF ratio (ln LF/HF ratio) in the conditions (pre-KPT rest with eyes open, pre-KPT rest with eyes closed, KPT dual-task session, KPT answer session, post-KPT rest with eyes open and post-KPT rest with eyes closed) were analyzed using [11,23] parametric one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). When statistically significant effects were found, intergroup differences were evaluated using the Tukey's honestly significant difference test. Stepwise multiple regression analyses were calculated between the VAS score for fatigue and autonomic nerve function. All p-values were two-tailed, and p-values less than 0.05 were considered significant. These analyses were performed with the IBM SPSS 20.0 software package (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL).
Results
The results for ECG are summarized in Figure 2. One-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect for ln LF [F(5, 27) = 8.46, p < 0.001] and ln HF [F(5, 27) = 3.16, p = 0.010], and a trend of a main effect for LF/HF ratio [F(5, 27) = 2.10, p = 0.069]. The ln LF of post-KPT rest with eyes open was significantly higher than that of pre-KPT rest with eyes open and closed, KPT dual-task session, KPT answer session, and post-KPT rest with eyes closed (Figure 2a). The ln HF of the dual-task session and the answer session were lower than that of pre-KPT rest with eyes open (Figure 2b). The LF/HF ratio of post-KPT rest with eyes open showed a trend to be greater than that of KPT answer session (Figure 2c).
To investigate the effect of each participant’s subjective fatigue sensation on autonomic nerve activity, multiple regression analyses between baseline fatigue sensation and change in autonomic nerve activity from the pre-KPT rest with eyes open condition (baseline) to other conditions were performed. The change in each absolute power (ln TP, ln VLF, ln LF and ln HF) and derived power (LF nu, HF nu and LF/HF) was calculated by subtracting the value during pre-KPT rest with eyes open from the value during pre-KPT rest with eyes closed, KPT dual-task session, KPT answer session, post-KPT rest with eyes open, and post-KPT rest with eyes closed. Results of multiple regression analyses are shown in Figure 3. After stepwise selection in the absolute power variables, a significant negative relation was observed between VAS score for fatigue and change in ln HF in post-KPT rest with eyes open (β = −0.408, p = 0.043) based on a significant regression model (p = 0.043; Figure 3a). After stepwise selection in the derived power variables, there was also a positive correlation between VAS score for fatigue and change in LF/HF ratio in the KPT dual-task session (β = 0.397, p = 0.036; Figure 3b) and post-KPT rest with eyes open (β = 0.417, p = 0.027; Figure 3c) and post-KPT rest with eyes closed (β = 0.444, p = 0.018; Figure 3d) conditions based on each significant regression model (p < 0.05).
Discussion
During the KPT dual-task session, there was a decrease in parasympathetic sinus modulation compared with pre-KPT rest with eyes open (Figure 2b) and fatigue sensation was associated with the decrease in parasympathetic sinus modulation and increase in sympathetic sinus modulation in this session (Figure 3b). This relation between the alteration of autonomic activity and fatigue sensation during the dual task may be related to interactions among the neural substrates of the KPT, fatigue and autonomic nerve function. We and another study group have used functional magnetic resonance imaging to show that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and cingulate cortex are activated during the KPT [25,26]. We have also used positron emission tomography to evaluate regional cerebral blood flow, and showed that the orbitofrontal cortex is associated with fatigue sensation assessed by VAS [27]. As for autonomic nerve function, a central autonomic network that controls sympathetico-vagal balance is comprised of the orbitofrontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, insula, amygdala, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, hypothalamus, periaqueductal gray matter, pons and medulla oblongata [28,29]. The anterior cingulate cortex plays a particularly crucial role in the central control of sympathetico-vagal balance [30]. There are anatomical and functional connections between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and medial prefrontal cortex, including the anterior cingulate cortex and the orbitofrontal cortex [31-34]. This indicates that there are interactions between the activities of task-dependent regions, fatigue sensation-related regions and autonomic nerve function-associated regions. Sympathoexcitatory subcortical threat circuits are normally under the inhibitory control of the medial prefrontal cortex [35-37]. During the KPT, wider prefrontal areas including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and part of the medial prefrontal cortex were more active in the single-task session than in the dual-task session [26]. More extension activation of prefrontal regions, which reflect mental effort, is also related to fatigue during a verbal working memory task [38]. These results suggest that fatigue which induces greater prefrontal activity corresponds to mental effort to accurately answer the questions and results in decreases in parasympathetic nerve activity and inhibitory capacity for sympathoexcitatory response.
