Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014 Jun 13;38(7):2024–2033. doi: 10.1111/acer.12446

Directional Relationships Between Alcohol Use and Antisocial Behavior Across Adolescence

Seung-Bin Cho 1, Jon Heron 1, Fazil Aliev 1, Jessica E Salvatore 1, Glyn Lewis 1, John Macleod 1, Matthew Hickman 1, Barbara Maughan 1, Kenneth S Kendler 1, Danielle M Dick 1
PMCID: PMC4139289  NIHMSID: NIHMS606212  PMID: 24930394

Abstract

Background

The co-occurrence of alcohol use and antisocial behavior is well established, but different hypotheses exist regarding the direction of effects between the 2 behaviors. We used longitudinal data to examine the directional relationship between the 2 behaviors across adolescence.

Methods

A cross-lagged model was applied to longitudinal data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. The sample used in the present study consisted of 4,354 females and 3,984 males. Alcohol use and antisocial behavior were measured with multiple items collected at 12, 13, 15, and 17 years of age.

Results

Both alcohol use and antisocial behavior were highly stable, as evidenced by highly significant autoregressive paths. Regarding the cross-lagged paths, neither behavior was predictive of the other during early adolescence (between ages 12 and 13). During mid-to late adolescence (from ages 13 to 17), antisocial behavior was predictive of subsequent alcohol use. Alcohol use was predictive of antisocial behavior in late adolescence (between ages 15 and 17), although this relationship was mainly driven by males and was not significant in the female subgroup.

Conclusions

The result generally supported the direction from antisocial behavior to alcohol use, especially during mid-to late adolescence. However, there was also a suggestion that the direction of relationship between the 2 behaviors changes across adolescence. The results highlight the importance of considering developmental stages to understand the directional relationships between the 2 behaviors.

Keywords: ALSPAC, Alcohol Use, Antisocial Behavior, Directional Relationship, Longitudinal Design


The co-occurrence of antisocial behavior with alcohol use is one of the most robust findings in the alcohol field (Armstrong and Costello, 2002; Molina et al., 2002; Moss and Lynch, 2001; Sanford, 2001). For example, in a review of studies on adolescent substance use/abuse and psychiatric comorbidity (Armstrong and Costello, 2002), conduct disorder was the most commonly diagnosed psychiatric condition among adolescents who used or abused alcohol and other drugs. In another study, conduct disorder was the only diagnosis that showed a significant association with alcohol problems in both men and women (Moss and Lynch, 2001).

Developmentally, antisocial behavior precedes alcohol use (Kuperman et al., 2001), and, more importantly, antisocial behavior in early adolescence and childhood is an important predictor of alcohol-related problems in late adolescence and in early adulthood (Englund et al., 2008; Merline et al., 2008; Windle, 1990). This may suggest a causal relationship from antisocial behavior to alcohol use. However, there is also support for different types of directional hypotheses between the 2 behaviors. Researches that focused on the relatively short-term pharmacological (disinhibitory) effects of alcohol in the context of experimental studies generally supported the effect of alcohol to cause or aggravate antisocial behavior (Bushman and Cooper, 1990; Cherek et al., 1985; Miczek et al., 2004). In addition, several longitudinal studies have also found longer term effects of alcohol use on antisocial behavior (Brook et al., 1998; Ellickson et al., 2003; Maldonado-Molina et al., 2011; Resnick et al., 2004) due to chronic damage to the central nervous system (Howard, 2006) and/or impaired social functioning (Ellickson et al., 2003) associated with extended alcohol use. It is also possible that the relationship is reciprocal (White et al., 1999).

Another possibility is that the 2 behaviors co-occur due to a common set of etiological factors. This has been supported by phenotypic and genetic studies. Studies on the phenotypic covariance structure of comorbid psychiatric disorders have consistently reported higher order internalizing and externalizing factors among individual disorders (Krueger et al., 1998, 2002). In these studies, the externalizing factor included alcohol and other substance use disorders and antisocial behavior (including conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder). The externalizing factor was distinct from the internalizing factor, which can be divided into 2 subfactors: distress and fear. The distress factor included major depression, dysthymia, and generalized anxiety disorder, and the fear factor included phobic and panic disorders (Krueger, 1999; Watson, 2005). Analyses of genetically informative data have suggested that a shared genetic liability contributes to the common externalizing factor (Dick et al., 2005; Kendler et al., 2003; Krueger et al., 2002; Slutske et al., 1998). Association studies have found candidate genes (e.g., GABRA2 and CHRM2) that are associated with both conduct/behavior problems and alcohol use/misuse (Dick et al., 2006, 2008, 2009).

Longitudinal data provide an opportunity to test directional relationships from earlier events to later events in cases where experimental manipulation is not possible. We are aware of 2 previous cross-lagged analyses of the relationship between alcohol use and antisocial behavior during adolescence. In a study of 1,380 adolescents measured at ages of 12, 15, and 18 (White et al., 1993), aggression at age 12 predicted alcohol use and alcohol-related aggressive behaviors at 15, but alcohol use at either 12 or 15 years of age did not predict aggressive behaviors at 15 or 18 years of age, respectively. In another study that followed 2,586 adolescents at ages 11, 13, and 15 years (Young et al., 2008), the cross-lagged paths from antisocial behaviors to alcohol use were significant at all time points, but no path from alcohol use to antisocial behaviors was significant. Accordingly, both studies supported the effect of antisocial behavior on subsequent alcohol use, but did not provide support for the effects of alcohol use on subsequent antisocial behavior.

