Abstract
A key component of genetic architecture is the allelic spectrum influencing trait variability. For autism spectrum disorder (henceforth autism) the nature of its allelic spectrum is uncertain. Individual risk genes have been identified from rare variation, especially de novo mutations1–8. From this evidence one might conclude that rare variation dominates its allelic spectrum, yet recent studies show that common variation, individually of small effect, has substantial impact en masse9,10. At issue is how much of an impact relative to rare variation. Using a unique epidemiological sample from Sweden, novel methods that distinguish total narrow-sense heritability from that due to common variation, and by synthesizing results from other studies, we reach several conclusions about autism’s genetic architecture: its narrow-sense heritability is ≈54% and most traces to common variation; rare de novo mutations contribute substantially to individuals’ liability; still their contribution to variance in liability, 2.6%, is modest compared to heritable variation.
Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder typified by striking deficits in social communication, and genetically by a mix of de novo and inherited variation contributing to liability. Rare variants clearly play a role, with the contribution of de novo variation being the most obvious and easy to characterize, but inherited variation also has a role in liability11,12. The contribution from inherited common variation is less substantiated. A handful of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have been conducted, significant findings have been few, and those are specific to a single GWAS13–15. The results mirror the early GWAS for schizophrenia, which in retrospect were underpowered, as witnessed by replicable associations involving common variants found from studies of tens of thousands of subjects10. In another parallel with early GWAS for schizophrenia, one of the first rays of hope for understanding how common variants impact liability was through the use of genetic scores, which were built from a large number of common variants and were shown to predict liability reliably16. For autism, scores also predict risk13. Some of the SNPs conferring risk to schizophrenia appear to confer risk to autism17, a result that compliments results for copy number variants (CNV)18,19.
A natural complement to genetic scores from common variants is the estimation of narrow-sense heritability from the same variants. Two recent studies estimate heritability attributable to common variation to be substantial9,10, yet one estimates heritability at roughly 50% (Fig. 1a), the other at 17%. As described in the reports9,10, there are several technical reasons for these differences, one being quite different study designs and another being ascertainment. As we shall show here, 50% appears more realistic.
In any case, neither of these values approaches early twin studies, which place autism heritability close to 1 (Supplementary Table 1 for data and discussion). Still results from early studies could be compatible with estimates from common variants (Fig. 2). The key issue is that early twin studies of autism assume that the genetic covariance of monozygotic twins is determined solely by additive effects, and that non-additive and de novo effects on monozygotic similarity can be ignored. These are dubious assumptions, creating ample room for the discrepancy between study designs. In contrast, a recent, large study of twins places heritability at 38%20 (Figs. 1a, 2) under the same assumptions.
To resolve this conundrum we are evaluating a population sample by a variety of genetic analyses to estimate the relative contribution of rare, common, inherited and de novo variation to overall liability. We have ascertained subjects with strict autism (“autistic disorder”) from a Swedish epidemiological sample (“Population-based Autism Genetics and Environment Study” or PAGES).
Concurrently a comprehensive study of autism in Sweden has been ongoing and it recently reported the largest study of familial risk to date21. This “Swedish family study”, a population-based cohort of all Swedish children born from 1982–2007 and a registry of all diagnoses prior to 2010, includes more than 1.6 million families with at least two children, yielding 5,799,875 cousin pairs, 2,642,064 full sibling pairs, 432,281 maternal half-sibling pairs, 445,531 paternal half-sibling pairs and 37,570 twins. Of the 14,516 cases of broad autism, 5,689 (39%) have a strict diagnosis. This massive homogeneous sample permits precise estimation of relative recurrence risk of autism, given the diagnosis in relatives from monozygotic twins to first cousins, and after modeling covariates such as sex, birth year, parental psychiatric history and parental age at birth. By analyzing these recurrence risks for additive and non-additive genetic effects and shared and non-shared environmental effects, the best model consists of only additive genetic and non-shared environmental effects and yields quite precise estimates of the narrow-sense heritability of autism (h2=54%, SE=5%).