In the present study, we focused on the difference in autonomic nerve activity between eyes open and eyes closed conditions. Previously, sympathetic hyperactivity was observed in the eyes closed condition after a fatigue-inducing task had been performed for 30 min [10] and 8 h [11]. Although sympathetic and parasympathetic sinus modulation were similar in pre-KPT rest with eyes open and pre-KPT rest with eyes closed, sympathetic nerve activity was higher and parasympathetic nerve activity was lower in post-KPT rest with eyes open than in post-KPT rest with eyes closed. Because attention level is different between eyes open and eyes closed conditions, sympathetic nerve activity is thought to be higher in eyes open condition than in the eyes closed condition [15]. However, before performing the KPT, the extent of the difference in sympathetic sinus modulation between eyes open and closed conditions was not observed because the brain network, including the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices, which play an important role in the regulation of autonomic nerve activity [39], was not driven; thus, control capacity by these brain regions was sufficient to inhibit the increase in sympathetic sinus modulation and decrease in parasympathetic sinus modulation in the eyes open condition. Because these brain regions are activated during the KPT [25,26], the increase in sinus sympathetic modulation and decrease in parasympathetic sinus modulation in the eyes open condition could not be adequately inhibited after the KPT. Inhibition of parasympathetic sinus modulation and the correlation between this activity and fatigue sensation was especially prevalent in this condition, suggesting that a brief mental task can be used to evaluate the change in autonomic nerve activity with fatigue if the eyes open condition is used. However, fatigue sensation was also associated with a decrease in parasympathetic sinus modulation in the post-KPT rest with eyes closed. Therefore, the extent to which parasympathetic nerve activity is inhibited in the recovery phase of the resting state in the eyes-closed condition may depend on the extent of fatigue.
Some researchers have found a difference in the fatigability of females and males with regard to physical and muscle fatigue [40,41]. In the case of cognitive fatigue, performance of the Stroop test under fatigue was lower in females than in male [42]. In the present study, we recruited only healthy females. This was to simplify and strengthen the analysis, and because epidemiological studies have shown that the number of females with chronic fatigue in the general population is twice that of males [5,6] and the fatigue level of female university students is higher than that of males [7]. In support of our findings, sympathetico-vagal balance was associated with fatigue in female volunteers aged from 19–24 years, but not in male volunteers of the same age [43].
Fatigue-related alterations of autonomic nerve activity have been reported in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome [44], multiple sclerosis [45,46], and primary biliary cirrhosis [47]. Here, we have shown that a 16-min period of ECG is adequate for evaluating the association between baseline fatigue sensation and altered autonomic nerve activity during and after a brief cognitive load. It is possible that this fatigue measurement method is useful to assess the severity of symptoms and treatment effects in these patients, and the measurement does not place excessive burden on patients.
Limitations
There are limitations of this study. In order to generalize our results, further study involving a larger number of participants is essential. We did not follow the participants’ cycles of menstruation in the present study. Previous reports have reported an association between fatigue and menstruation [48,49], and future studies should consider participants’ menstruation cycles.
Conclusions
Fatigue sensation was correlated with a decrease in parasympathetic sinus modulation during and after a cognitive test lasting 4 min, suggesting that we established a practical fatigue measurement method that can be used to assess the association between fatigue sensation and changes in autonomic nerve activity induced by brief cognitive testing. This newly developed method may contribute to evaluating the extent of physiological fatigue in not only healthy people, but also in those with fatigue-related disorders [50]. In addition, these methods also might contribute to investigations into the effect of interventions on recovery from fatigue via normalization of parasympathetic nerve alterations [51,52].