The relationship between alcohol use and antisocial behavior may not necessarily be the same in males and females. Sex differences in cross-lagged relationships were previously observed (Silberg et al., 2003; Young et al., 2008). In Silberg and colleagues (2003), the cross-lagged correlation between early alcohol use and later conduct disorder was noticeably higher in males than in females (0.25 in males and 0.17 in females). Young and colleagues (2008) observed a cross-lagged coefficient from antisocial behavior to alcohol use between ages 13 and 15 that was larger in males compared to females (0.23 for males and 0.11 for females). Although differences in the cross-lagged relationships were observed in these studies, sex differences were not formally tested (Silberg et al., 2003; Young et al., 2008), or the model could not be fit in the female subsample due to the low prevalence of antisocial behavior (White et al., 1993). Studies of sex differences in the comorbidity of alcohol use and antisocial behavior have yielded mixed results. Alcoholism subtypes that are characterized by high antisociality are generally overrepresented in alcoholic males as compared to alcoholic females (Epstein et al., 2002; Moss et al., 2007; Pombo and Lesch, 2009). However, the relationship between alcohol use disorder and antisocial behaviors was not particularly stronger in men in recent studies that adjusted for socioeconomic status and other comorbid psychiatric conditions (Dawson et al., 2010; Goldstein et al., 2012). Furthermore, sex differences in the prevalence of alcohol use disorders and comorbidity with antisocial behavior have not necessarily been replicated in adolescent samples (Johnston et al., 2012; Moffitt et al., 2001; Young et al., 2002). To fill this gap in the literature, we tested for sex differences in the relationships between the 2 behaviors using multiple group structural equation modeling in a sample that was sufficiently powered to detect sex differences.

In this study, we applied a cross-lagged model to measures of alcohol use and antisocial behavior from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) to examine the directional relationships between the 2 behaviors. ALSPAC is a large population-based sample that has followed a cohort of more than 10,000 children since their mothers were pregnant. Here, we used measures of alcohol use and antisocial behavior collected at 12, 13, 15, and 17 years of age to examine the interrelationship between antisocial behavior and alcohol use across this age range. This represents, to our knowledge, the largest cross-lagged analysis to examine the directional relationships between antisocial behavior and alcohol use to date. The sample size and prevalence rates of the 2 behaviors were large enough to allow us to formally test potential sex differences in the relationships between the 2 behaviors.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Sample

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is an ongoing population-based prospective study designed to understand the genetic and environmental factors that influence health and development. All pregnant women identified as residing in Bristol and the surrounding areas of South West England with an expected date of delivery between April 1, 1991 and December 31, 1992 were invited to participate. Among 20,248 eligible pregnancies during 1990 to 1992, the mothers of 14,541 pregnancies (71.8%) were enrolled, with 13,988 live infants at 1 year from birth. Postnatal recruitment was conducted to further increase the enrollment rate and to improve the representativeness of the ALSPAC sample. Postnatal recruitment efforts at ages 7 to 18 years added 713 children (4.85% of overall ALSPAC sample) from 706 pregnancies yielding total of 14,701 live-born children from 15,247 enrolled pregnancies (75.3%) (Boyd et al., 2013). Please note that the study website contains details of all the data that are available through a fully searchable data dictionary at “http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary.”

We used a subset of the ALSPAC sample for which measures of alcohol use and antisocial behavior were available at 12, 13, 15, and 17 years of age. Participants’ average ages at assessment were 12.8, 13.8, 15.5, and 17.8 years for alcohol use, and 12.8, 13.9, 15.5, and 17.8 for antisocial behavior. Of 8,338 adolescents who had either alcohol use or antisocial behavior data at 1 or more measurement occasions and had nonmissing responses on sex were included in our analysis (Table 1). Our sample included 381 postnatally recruited participants (4.57%), which is similar proportion of postnatal recruitment in the overall ALSPAC sample (4.85%). Among included participants, 4,354 (52.2%) were females and 3,984 (47.8%) were males. Descriptive statistics of key demographic variables for the overall ALSPAC sample and individuals included in this study are summarized in Table 2. Compared to the overall ALSPAC sample, individuals included in this study were more likely to (i) be female, (ii) be White, and (iii) have parents with professional occupations and college degree or higher education levels. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees.

Table 1.

Number of Individuals and Measurement Items at Each Age

Antisocial behavior Alcohol use


Age n No. of items n No. of items
12.8 6,738 9 3,628 3
13.8/13.9a 6,172 10 3,558 3
15.5 3,325 16 3,399 15
17.8 3,952 6 4,202 14
a

Age 13 alcohol use and antisocial behavior were measured at 13.8 and 13.9 years, respectively.

Table 2.

Demographics of Samples Included in this Study and Overall ALSPAC Sample

Demographics In this study (%) ALSPAC (%)
Sex
  Male 3,984 (47.8) 7,573 (51.5)
  Female 4,354 (52.2) 7,121 (48.5)
Ethnicity
  Non-White 905 (10.8) 3,034 (20.1)
  White 7,443 (89.2) 12,068 (79.9)
Social class-mother
  Professional 461 (7.0) 596 (5.9)
  Managerial & technical 2,250 (34.3) 3,180 (31.4)
  Skilled: nonmanual 2,773 (42.2) 4,326 (42.8)
  Skilled: manual 440 (6.7) 791 (7.8)
  Partly skilled 536 (8.2) 997 (9.9)
  Unskilled 102 (1.6) 221 (2.2)
  Armed forces 2 (<0.1) 4 (<0.1)
Social class-father
  Professional 900 (12.9) 1,205 (10.9)
  Managerial & technical 2,504 (35.9) 3,749 (34.0)
  Skilled: nonmanual 830 (11.9) 1,199 (10.9)
  Skilled: manual 1,988 (28.5) 3,464 (31.4)
  Partly skilled 583 (8.4) 1,078 (09.8)
  Unskilled 163 (2.3) 316 (02.9)
  Armed forces 11 (0.2) 28 (0.3)
Highest education-mother
  Nondegree 7,154 (85.5) 13,586 (89.4)
  Degree 1,218 (14.5) 1,607 (10.6)
Highest education-father
  Nondegree 6,540 (80.1) 12,531 (85.2)
  Degree 1,620 (19.9) 2,181 (14.8)

Measures

Alcohol use and related problems were measured by computerized questionnaires during in-person clinic sessions. Items that measured alcohol use and related problems were adapted from the Semi-Structured Assessment of the Genetics of Alcoholism interview, developed by the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (Bucholz et al., 1994; Hesselbrock et al., 1999). These items corresponded to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th Edition (DSM-IV); American Psychiatric Association, 1994 criteria of alcohol abuse and dependence. Antisocial behavior items were adapted from the Edinburgh Study of Youth Transitions and Crime (ESYTC) (Smith and McVie, 2003) and represented various domains of delinquent behaviors, such as theft, violence, and truancy. At ages 12, 15, and 17, the same computerized questionnaire protocol that was used for alcohol use was used to measure antisocial behavior during in-person clinic sessions. At age 13, measures of antisocial behavior were not available from the same computerized questionnaire, and we instead used antisocial behavior data collected by mailed questionnaire, which was administered when participants’ average age was 13.9 years. Some items were excluded from the analyses because of low endorsement rates, which caused convergence problems with parameter estimation. The measurement items used in this study are listed in Tables 3 and 4 for alcohol use and antisocial behavior, respectively.