The Swedish family study provides a sound foundation from which to address other questions about the genetic architecture of autism using PAGES. There are no major differences in the population and samples underlying both studies. To estimate heritability for PAGES, controls were sampled from the Swedish population and both cases and controls were genotyped on a common genotyping platform. After genotyping and quality control we analyzed data from 531,906 SNPs characterized on 3046 subjects, 466 with autism and 2580 subjects10 not known to be affected.
We used GCTA22 to estimate heritability due to common variants, i.e., SNP-based heritability. To ensure that all cases and controls were essentially unrelated (no pairs with kinship greater than 5th degree relatives), 151 individuals were excluded. The resulting estimate of total variance in liability explained by measured SNPs was 49.4% (SE=9.6) (Fig. 1a). This heritability estimate compared remarkably well with findings based on independent data from population samples and similar methods9. The common variation imparting this heritability was distributed roughly uniformly across chromosomes (Fig. 1b), an expectation of polygenic inheritance that is reflected in the significant correlation between heritability per chromosome and its size (r=0.49, p-value=0.018). Prevalence of strict autism, required to calculate heritability, was set to 0.3% (Fig. 1c; Supplementary Fig. 1) for these heritability calculations. The estimate is a lower bound for total narrow-sense heritability because it includes contributions from causal variants not tagged by the measured SNPs. Although synthetic association23 – a pileup of rare risk variants in linkage disequilibrium with a common variant – could account for a small fraction of this heritability, it cannot be large, as described below and previously24,25.
To obtain an estimate of heritability due to both common and rare variation, we next included more closely related individuals. In a traditional analysis of heritability, e.g., the Swedish family study, the relationship matrix is given. Instead we estimated these relationships from the SNPs genotyped for PAGES. Because estimates of relationships from SNP genotypes tend to be noisy, we used treelet covariance smoothing26 to improve estimates of pairwise relationships, especially for more distantly related individuals, and thereby refine estimates of heritability26. When we included relatives, albeit mostly distant (Supplementary Fig. 2), the estimated total heritability was 52.4% (SE=9.5). While estimates were somewhat sensitive to prevalence, the differences between SNP-based heritability versus those based on estimated relatedness were insensitive to prevalence (Fig. 1d): at any prevalence, the difference was approximately 3%. To evaluate how successful this approach could be at partitioning sources of heritability, we performed a simple simulation experiment that demonstrates that we can successfully partition heritability into the portion explained by common and rare variants (Methods).
Previous work has shown that autism heritability could fluctuate substantially when the bulk of the sample was comprised of simplex families, which lowers heritability, versus multiplex families, which raises heritability9. These observations are consistent with liability being a classical quantitative genetic trait. Because PAGES is population based, it has no obvious simplex/multiplex ascertainment bias. Still there could be sources of more subtle bias. Based on the conjecture that subjects with intellectual disability and autism have a greater fraction of liability determined by de novo variants, one obvious bias would be misclassification of individuals who are comorbid for intellectual disability and autism as one or the other. To evaluate this issue we first determined the diagnostic classification of Swedes, according to governmental records. In this population 43.6% of subjects with autism also have intellectual disability, a rate comparable to other populations (note that strict autism has a higher rate of comorbid intellectual disability than broad autism). Next, to determine if IQ has a substantial impact on heritability, we contrast two estimates from data from the Autism Genome Project: using the full sample, heritability equaled 51.1% (SE=4.8, N=2097); whereas, for subjects with IQ > 80, it was slightly but not significantly larger (59.3%, SE=7.8, N=871). Subjects in the sample meet broad criteria for an autism diagnosis; for the subset of individuals given a strict diagnosis, heritability was 52.3% (SE=6.2, N=1242).
Next we asked how much of the variance in liability to autism could be explained by de novo mutations, applying the standard liability model to reported rates of de novo CNVs and loss-of-function (LoF) mutations in autistic subjects and their siblings from the Simons Simplex Collection. In this sample, structured to enrich for de novo CNV and LoF mutations, their contribution to the variance in liability is 2.6% (Supplementary Note; Supplementary Tables 2–3). Yet de novo events can have a large impact on liability and 14% of subjects carry such mutations: roughly 80% of subjects that are carriers of a de novo CNV would not be affected if they were not carriers; likewise, for carriers of de novo LoF mutations, 57% would not be affected (Supplementary Note).