Abbreviations
ANOVA: Analysis of variance; ECG: Electrocardiogram; HF: High-frequency component power; LF: Low-frequency component power; nu: Normalized unit; KPT: Kana pick-out test; TP: Total power; VAS: Visual analogue scale; VLF: Very low frequency component power.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ contributions
KM took part in planning and designing the experiment and cognitive tests, performed the data analyses and drafted the manuscript. KT contributed to correct the data, and helped performing the data analyses. HK contributed to the design and planning of experiment, and corrected the data and helped draft the manuscript. YW took part in planning and designing the experiment and helped draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Contributor Information
Kei Mizuno, Email: keimizuno@riken.jp.
Kanako Tajima, Email: kanakotajima@riken.jp.
Yasuyoshi Watanabe, Email: yywata@riken.jp.
Hirohiko Kuratsune, Email: kura@fuksi-kagk-u.ac.jp.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare of Japan (grant number, H25-Shinkei/Kin-Ippan-006 to HK). This work was also partially supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (MEXT) of Japan (grant number, 25282211 to KM) and the RIKEN Special Postdoctoral Researchers Program (grant number, K24232 to KM). We would like to thank Ms. Hitomi Taniguchi for her excellent technical assistance in performing the experiment and Forte Science Communications for editorial help with the manuscript.
References
- Chaudhuri A, Behan PO. Fatigue in neurological disorders. Lancet. 2004;363(9413):978–988. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15794-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen MK. The epidemiology of self-perceived fatigue among adults. Prev Med. 1986;15(1):74–81. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(86)90037-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pawlikowska T, Chalder T, Hirsch SR, Wallace P, Wright DJ, Wessely SC. Population based study of fatigue and psychological distress. BMJ. 1994;308(6931):763–766. doi: 10.1136/bmj.308.6931.763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe Y. In: Fatigue Science for Human Health. Watanabe Y, Evengård B, Natelson BH, Jason LA, Kuratsune H, editor. New York: Springer; 2008. Preface and mini-review: fatigue science for human health; pp. 5–11. [Google Scholar]
- Evengård B, Jacks A, Pedersen NL, Sullivan PF. The epidemiology of chronic fatigue in the Swedish Twin Registry. Psychol Med. 2005;35(9):1317–1326. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van’t Leven M, Zielhuis GA, van der Meer JW, Verbeek AL, Bleijenberg G. Fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome-like complaints in the general population. Eur J Public Health. 2009;20(3):251–257. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pikó B, Barabás K, Boda K. Epidemiology of psychosomatic symptoms and its effect on the self-evaluation of general health in university students. Orv Hetil. 1995;136(31):1667–1671. (in Hungarian) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beurskens AJ, Bültmann U, Kant I, Vercoulen JH, Bleijenberg G, Swaen GM. Fatigue among working people: validity of a questionnaire measure. Occup Environ Med. 2000;57(5):353–357. doi: 10.1136/oem.57.5.353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka M, Mizuno K, Tajima S, Sasabe T, Watanabe Y. Central nervous system fatigue alters autonomic nerve activity. Life Sci. 2009;84(7–8):235–239. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka M, Mizuno K, Yamaguti K, Kuratsune H, Fujii A, Baba H, Matsuda K, Nishimae A, Takesaka T, Watanabe Y. Autonomic nervous alterations associated with daily level of fatigue. Behav Brain Funct. 2011;7:46. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Yamaguti K, Kajimoto O, Kuratsune H, Watanabe Y. Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity. Behav Brain Funct. 2011;7:17. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Fukuda S, Imai-Matsumura K, Watanabe Y. Relationship between cognitive functions and prevalence of fatigue in elementary and junior high school students. Brain Dev. 2011;33(6):470–479. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2010.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomoda A, Mizuno K, Murayama N, Joudoi T, Igasaki T, Miyazaki M, Miike T. Event-related potentials in Japanese childhood chronic fatigue syndrome. J Pediatr Neurol. 2007;5:199–208. [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka M, Shigihara Y, Funakura M, Kanai E, Watanabe Y. Fatigue-associated alterations of cognitive function and electroencephalographic power densities. PLoS One. 2012;7(4):e34774. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hori K, Yamakawa M, Tanaka N, Murakami H, Kaya M, Hori S. Influence of sound and light on heart rate variability. J Hum Ergol. 