Table 3.

Alcohol Use Items

Items Key wordsa
12 years
  Had 3 or more drinks 3+ drinks
  Had whole drink Whole drink
  Maximum number of drinks in a typical week Typical
13 years
  Had whole drink Whole drink
  Maximum number of drinks in a typical week Typical
  Ever had 3 or more drinks 3+ drinks – ever
15 years
  Spent great deal of a day drinking Day drinking
  Had a blackout Blackout
  Felt the need to cut back or stop Cut back
  Parents/friends complained Complaints
  Had 4 or more drinks 4+ drinks
  Continued drinking despite problems Continued drinking
  Used alcohol in dangerous situations Dangerous situations
  Had 5 or more drinks 5+ drinks
  Gotten into fights when drinking Fights
  Not done what would usually do Daily tasks
  Had whole drink Whole drink
  Typical drinking Typical
  Had been physically hurt Physical injury
  Had problems with police Problem with police
  Went over limits set Over limits
17 years
  Spent great deal of day drinking Day drinking
  Had problems with police Problem with police
  Unable to remember what happened the night before Blackout
  Felt the need to cut back or stop Cut back
  Friend/parent/doctor complained Complaints
  Continued despite problems 4+ drinks
  Had 6 or more drinks 6+ drinks
  Gotten into fights when drinking Fights
  Failed to do what was expected Daily tasks
  Unable to keep up with school work etc. School
  Typical drinking Typical
  Frequency of drinking Frequency
  Has been injured Physical injury
  Unable to stop drinking Limits
a

tem parameters (loadings and thresholds) were constrained equal across time for the items with the same key word.

Table 4.

Antisocial Behavior Items

Items Key wordsa
12 years
  Ran away from home and stayed out overnight Stay out
  Stole something, victim not present Theft
  Physically cruel to somebody Cruelty – personal
  Child destroyed/broke something Damage
13 years
  Deliberately damaged or destroyed property Damage
  Has been rowdy or rude in a public place Rude
  Taken money or something else from school Theft – school
  Taken money or something else from shops Theft – shops
  Skipped school Truancy
  Not paid the correct fare or not paid on a bus or train Fare
  Hit, kicked, or punched someone on purpose Violence – physical
  Written things or sprayed paint on a property Graffiti
  Carried a knife or weapon with them for protection Weapon
15 years
  Set fire or tried to set fire to something on purpose Arson
  Ignored someone they know on purpose Bully
  Deliberately damaged or destroyed property Damage
  Sold an illegal drug to someone Drug dealing
  Has been rowdy or rude in a public place Rude
  Hit, spat, or thrown stones at someone they know Beating
  Sold something that did not belong to them or stolen Trade stolen
  Stolen something from a shop or store Theft – shops
  Threatened to hurt someone they know Threat
  Traveled on a bus/train without paying No fare
  Ridden in a stolen car/van/motorbike Vehicle
  Hit/kicked/punched a brother, sister, or someone else on purpose Beating – siblings
  Said nasty things to someone they know Insult
  Written things or sprayed paint on property Graffiti
  Carried a knife or other weapon with them Weapon
17 years
  Damaged or destroyed property Damage
  Sold an illegal drug to someone during last year Drug dealing
  Got in trouble with the police Police
  Was loud, rowdy or unruly in a public place Rude
  Stole something from a shop or store Theft – shops
  Hit, kicked, punched or attacked someone with weapon Violence – physical
a

Item parameters (loadings and thresholds) were constrained equal across time for the Items with the same key word.

Statistical Analysis

Before applying a cross-lagged model to the data, we examined the unidimensionality of the alcohol use and antisocial behavior items at each occasion using exploratory factor analysis (EFA). We also examined the mean and covariance structure of the alcohol use and antisocial behavior factors by fitting a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with unstructured factor means and covariance matrix. This model allows the estimation of the mean and covariance of factors of alcohol use and antisocial behavior with the minimal number of parameter constraints that is required to identify the model (Mehta et al., 2004; Millsap and Yun-Tein, 2004). We then fit cross-lagged models to examine the directional relationship between the 2 behaviors.

The cross-lagged model that is used in our study is illustrated in a path diagram in Fig. 1 for 2 time points. Factors for alcohol use (ALC) and antisocial behavior (ASB) were measured by multiple items at each occasion. The cross-lagged model estimates cross-lagged paths, from 1 factor measured earlier to another factor measured later, as well as autoregressive paths, from a factor measured earlier to the same factor measured later. Cross-lagged paths then represent the effects of a preceding factor to another subsequent factor after accounting for the autoregressive effect on itself. In cross-lagged analyses, it is desirable to have equivalently measured factors across time for more meaningful interpretation of path coefficients. This requirement has been commonly met using the same items over time to measure latent variables and setting their measurement parameters equal across time (White et al., 1993; Young et al., 2008). However, this strategy is not applicable to the ALSPAC data because the measures of alcohol use and antisocial behavior varied over time. Accordingly, we used the item response theory approach to calibrate alcohol use and antisocial behavior factors to the equivalent scales across measurement occasions (McArdle et al., 2009). Setting measurement parameters of common items equal across time calibrates the measures of latent variables by different sets of indicators across time to a commensurate scale.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The cross-lagged model of alcohol use and antisocial behavior with ages 12 and 13.