The estimate of heritability could indirectly include dominant or non-additive effects but should not include the impact of recessive inheritance. A recent study estimated the contribution from rare, recessive variation to be about 3%27, a contribution similar to that from additive effects of rare variants. Rare hemizygous LoF mutations accounted for another 2% of liability.
We conclude that inherited rare variation explains a smaller fraction of total heritability compared to common variation (Fig. 2). While uncertainty is inherent in all of these estimates (Fig. 1), results converge on total heritability in the range of 50–60% with common variants explaining the bulk of it. Our analyses illustrate an approach to identify the contribution of rare and common variation to the heritability of any phenotype28. Estimating the total contribution of genetic variation to variation in liability, which includes non-additive effects and de novo variation, is more challenging. If the only non-additive effects of genes were due solely to recessive inheritance, this would add rough 5% to the total, but that estimate could be low based on both theoretical and empirical grounds29,30. And, while 14% of affected subjects carry de novo CNV and LoF mutations, the contribution of these mutations to the variance in liability is only 2.6%. Summing these estimates suggests that genetic variation accounts for roughly 60% of the variation in risk for autism in Sweden, implying that the majority of risk is due to genetic variation.
By contrast, a recent twin study finds that shared twin environment accounts for the majority of the variation in risk, 55%, based on a population sample of Californians from the USA. These different populations could have different genetic architectures or there could be an unknown sampling bias. Alternatively the California study fits many parameters to a relatively small data set – concordance rates on 54 monozygotic pairs and 138 dizygotic pairs – from which the study selects the best model based on statistical criteria. For small samples, however, the correct model, the one truly generating the data, can be quite different in structure from the selected model and yet the two can have only small differences in likelihood. It is possible that the distinct conclusion of the California study, versus others of its design, is due to a modest stochastic difference that altered model selection. In this regard a cautionary note for all such studies, including ours, is worthwhile: while we assume here a simple model structure, ours is but one of many possible models that could underlie trait covariance (e.g.31); the assumed model can alter inference, sometimes substantially; and many of these models can fit the data almost equally well. Nonetheless, that all Swedish studies, regardless of design, converge on similar estimates of heritability lends strong support for our conclusion that the bulk of risk for autism arises from genetic variation.
Data used in the preparation of this article reside in the NIH-supported National Database for Autism Research (NDAR), in [dataset identifier].
Online Methods
Ascertainment of subjects
We developed an epidemiological sample of autism or, more precisely, autistic disorder taking advantage of the detailed birth and medical registries and universal access to health care. Our sample frame was the medical birth register including all births in Sweden, where there is mandatory screening of all children at age 4 for neurodevelopmental disorders. The medical registries included all individuals diagnosed with autistic disorder at any time. Cases with autistic disorder (ICD-9 codes: 299A or ICD-10 codes: F84.0–F84.1), henceforth autism, were identified from the Swedish National Patient Register (NPR). Controls free from schizophrenia or bipolar disease were recruited from the general Swedish population matched by county, gender and birth year. Prevalence was 30 cases per 10,000 for autism and approximately 100 cases per 10,000 for the more inclusive broad autism diagnosis (Supplementary Figure 1). Inclusion criteria were diagnosis of autism in the NPR; born in Sweden; both parents born in a Nordic country; between age 10–65 years; and signed consent by a parent or a legal guardian (or by the subject, when possible and appropriate). Exclusion criteria were individuals with a diagnosis of autism, but who also had a genetic disorder known to be associated with autistic features (e.g., Fragile X, Down and Klinefelter syndromes); or medical or psychiatric history that could mitigate a confident diagnosis of autism. In this way 536 autism subjects were recruited from 12 counties in Sweden.
Genetic characterization
Samples were genotyped on the Illumina HumanOmniExpressExome BeadChip. Here we analyzed only the OmniExpress content of > 715,000 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) across the genome. Duplicate samples and samples with genotype completion rates < 98% were removed, resulting in a final sample of 3046 individuals, of which 466 were autism cases and 2580 were controls. We controlled for more subtle population structure using seven significant dimensions of ancestry as covariates in all subsequent analyses (n=3044, omitting one of each set of twins).