2005;34(1–2):25–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furlanetto KC, Mantoani LC, Bisca G, Morita AA, Zabatiero J, Proenca M, Kovelis D, Pitta F. Reduction of physical activity in daily life and its determinants in smokers without airflow obstruction. Respirology. 2014;19(3):369–375. doi: 10.1111/resp.12236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamidovic A, De Wit H. Sleep deprivation increases cigarette smoking. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;93(3):263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giessing C, Thiel CM, Alexander-Bloch AF, Patel AX, Bullmore ET. Human brain functional network changes associated with enhanced and impaired attentional task performance. J Neurosci. 2013;33(14):5903–5914. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4854-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middlekauff HR, Park J, Agrawal H, Gornbein JA. Abnormal sympathetic nerve activity in women exposed to cigarette smoke: a potential mechanism to explain increased cardiac risk. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2013;305(10):H1560–1567. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00502.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanaya N, Hirata N, Kurosawa S, Nakayama M, Namiki A. Differential effects of propofol and sevoflurane on heart rate variability. Anesthesiology. 2003;98(1):34–40. doi: 10.1097/00000542-200301000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takusagawa M, Komori S, Umetani K, Ishihara T, Sawanobori T, Kohno I, Sano S, Yin D, Ijiri H, Tamura K. Alterations of autonomic nervous activity in recurrence of variant angina. Heart. 1999;82(1):75–81. doi: 10.1136/hrt.82.1.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawada Y, Ohtomo N, Tanaka Y, Tanaka G, Yamakoshi K, Terachi S, Shimamoto K, Nakagawa M, Satoh S, Kuroda S, Iimura O. New technique for time series analysis combining the maximum entropy method and non-linear least squares method: its value in heart rate variability analysis. Med Biol Eng Comput. 1997;35(4):318–322. doi: 10.1007/BF02534083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology (TFESC) & (North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (NASPE) Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation. 1996;93:1043–1065. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piccirillo G, Ogawa M, Song J, Chong VJ, Joung B, Han S, Magrì D, Chen LS, Lin SF, Chen PS. Power spectral analysis of heart rate variability and autonomic nervous system activity measured directly in healthy dogs and dogs with tachycardia-induced heart failure. Heart Rhythm. 2009;6(4):546–552. doi: 10.1016/j.hrthm.2009.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Tanabe HC, Sadato N, Watanabe Y. The neural substrates associated with attentional resources and difficulty of concurrent processing of the two verbal tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(8):1998–2009. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tachibana A, Noah JA, Bronner S, Ono Y, Hirano Y, Niwa M, Watanabe K, Onozuka M. Activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in a dual neuropsychological screening test: an fMRI approach. Behav Brain Funct. 2012;8:26. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-8-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tajima S, Yamamoto S, Tanaka M, Kataoka Y, Iwase M, Yoshikawa E, Okada H, Onoe H, Tsukada H, Kuratsune H, Ouchi Y, Watanabe Y. Medial orbitofrontal cortex is associated with fatigue sensation. Neurol Res Int. 2010;2010:671421. doi: 10.1155/2010/671421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benarroch EE. In: Clinical Autonomic Disorder. 2. Low PA, editor. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven; 1997. The central autonomic network; pp. 17–23. [Google Scholar]
- Loewy AD. In: Central Regulation of Autonomic Functions. Loewy AD, Spyer KM, editor. New York: Oxford Univ Press; 1990. Central autonomic pathways; pp. 88–103. [Google Scholar]
- Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Josephs O, O'Doherty J, Zanini S, Dewar BK, Cipolotti L, Shallice T, Dolan RJ. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 10):2139–2152. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koski L, Paus T. Functional connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex within the human frontal lobe: a brain-mapping meta-analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133(1):55–65. doi: 10.1007/s002210000400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paus T, Castro-Alamancos MA, Petrides M. Cortico-cortical connectivity of the human mid-dorsolateral frontal cortex and its modulation by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;14(8):1405–1411. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01757.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Pandya DN. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11(3):1011–1036. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt BA, Pandya DN. Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: II. Cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol. 1987;262(2):271–289. doi: 10.1002/cne.902620208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Bland ST, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(3):365–371. doi: 10.1038/nn1399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer JF, Sternberg E. Beyond heart rate variability: vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1088:361–372. doi: 10.1196/annals.1366.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer JF. On the importance of inhibition: central and peripheral manifestations of nonlinear inhibitory processes in neural systems. Dose Response. 2006;4(1):2–21. doi: 10.2203/dose-response.004.01.002.Thayer. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange G, Steffener J, Cook DB, Bly BM, Christodoulou C, Liu WC, Deluca J, Natelson BH. Objective evidence of cognitive complaints in chronic fatigue syndrome: a BOLD fMRI study of verbal working memory. Neuroimage. 2005;26(2):513–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang YY, Ma Y, Fan Y, Feng H, Wang J, Feng S, Lu Q, Hu B, Lin Y, Li J, Zhang Y, Wang Y, Zhou L, Fan M. Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(22):8865–8870. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904031106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter SK. Sex differences in human fatigability: mechanisms and insight to physiological responses. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 2014;210(4):768–789. doi: 10.1111/apha.12234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller ML, Pruse J, Yoon T, Schlinder-Delap B, Harkins A, Hunter SK. Supraspinal fatigue is similar in men and women for a low-force fatiguing contraction. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43(10):1873–1883. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318216ebd4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szinnai G, Schachinger H, Arnaud MJ, Linder L, Keller U. Effect of water deprivation on cognitive-motor performance in healthy men and women. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2005;289(1):R275–280. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00501.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park YB, Park YJ, Ko YI. Relationships of pulse waveform parameters to mood states and chronic fatigue. J Altern Complement Med. 2012;18(11):1050–1060. doi: 10.1089/acm.2011.0430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newton JL, Okonkwo O, Sutcliffe K, Seth A, Shin J, Jones DE. Symptoms of autonomic dysfunction in chronic fatigue syndrome. QJM. 2007;100(8):519–526. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcm057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keselbrener L, Akselrod S, Ahiron A, Eldar M, Barak Y, Rotstein Z. Is fatigue in patients with multiple sclerosis related to autonomic dysfunction? Clin Auton Res. 2000;10(4):169–175. doi: 10.1007/BF02291352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merkelbach S, Dillmann U, K?lmel C, Holz I, Muller M. Cardiovascular autonomic dysregulation and fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2001;7(5):320–326. doi: 10.1177/135245850100700508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newton JL, Davidson A, Kerr S, Bhala N, Pairman J, Burt J, Jones DE. Autonomic dysfunction in primary biliary cirrhosis correlates with fatigue severity. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2007;19(2):125–132. doi: 10.1097/01.meg.0000252629.96043.67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Negriff S, Dorn LD. Morningness/Eveningness and menstrual symptoms in adolescent females. J Psychosom Res. 2009;67(2):169–172. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods NF, Most A, Dery GK. Prevalene of perimenstrual symptoms. Am J Public Health. 1982;72(11):1257–1264. doi: 10.2105/ajph.72.11.1257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein DS, Robertson D, Esler M, Straus SE, Eisenhofer G. Dysautonomias: clinical disorders of the autonomic nervous system. Ann Intern Med. 2002;137(9):753–763. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-137-9-200211050-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tajima K, Tanaka M, Mizuno K, Okada N, Rokushima K, Watanabe Y. Effects of bathing in micro-bubbles on recovery from moderate mental fatigue. Ergon IJE HF. 2008;30(2):134–145. [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka M, Yamada H, Nakamura T, Watanabe Y. Effects of pellet stove on recovery from mental fatigue. Med Sci Monit. 2012;18(3):CR148–153. doi: 10.12659/MSM.882519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]