We examined the equivalency of the relationship between alcohol use and antisocial behavior between sexes using multiple-group analyses. Examining the equivalency of the directional relationship between the 2 behaviors across sex involves comparing 2 sets of parameters: path coefficients (cross-lagged and autoregressive paths) and the measurement parameters (factor loadings and thresholds for categorical indicators) of ALC and ASB factors. Path coefficients, especially of the cross-lagged paths across time points, estimate the strength of the longitudinal relationship between the 2 behaviors. Even when the path coefficients are equivalent across sexes, the relationship between the 2 behaviors may not be comparable if the measurements of alcohol use and antisocial behavior are not equivalent across sexes. Thus, when we examined the equivalency of the path coefficients, we also examined the equivalency of the measurement parameters of ALC and ASB factors. We fit multiple-group cross-lagged models with male and female participants as subgroups. Models with and without equality constraints on path coefficients and measurement parameters were compared and their differences of chi-squared statistics were tested. Significant differences in chi-squared statistics between models with and without across-group equality constraints indicate that the parameters that are constrained equal across groups may not be appropriate. Parameters were estimated by weighted least square estimator using Mplus version 7 (Muthén and Muthén, 2012).

Attrition of participants is inevitable in longitudinal data collection. If participants who missed follow-up assessments are systematically different in the measured factors from the participants with full data, the assumptions that Mplus’ weighted least square estimator relies on are violated (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2010). To check the appropriateness of this assumption, we tested whether the factor models of alcohol use and antisocial behavior at each point are equivalent across individuals who did and did not miss subsequent measurements by conducting multiple-group CFA with individuals with or without the subsequent measurements as subgroups at each occasion.

RESULTS

EFA of the alcohol use and antisocial behavior items at each measurement occasion indicated that a single factor could reasonably be assumed for each set of alcohol use and antisocial behavior items at each time point. For alcohol use items, the ranges of fit indices of single factor models were [0.992, 0.996] for comparative fit index (CFI), [0.988, 0.995] for Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and [0.036, 0.071] for root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). For antisocial behavior items, the ranges of fit indices were [0.942, 0.992] for CFI, [0.935, 0.991] for TLI, and [0.016, 0.054] for RMSEA. In addition, from multiple-group CFAs at each measurement occasion, no significant differences were observed in the parameters of factor models between the subgroups of individuals with and without subsequent assessments (p-values of chi-squared difference tests were between 0.1281 and 0.6104), indicating that this assumption for the weighted least square estimator was not violated. Table 5 summarizes means and covariance matrices of the ASB and ALC factors from different ages estimated from a CFA with unstructured means and covariance matrix. Means of ASB factors increased from age 12 to age 15 and declined between ages 15 and 17. Males had higher levels of antisocial behavior than females at all ages. Means of ALC factors increased between ages 12 and 17 in both females and males without noticeable sex differences. The variances of both ASB and ALC factors decreased throughout the included age range.

Table 5.

Factor Means and Correlation Matrix by Sex

Correlations ASB12a ASB13 ASB15 ASB17 ALC12 ALC13 ALC15 ALC17
Male
  ASB12
  ASB13 0.695
  ASB15 0.670 0.758
  ASB17 0.546 0.596 0.732
  ALC12 0.615 0.466 0.412 0.330
  ALC13 0.468 0.476 0.425 0.374 0.662
  ALC15 0.451 0.499 0.633 0.572 0.464 0.613
  ALC17 0.305 0.375 0.448 0.715 0.277 0.371 0.596
Meanb 0.000 0.272 0.769 0.181 0.000 0.473 1.577 2.264
Variancec 1.000 0.896 0.842 0.707 1.000 0.915 0.615 0.544
Female
  ASB12
  ASB13 0.703
  ASB15 0.574 0.720
  ASB17 0.529 0.606 0.721
  ALC12 0.711 0.432 0.356 0.318
  ALC13 0.537 0.511 0.362 0.321 0.718
  ALC15 0.480 0.499 0.620 0.486 0.535 0.618
  ALC17 0.286 0.339 0.438 0.704 0.319 0.361 0.579
Mean −0.443 0.185 0.527 −0.220 0.047 0.511 1.690 2.254
Variance 1.041 0.835 0.750 0.676 0.978 0.921 0.649 0.595
a

ASB and ALC are factors for antisocial behavior and alcohol use, respectively.

b

Factor mean with age 12 in males set as the reference with the mean fixed at 0.

c

Factor variances with age 12 in males set as the reference, with the variance fixed at 1.

The difference of chi-squared statistics between the measurement invariant model, in which measurement parameters (factor loadings and thresholds) are constrained equal between males and females, and the measurement noninvariant model was significant (Δχ2 = 305.425, Δdf = 124, p < 0.001). This can indicate that measurement parameters between the subgroups are not equivalent, and alcohol use and antisocial behavior were differentially measured in male and female participants. However, the chi-squared difference test is prone to type I error especially when sample size is this large. In addition, fit indices indicated that the measurement invariant model fit well to the data (CFI = 0.960, TLI = 0.961, RMSEA [90% CI] = 0.018 [0.017, 0.018]). In fact, the measurement invariant model fit slightly better than the measurement noninvariant model in terms of fit indices (CFI = 0.957, TLI = 0.957, RMSEA [90% CI] = 0.019 [0.018, 0.019]). Thus, in the following analyses, measurement parameters were constrained to be equal across the sexes to ensure equivalent measurement of alcohol use and antisocial behavior factors in male and female subgroups.