Heritability
To estimate heritability the Swedish family study relied on an extended sibling design, which included full siblings, half siblings, cousins and twins. The design facilitated estimation of additive and non-additive genetic sources of variance, as well as shared and non-shared environmental sources of variance.
For all genetic analyses of heritability, SNPs with minor allele frequency MAF > 0.05 were evaluated using the program GCTA22 to produce an estimated genetic relationship matrix (GRM). As described further in the Supplementary Note, we then modeled the case-control status via the mixed linear model y = Xβ + g + e, where y is the vector of case-control status, β is the vector of coefficients for the fixed effects (7 ancestry dimensions) with associated design matrix X, g, is the vector of random additive genetic effects associated with SNPs, and e is a vector of random errors, which were assumed to be independent. To obtain estimates of the heritability, variance of the phenotypes was expressed as (where A is the genetic relationship matrix (GRM) and I is an identity matrix, while and partition the total phenotypic variation into pieces attributable to additive genetic effects and random error, respectively) and heritability calculated as on the observed scale, which is transformed to the liability scale as a function of the population prevalence (K).
To estimate heritability due to common SNPs we used GCTA, applying to a GRM calculated based on a sample of essentially unrelated individuals (A < .025). To estimate total narrow-sense heritability we included all sampled individuals, computed the GRM, smoothed this matrix using Treelet Covariance smoothing26 (TCS), and then computed heritability from the GCTA package. See Supplementary Note for implementation of TCS. We used simulations to assess accuracy of this procedure of estimating heritability (see Supplementary Note for complete details). We started with phased genomes (haplotypes) of individuals from the HapMap 3 database, selecting two populations of European ancestry (CEU and TSI), and utilizing the available haplotypes we generated a large sample of haplotypes, representative of those that might be sampled from unrelated founders of a population. After generating haplotype pairs, chromosomes were randomly assigned to founders in each of 100 families and the founder chromosomes dropped through a five-generation pedigree. One hundred sets of independent pedigrees, including 20 individuals sampled per pedigree, were combined to generate the full genotype sample of size 2000. For the given set of genotypes, 50 independent vectors of phenotypes were simulated. For each simulation a random set of causal variants were chosen: 1000 rare (MAF < .01) and 1000 common variants. These two classes of SNPs generated 25% and 50% of the heritability (h2), respectively, for a total of h2 = 75%. Using GCTA to estimate h2 solely from common variant genotypes – after removing relatives – the mean h2 = 50.7% (SE = 3.5). That this estimate is close to the simulated value for common variants suggests that the impact of synthetic association is minimal. Next, applying GCTA to genotypes from common variants and the full sample, including relatives, produces mean h2 = 72.4% (SE = 1.2) with TCS and mean h2 = 70.6% (SE = 1.2) without TCS. Both capture most of the h2 due to rare variation.
Impact of clinical features on estimates of heritability, exemplified by diagnosis and intellectual function
Consistent with quantitative genetics theory it has already been shown that families who are multiplex for autism carry a larger load of liability alleles relative to simplex families (defined as families with only one affected subject within the set of first and second degree relatives). Clinical phenotypes could also affect heritability/genetic load, although how much impact they might have is an open question. To evaluate this question we evaluated two phenotypes thought to have major impact on the genetics of autism, namely diagnosis per se and higher versus lower functioning, as measured by IQ. First, by linking registry data from Sweden, an estimate of the fraction of subjects with autism and intellectual disability (IQ < 70) was obtained to determine its comparability to other population samples. To assess the impact of diagnosis we use Autism Genome Project (AGP) data and follow the AGP by analyzing strict autism, as defined by meeting criteria for autism on the ADI-R and ADOS, versus broad autism, which includes autism disorder and subjects who meet looser criteria for a spectrum diagnosis (see Supplementary Note). For IQ we target subjects with IQ ≥ 80, beyond the bound for intellectual disability. After quality control, there were 2097 AGP cases13 and 1663 HABC controls9 genotyped for 828,352 markers. After analysis using GCTA we observed heritabilities of 51.1±4.8%, 52.3±6.2%, and 59.3±7.8 % for broad autism, strict autism, and autism with IQ ≥ 80, respectively.