Tables 6 and 7 summarize the standardized path coefficients and correlations from cross-lagged models fit to the ALSPAC data. In Model 1 (Table 6), path coefficients were constrained equal across male and female subgroups. Autoregressive paths indicated that both ASB and ALC factors were highly stable over time. All autoregressive paths were highly significant, and preceding ALC or ASB factors were the strongest predictors of its subsequent factors. Cross-lagged paths showed different patterns of the relationship at different ages. Neither the ASB-to-ALC nor the ALC-to-ASB path was supported between ages 12 and 13. From ages 13 to 17, ASB-to-ALC paths were supported (0.274, CI = [0.221, 0.327], p < 0.001 between ages 13 and 15, and 0.124, CI = [0.067, 0.181], p < 0.001 between ages 15 and 17). Between ages 15 and 17, the ALC-to-ASB path was also supported (0.116, CI = [0.043, 0.189], p = 0.002). Residuals were strongly correlated at all occasions and showed an increasing trend through ages 13 to 17 (0.239 at age 13, 0.455 at age 15, and 0.648 at age 18). Strong residual correlation indicated that alcohol use and antisocial behavior were still associated with each other after accounting for preceding alcohol use and antisocial behavior.

Table 6.

Parameter Estimates from Model 1a

Estimate 95%CI p
Autoregressive
  ASB12→ASB13b 0.787 0.681, 0.893 <0.001
  ASB13→ASB15 0.751 0.688, 0.814 <0.001
  ASB15→ASB17 0.680 0.609, 0.751 <0.001
  ALC12→ALC13b 0.673 0.595, 0.751 <0.001
  ALC13→ALC15 0.496 0.453, 0.539 <0.001
  ALC15→ALC17 0.508 0.461, 0.555 <0.001
Cross-lagged
  ALC12→ASB13 −0.059 −0.177, 0.059 0.319
  ALC13→ASB15 0.049 −0.018, 0.116 0.155
  ALC15→ASB17 0.116 0.043, 0.189 0.002
  ASB12→ALC13 0.063 −0.033, 0.159 0.204
  ASB13→ALC15 0.274 0.221, 0.327 <0.001
  ASB15→ALC17 0.124 0.067, 0.181 <0.001
Correlationc
  ASB12-ALC12 0.673 0.622, 0.724 <0.001
Residual correlationc
  ASB13-ALC13 0.239 0.116, 0.362 <0.001
  ASB15-ALC15 0.455 0.384, 0.526 <0.001
  ASB18-ALC17 0.648 0.579, 0.717 <0.001
Fit indices χ2 = 11.421, df = 5,425, p < 0.001
CFI = 0.966, TLI = 0.967
RMSEA [90%CI] = 0.016 [0.016, 0.017]

CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; TLI, Tucker-Lewis index.

a

In Model 1, all parameters were constrained to be equal between the male and female subgroups.

b

ASB and ALC are factors for antisocial behavior and alcohol use, respectively.

c

Correlations were estimated at the initial age (age 12), and residual correlations were estimated for the rest of the ages because ALC and ASB factors at ages 13 to 17 were endogenous factors (i.e., dependent latent variables).

Table 7.

Parameter Estimates from Model 2a

Male Female


Coefficients Estimate 95%CI p Estimate 95%CI p
Autoregressive
  ASB12→ASB13b 0.789 0.658, 0.920 <0.001 0.782 0.664, 0.900 <0.001
  ASB13→ASB15 0.746 0.673, 0.819 <0.001 0.757 0.686, 0.828 <0.001
  ASB15→ASB17 0.645 0.559, 0.731 <0.001 0.725 0.633, 0.817 <0.001
  ALC12→ALC13b 0.670 0.564, 0.776 <0.001 0.673 0.573, 0.773 <0.001
  ALC13→ALC15 0.497 0.436, 0.558 <0.001 0.498 0.443, 0.553 <0.001
  ALC15→ALC17 0.507 0.436, 0.578 <0.001 0.507 0.448, 0.566 <0.001
Cross-lagged
  ALC12→ASB13 −0.062 −0.229, 0.105 0.468 −0.057 −0.202, 0.088 0.446
  ALC13→ASB15 0.079 −0.017, 0.175 0.102 0.019 −0.071, 0.109 0.676
  ALC15→ASB17 0.156 0.062, 0.250 0.001 0.057 0.051, 0.165 0.300
  ASB12→ALC13 0.047 −0.090, 0.184 0.503 0.078 −0.051, 0.207 0.237
  ASB13→ALC15 0.276 0.202, 0.350 <0.001 0.269 0.202, 0.336 <0.001
  ASB15→ALC17 0.120 0.036, 0.204 0.005 0.131 0.055, 0.207 0.001
Correlationc
  ASB12-ALC12 0.676 0.625, 0.727 <0.001
Residual correlationc
  ASB13-ALC13 0.238 0.115, 0.361 <0.001
  ASB15-ALC15 0.459 0.390, 0.528 <0.001
  ASB18-ALC17 0.649 0.580, 0.718 <0.001
Fit indices χ2 = 12,557.796, df = 5,413, p < 0.001
CFI = 0.960, TLI = 0.961
RMSEA [90%CI] = 0.017 [0.017, 0.018]

CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; TLI, Tucker-Lewis index.

a

In Model 2, autoregressive and cross-lagged paths were allowed to vary between the male and female subgroups, so separate autoregressive and cross-lagged paths by sex are presented.

b

ASB and ALC are factors for antisocial behavior and alcohol use, respectively.

c

Correlations were constrained equal across male and female subgroups in Model 2. Correlations were estimated at the initial age (age 12), and residual correlations were estimated at the rest of ages because ALC and ASB factors at ages 13 to 17 were endogeneous factors (i.e., dependent latent variables).

In Model 2 (Table 7), path coefficients were allowed to vary across male and female subgroups, while other parameters, including measurement parameters and cross-sectional residual correlations, were constrained to be equal across the sexes. The patterns of path coefficients from male and female subgroups were mostly similar to those from Model 1. However, a notable difference between male and female subgroups was observed for the ALC-to-ASB path between ages 15 and 17 (underlined). This path was significant in Model 1 and in the male subgroup of Model 2, but was not significant in the female subgroup. However, this difference did not result in a significant chi-squared difference between Model 1 and Model 2 (Δχ2 = 11.146, Δdf = 12, p = 0.5165).

DISCUSSION

We examined the directional relationships between alcohol use and antisocial behavior by applying a cross-lagged model to longitudinal data from ALSPAC. This study is, to our knowledge, currently the largest cross-lagged analysis of the directional relationships between alcohol use and antisocial behavior. The large sample size, along with measures covering various domains of the 2 behaviors, also provided a rare opportunity to test potential sex differences in the relationships between the behaviors. The results indicated that the relationship between alcohol use and antisocial behavior might change across adolescence. During early adolescence (between ages 12 and 13), neither alcohol use nor antisocial behavior predicted subsequent measures of one another. Early antisocial behavior was predictive of subsequent alcohol use during mid-to late adolescence (from ages 13 to 17). However, alcohol use was only predictive of antisocial behavior during late adolescence (between ages 15 and 17), and this relationship appeared to be mainly driven by males. The results of the model comparisons indicated that the relationships between alcohol use and antisocial behavior were largely equivalent across the sexes, although a possible sex difference was suggested in late adolescence.

Our observation that antisocial behavior predicted alcohol use (but not vice versa) during mid-to late adolescence is in line with previous results of cross-lagged studies (Silberg et al., 2003; White et al., 1993; Young et al. 2008) and is consistent with the observations that antisocial behavior precedes and predicts alcohol use (Englund et al., 2008; Kuperman et al., 2001; Merline et al., 2008; Windle, 1990). The result may support the “susceptibility” hypothesis, which posits that predispositions toward antisocial activities lead a person to choose situations that encourage heavy drinking (Bushman and Cooper, 1990; Graham et al., 1998; White et al., 1993). Although we did not find robust evidence for an association of alcohol use with subsequent antisocial behavior, we note that previous findings on the disinhibitory effects of alcohol on antisocial behavior and aggression largely come from experimental settings that focused on the immediate effect of alcohol on one’s behavior (Bushman and Cooper, 1990; Cherek et al., 1985; Miczek et al., 2004). Cross-lagged analyses, including our study, have focused on relatively longer term effects, mostly over a year (White et al., 1993; Young et al., 2008). Studies that reported longer term effects of alcohol use on antisocial behavior (Brook et al., 1998; Ellickson et al., 2003; Maldonado-Molina et al., 2011; Resnick et al., 2004) have examined antisocial behavior measured in early adulthood as a consequence of earlier alcohol use.

We did not find a significant effect of alcohol use on antisocial behavior in early- to mid-adolescence. This may suggest that adolescent alcohol use from ages 12 to 15 is not yet associated with the spectrum of negative outcomes (including antisocial behavior) that are evidenced later in adolescence. Between ages 15 and 17, we found significant effects from alcohol use to antisocial behavior as well as from antisocial behavior to alcohol use. Several factors may contribute to the emergence of the effects from alcohol use to antisocial behavior during later adolescence. First, alcohol use might reach the level that can result in long-term negative consequences during late adolescence. As seen in Table 5, the level of alcohol use steadily increased from ages 12 to 17. Alcohol use during early- to mid-adolescence (from ages 12 to 15) might not have reached the level that could cause negative effects. However, during late adolescence (from ages 15 to 17), an elevated level of alcohol use might be sufficient to cause long-term negative consequences by damaging one’s central nervous system (Howard, 2006) or by impairing social functioning (Ellickson et al., 2003). Second, increased alcohol use might prevent the desistance of antisocial behavior during late adolescence. The period between ages 16 and 20 is a rich developmental period that is characterized by increased responsibility and tasks to accomplish that are important for a successful transition to adulthood (Brown et al., 2008). These changes are not compatible with antisocial lifestyles, and the normative trajectories of antisocial behavior during late adolescence generally follow a decreasing trend (Moffitt, 1993). However, elevated alcohol use during this period may keep an adolescent from desisting from antisocial behavior by interfering with his/her ability to manage developmental demands (Hussong et al., 2004).

The directional relationships suggested in this study do not completely rule out other types of relationships between the 2 behaviors. First, a cross-lagged model based on retrospective measures may not be able to capture a short-term disinhibitory effect of alcohol on antisocial behavior. Evidence from experimental studies indicates that alcohol can increase aggression and violence (Bushman and Cooper, 1990; Cherek et al., 1985; Miczek et al., 2004). Highly significant residual correlations between alcohol use and antisocial behavior may also indicate such short-term or cross-sectional relationships between the 2 behaviors, but the directional relationship between residuals could not be included in the model without fixing other parameters to a constant.

Second, alcohol use and antisocial behavior may share common causes. As discussed previously, antisocial behavior and alcohol use are part of a broader externalizing behavior factor (Krueger et al., 1998, 2002). It is also known that common genetic influences contribute to a spectrum of correlated externalizing behaviors (Dick et al., 2005, 2008, 2009; Kendler et al., 2003; Krueger et al., 2002; Slutske et al., 1998), and that genetic associations with these behaviors may change across development. For example, variants in the GABRA2 gene are associated with conduct disorder during childhood, and then alcohol problems beginning in late adolescence through adulthood (Dick et al., 2006, 2013). This suggests that genetic predispositions may manifest differently at different ages, which may be due to environmental changes such as greater autonomy and greater opportunities to express those predispositions. This may explain the sequential expression of antisocial behavior and alcohol use and could provide an alternative explanation for the directional relationships between the 2 behaviors found here. As the field continues to identify genes associated with alcohol problems and antisocial behavior, future models that incorporate genotypic information that is available in the ALSPAC sample could be used to estimate the directional relationships between the 2 behaviors over and above their shared genetic liability.

Although most items used in this study were adopted from previously established systems, such as DSM-IV and ESYTC, items used in our study may not perfectly map onto any of these systems due to item exclusions and availabilities at different measurement occasions. In addition, the results of our study should be interpreted in terms of the relationships between the latent factors measured by alcohol use and antisocial behavior items, not the relationships between the diagnoses of alcohol dependence and conduct disorder. We used available items as indicators of continuous latent variables in our cross-lagged models. Thus, the diagnostic criteria for DSM-IV alcohol dependence do not apply to the sets of items used in our study. ESYTC, from which most antisocial behavior items were adopted, was not developed for diagnostic purposes.