Meta analysis of heritability
A meta estimate of h2 due to common variants can be derived by taking a weighted average of two estimates of this quantity obtained from two independent samples: the PAGES study (h2 = 49.4%, SE = 9.5) and, the other, 1242 strict autism subjects from AGP data (h2 = 52.3%, SE = 6.2). We did not use the estimate based on the SSC (provided in Figure 1) because the SSC ascertainment of only simplex families induces a negative bias on the estimate. Meta-analysis produced h2 = 51.4% (SE = 5.2) and corresponding 95% confidence interval (41.0–61.8). Contrasting this with the total h2 obtained from the Swedish family study (h2 = 54%, SE = 5) produced an estimate of h2 due to rare variants (SE = 7.2, CI = (0, 17%)).
Estimating the contribution of de novo mutations and heritable variation to liability and variation in liability to autism
For motivation and computational methods, see Supplementary Note. To estimate the variance in liability explained by de novo variation, results from the SSC were analyzed, contrasting the rate of de novo copy number variants (CNVs), loss of function (LoF) mutations and missense mutations. All three have been shown to be significantly in excess in autism probands, relative to their unaffected siblings, although not all studies found de novo missense variation to be in excess1–5,7,34. For inference, we assumed the excess proportion of cases carrying de novo mutations, relative to control siblings, conferred liability.
De novo CNVs
As described further in the Supplementary Note, 75 de novo CNVs were found in 858 probands and 19 de novo CNVs in 863 sibling controls34 (relative risk=4.25). Assuming an ‘exposure rate’ of 0.022 (=19/863), the classical liability model determined that de novo CNVs accounted for 1.46% of the variability on the liability scale.
De novo LoF mutations
72 of 599 autism probands had a de novo LoF mutation compared with 32 of the 599 sibling controls1,4,35 (relative risk =2.42). For exposure rate of 0.053, de novo LoF mutations accounted for 1.11% of the variance in liability.
De novo missense mutations
253 out of 599 probands had at least one de novo missense mutation compared with 238 of 599 sibling controls (relative risk =1.11). For exposure rate of 0.397, de novo missense mutations accounted for negligible variance in liability (0.04%).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Grants MH057881 and MH097849, and also in part through the computational resources and staff expertise provided by the Scientific Computing Facility at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai. We thank the Mount Sinai Genomics Core Facility for carrying out Illumina bead array genotyping. We thank Drs. David Cutler, Mark Daly and Shaun Purcell for comments on the manuscript and Drs. Daly and Patrick Sullivan for facilitating access to control samples, collected and genotyped by Drs. Daly, Hultman, Sklar, and Sullivan, and supported by NIMH grants MH095034 and MH077139. We also thank the nurses, Ann-Kristin Sundberg and Ann-Britt Holmgren, for their hard work in collecting the samples. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and does not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH.
Footnotes
Author Contributions
AR, CMH, BD, KR, and JDB conceived the project and designed its components. AR, CMH, BD, KR, and JDB identified funding for the study. SS, OS, and CHM were responsible for ascertaining case samples and PS and CHM for ascertaining control samples. JR, DM, and MM were responsible for genotyping the case samples. APG organized and managed the data files and TG and APG carried out quality control of the SNP data. SJS carried out simulations and additional analyses to assess contribution of rare variation to variance in liability, while SR carried out imputation to 1KG. TG, CAB, and ABL carried out statistical analyses under the guidance of BD, LK and KR, while YP, SS, OS, AR, and CHM carried out epidemiological analyses. BD, KR, and JDB took the lead in writing the manuscript and all authors reviewed and approved the manuscript.
Competing Financial Interests
The authors have no competing financial interests.