Differences in the measurements of alcohol use and antisocial behavior items across time may have affected the relationship between the 2 behaviors observed in this study. We aligned the scales of factors measured by different items across time by constraining the measurement parameters to be equal for items that overlapped across time. This method permits the estimation of factors equivalent across time when measurement invariance holds. Measurement invariance across time can be tested by comparing models with or without equality constraints on measurement parameters if there are enough overlapping items to identify the measurement model (Millsap and Yun-Tein, 2004). Unfortunately, our data did not allow the test of measurement invariance across time due to insufficient number of overlapping items, especially at early ages. Thus, we cannot completely rule out the possibility that the relationship between alcohol use and antisocial behavior was affected by noninvariant measurement of the factors across time.

The characteristics of the sample included in our analyses may limit the generalization of the directional relationship between alcohol use and antisocial behavior suggested in this study. Although the ALSPAC sample was designed to be representative of epidemiological characteristics of the area of South West England (Bristol area), residents in this area were more likely to be White and to have higher socioeconomic status compared to the overall British population. Furthermore, ALSPAC participants were more likely to have higher socioeconomic status compared to the population of the recruitment area (Fraser et al., 2012). In addition, the socioeconomic characteristics of the sample included in this study were still not completely equivalent with that of the overall ALSPAC sample (Table 2). Adolescents included in this study tended to be White, female, and to have parents who had higher educational achievement and higher status professional occupations compared to the overall ALSPAC sample. This may limit generalizability of results from our study to other populations given the evidence that socioeconomic status is related to both alcohol use and antisocial behaviors in the ALSPAC sample (Heron et al. 2013). We might expect that the more constrained variability introduced by the attrition of more disadvantaged subjects could have weakened the associations that potentially exist at the population level.

In conclusion, the results from the current study emphasize the importance of considering developmental stages to examine the directional relationship between alcohol use and antisocial behavior, as we observed different patterns of relationships between the 2 behaviors at different ages. Although the overall pattern of associations between alcohol use and antisocial behavior was mostly equivalent between males and females, the significant effect from alcohol use to antisocial behavior during late adolescence was primarily driven by males. This might indicate that the patterns of the directional relationship between antisocial behavior and alcohol use shift between adolescence and adulthood and begin to evidence sex differences. ALSPAC’s ongoing data collection will allow us to investigate how the relationships between the 2 behaviors changes during the transition to adulthood and whether sex difference observed in our study persists beyond adolescence.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Data

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust (grant ref: 092731) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and DMD and KSK will serve as guarantors for the contents of this study. This study was specifically funded by R01AA018333 (DMD and KSK), K02AA018755 (DMD), and F32AA22269 (JES).

Footnotes

SUPPORTING INFORMATION

Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article:

Table S1. Item parameters of alcohol use items from Model 2.

Table S2. Item parameters of antisocial behavior items from Model 2.

REFERENCES

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Armstrong TD, Costello EJ. Community studies on adolescent substance use, abuse, or dependence and psychiatric comorbidity. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70:1224–1239. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.6.1224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Weighted least squares estimation with missing data. Mplus Technical Appendix. 2010 [Google Scholar]
  4. Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Smith GD. Cohort profile: the “children of the 90s”—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:111–127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brook JS, Cohen P, Brook DW. Longitudinal study of co-occurring psychiatric disorders and substance use. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37:322–330. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199803000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown SA, McGue M, Maggs J, Schulenberg J, Hingson R, Swartzwelder S, Martin C, Chung T, Tapert SF, Sher K. A developmental perspective on alcohol and youths 16 to 20 years of age. Pediatrics. 2008;121:S290–S310. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243D. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI, Reich T, Schmidt I, Schuckit MA. A new, semistructured psychiatric interview for use in genetic-linkage studies – a report on the reliability of the ssaga. J Stud Alcohol. 1994;55:149–158. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bushman BJ, Cooper HM. Effects of alcohol on human aggression: an intergrative research review. Psychol Bull. 1990;107:341–354. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.3.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cherek D, Steinberg J, Manno B. Effects of alcohol on human aggressive behavior. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 1985;46:321. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1985.46.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dawson DA, Goldstein RB, Moss HB, Li TK, Grant BF. Gender differences in the relationship of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology to alcohol dependence: likelihood, expression and course. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2010;112:9–17. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dick DM, Aliev F, Wang JC, Grucza RA, Schuckit M, Kuperman S, Kramer J, Hinrichs A, Bertelsen S, Budde JP. Using dimensional models of externalizing psychopathology to aid in gene identification. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:310–318. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.3.310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dick DM, Bierut L, Hinrichs A, Fox L, Bucholz KK, Kramer J, Kuperman S, Hesselbrock V, Schuckit M, Almasy L. The role of GABRA2 in risk for conduct disorder and alcohol and drug dependence across developmental stages. Behav Genet. 2006;36:577–590. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-9041-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dick DM, Cho SB, Latendresse SJ, Aliev F, Nurnberger JI, Jr, Edenberg HJ, Schuckit M, Hesselbrock VM, Porjesz B, Bucholz KK, Wang J, Goate A, Kramer J, Kuperman S. Genetic influences on alcohol use across stages of development: GABRA2 and longitudinal trajectories of drunkenness from adolescence to young adulthood. Addict Biol. 2013 doi: 10.1111/adb.12066. Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dick DM, Latendresse SJ, Lansford JE, Budde JP, Goate A, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Role of GABRA2 in trajectories of externalizing behavior across development and evidence of moderation by parental monitoring. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:649–657. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dick DM, Viken RJ, Kaprio J, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ. Understanding the covariation among childhood externalizing symptoms: genetic and environmental influences on conduct disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder symptoms. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2005;33:219–229. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-1829-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ellickson PL, Tucker JS, Klein DJ. Ten-year prospective study of public health problems associated with early drinking. Pediatrics. 2003;111:949–955. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.5.949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Englund MM, Egeland B, Oliva EM, Collins WA. Childhood and adolescent predictors of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in early adulthood: a longitudinal developmental analysis. Addiction. 2008;103(Suppl. 1):23–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02174.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Epstein EE, Labouvie E, McCrady BS, Jensen NK, Hayaki J. A multisite study of alcohol subtypes: classification and overlap of unidimensional and multi-dimensional typologies. Addiction. 2002;97:1041–1053. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00164.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J, Smith GD, Henderson J, Macleod J, Molloy L, Ness A. Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;42:97–110. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Goldstein RB, Dawson DA, Chou SP, Grant BF. Sex differences in prevalence and comorbidity of alcohol and drug use disorders: results from wave 2 of the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73:938–950. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Graham K, Leonard KE, Room R, Wild TC, Pihl RO, Bois C, Single E. Current directions in research on understanding and preventing intoxicated aggression. Addiction. 1998;93:659–676. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.9356593.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Heron J, Maughan B, Dick DM, Kendler KS, Lewis G, Macleod J, Munafò M, Hickman M. Conduct problem trajectories and alcohol use and misuse in mid to late adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;133:100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hesselbrock M, Easton C, Bucholz KK, Schuckit M, Hesselbrock V. A validity study of the SSAGA - a comparison with the SCAN. Addiction. 1999;94:1361–1370. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.94913618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Howard R. How is personality disorder linked to dangerousness? A putative role for early-onset alcohol abuse. Med Hypotheses. 2006;67:702–708. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2006.03.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hussong AM, Curran PJ, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Carrig MM. Substance abuse hinders desistance in young adults’ antisocial behavior. Dev Psychopathol. 2004;16:1029–1046. doi: 10.1017/s095457940404012x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Institute for Social Research. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan; 2012. Monitoring the future national results on adolescent drug use: overview of key findings, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:929–937. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Krueger RF. The structure of common mental disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:921–926. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Krueger RF, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. The structure and stability of common mental disorders (DSM-III-R): a longitudinal-epidemiological study. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:216–227. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.2.216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Krueger RF, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ, Carlson SR, Iacono WG, McGue M. Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior and personality: modeling the externalizing spectrum. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:411–424. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kuperman S, Schlosser SS, Kramer JR, Bucholz K, Hesselbrock V, Reich T, Reich W. Developmental sequence from disruptive behavior diagnosis to adolescent alcohol dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:2022–2026. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.12.2022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Maldonado-Molina MM, Reingle JM, Jennings WG. Does alcohol use predict violent behaviors? The relationship between alcohol use and violence in a nationally representative longitudinal sample. Youth Violence Juv Justice. 2011;9:99–111. doi: 10.1177/1541204010384492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. McArdle JJ, Grimm KJ, Hamagami F, Bowles RP, Meredith W. Modeling life-span growth curves of cognition using longitudinal data with multiple samples and changing scales of measurement. Psychol Methods. 2009;14:126. doi: 10.1037/a0015857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mehta PD, Neale MC, Flay BR. Squeezing interval change from ordinal panel data: latent growth curves with ordinal outcomes. Psychol Methods. 2004;9:301–333. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.3.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Merline A, Jager J, Schulenberg JE. Adolescent risk factors for adult alcohol use and abuse: stability and change of predictive value across early and middle adulthood. Addiction. 2008;103:84–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02178.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Miczek KA, Fish EW, Almeida RM, Faccidomo S, Debold JF. Role of alcohol consumption in escalation to violence. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1036:278–289. doi: 10.1196/annals.1330.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Millsap RE, Yun-Tein J. Assessing factorial invariance in ordered-categorical measures. Multivar Behav Res. 2004;39:479–515. [Google Scholar]
  38. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev. 1993;100:674–701. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M, Silva PA. Sex and comorbidity: are there sex differences in the co-occurrence of conduct disorder and other disorders? In: Moffitt TE, editor. Sex Differences in Antisocial Behaviour: Conduct Disorder, Delinquency, and Violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2001. pp. 135–150. [Google Scholar]
  40. Molina BSG, Bukstein OG, Lynch KG. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and conduct disorder symptomatology in adolescents with alcohol use disorder. Psychol Addict Behav. 2002;16:161–164. doi: 10.1037//0893-164x.16.2.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Moss HB, Chen CM, Yi H. Subtypes of alcohol dependence in a nationally representative sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;91:149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Moss HB, Lynch KG. Comorbid disruptive behavior disorder symptoms and their relationship to adolescent alcohol use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2001;64:75–83. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(00)00233-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Muthén L. Mplus User’s Guide. 7th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  44. Pombo S, Lesch OM. The alcoholic phenotypes among different multidimensional typologies: similarities and their classification procedures. Alcohol Alcohol. 2009;44:46–54. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Resnick MD, Ireland M, Borowsky I. Youth violence perpetration: what protects? What predicts? Findings from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. J Adolesc Health. 2004;35:424e1–424e10. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sanford M. The relationship between antisocial behaviour and substance abuse in childhood and adolescence: Implications for aetiology, prevention and treatment. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2001;14:317–323. [Google Scholar]
  47. Silberg J, Rutter M, D’Onofrio B, Eaves L. Genetic and environmental risk factors in adolescent substance use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:664–676. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:363. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.3.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Smith DJ, McVie S. Theory and method in the Edinburgh study of youth transitions and crime. Br J Criminol. 2003;43:169–195. [Google Scholar]
  50. Watson D. Rethinking the mood and anxiety disorders: a quantitative hierarchical model for DSM-V. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:522–536. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. White HR, Brick J, Hansell S. A longitudinal investigation of alcohol use and aggression in adolescence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs Suppl. 1993;11:62–77. doi: 10.15288/jsas.1993.s11.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. White HR, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Farrington DP. Developmental associations between substance use and violence. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11:785–803. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Windle M. A longitudinal study of antisocial behaviors in early adolescence as predictors of late adolescent substance use: gender and ethnic group differences. J Abnorm Psychol. 1990;99:86–91. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.99.1.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Young R, Sweeting H, West P. A longitudinal study of alcohol use and antisocial behaviour in young people. Alcohol Alcohol. 2008;43:204–214. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agm147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Young S, Corley R, Stallings M, Rhee S, Crowley T, Hewitt J. Substance use, abuse and dependence in adolescence: prevalence, symptom profiles and correlates. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;68:309–322. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00225-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data

RESOURCES