References
- 1.Iossifov I, et al. De novo gene disruptions in children on the autistic spectrum. Neuron. 2012;74:285–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Neale BM, et al. Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2012;485:242–245. doi: 10.1038/nature11011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.O'Roak BJ, et al. Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network of de novo mutations. Nature. 2012;485:246–250. doi: 10.1038/nature10989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sanders SJ, et al. De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are strongly associated with autism. Nature. 2012;485:237–241. doi: 10.1038/nature10945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sebat J, et al. Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science. 2007;316:445–449. doi: 10.1126/science.1138659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Glessner JT, et al. Autism genome-wide copy number variation reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature. 2009;459:569–573. doi: 10.1038/nature07953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Sanders SJ, et al. Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including duplications of the 7q11.23 Williams syndrome region, are strongly associated with autism. Neuron. 2011;70:863–885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Pinto D, et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2014;94:677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.03.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Klei L, et al. Common genetic variants, acting additively, are a major source of risk for autism. Mol Autism. 2012;3:9. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-3-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lee SH, et al. Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat Genet. 2013;45:984–994. doi: 10.1038/ng.2711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.State MW, Levitt P. The conundrums of understanding genetic risks for autism spectrum disorders. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1499–1506. doi: 10.1038/nn.2924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Devlin B, Scherer SW. Genetic architecture in autism spectrum disorder. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2012;22:229–237. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2012.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Anney R, et al. Individual common variants exert weak effects on the risk for autism spectrum disorderspi. Hum Mol Genet. 2012;21:4781–4792. doi: 10.1093/hmg/dds301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wang K, et al. Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate with autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2009;459:528–533. doi: 10.1038/nature07999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Weiss LA, et al. A genome-wide linkage and association scan reveals novel loci for autism. Nature. 2009;461:802–808. doi: 10.1038/nature08490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.International Schizophrenia, C. et al. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009;460:748–752. doi: 10.1038/nature08185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, C. et al. Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat Genet. 2013;45:984–994. doi: 10.1038/ng.2711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Stefansson H, et al. CNVs conferring risk of autism or schizophrenia affect cognition in controls. Nature. 2014;505:361–366. doi: 10.1038/nature12818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Talkowski ME, et al. Sequencing chromosomal abnormalities reveals neurodevelopmental loci that confer risk across diagnostic boundaries. Cell. 2012;149:525–537. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hallmayer J, et al. Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:1095–1102. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sandin S, et al. The familial risk of autism. J Am Med Assoc. 2014 in press. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Yang J, Lee SH, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. GCTA: a tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. Am J Hum Genet. 2011;88:76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dickson SP, Wang K, Krantz I, Hakonarson H, Goldstein DB. Rare variants create synthetic genome-wide associations. PLoS Biol. 2010;8:e1000294. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Orozco G, Barrett JC, Zeggini E. Synthetic associations in the context of genome-wide association scan signals. Hum Mol Genet. 2010;19:R137–R144. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddq368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wray NR, Purcell SM, Visscher PM. Synthetic associations created by rare variants do not explain most GWAS results. PLoS Biol. 2011;9:e1000579. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Crossett A, Lee AB, Klei L, Devlin B, Roeder K. Refining genetically inferred relationships using treelet covariance smoothing. Annals of Applied Statistics. 2013;7:669–690. doi: 10.1214/12-AOAS598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lim ET, et al. Rare complete knockouts in humans: population distribution and significant role in autism spectrum disorders. Neuron. 2013;77:235–242. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Agarwala V, Flannick J, Sunyaev S, Altshuler D. Evaluating empirical bounds on complex disease genetic architecture. Nat Genet. 2013;45:1418–1427. doi: 10.1038/ng.2804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zuk O, Hechter E, Sunyaev SR, Lander ES. The mystery of missing heritability: Genetic interactions create phantom heritability. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:1193–1198. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119675109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lynch M, Walsh B. Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Devlin B, Daniels M, Roeder K. The heritability of IQ. Nature. 1997;388:468–471. doi: 10.1038/41319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lichtenstein P, Carlstrom E, Rastam M, Gillberg C, Anckarsater H. The genetics of autism spectrum disorders and related neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:1357–1363. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10020223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lundstrom S, et al. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic like traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:46–52. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Levy D, et al. Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuron. 2011;70:886–897. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Willsey AJ, et al. Coexpression networks implicate human midfetal deep cortical projection neurons in the pathogenesis of autism. Cell. 2013;155:997–1007. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.