Abstract
Misery is not miserly: sadness increases the amount of money decision makers give up to acquire a commodity (Lerner, Small, & Loewenstein, 2004). The present research investigated when and why the “misery-is-not-miserly” effect occurs. Drawing on William James’s (1890) concept of the material self, we tested a model specifying relationships among sadness, self-focus, and the amount of money decision makers spend. Consistent with our Jamesian hypothesis, results revealed that self-focus both moderates and mediates the effect of sadness on spending. Results were consistent across males and females. Because the study used real commodities and real money, results hold implications for everyday decisions. They also hold implications for theoretical development. Economic theories of spending may benefit from incorporating psychological theories – specifically theories of emotion and the self.
Keywords: emotion, sadness, self, self-focus, decision making, choice, behavioral economics
A man's Self is the sum total of all that he CAN call his, not only his body and his psychic powers, but his clothes and his house … his lands and horses, and yacht and bank-account. All these things give him the same emotions. (James, 1890, p. 291)
Since William James’s classic work (1890), the self has been regarded as one of the most important concepts in psychology. A survey of psychology articles from 1974–1993 found over 31,000 articles that addressed the self (Ashmore & Jussim, 1997). Baumeister’s authoritative review on the self for the Handbook of Social Psychology reported that “trying to keep abreast of the research on the self is like trying to get a drink from a fire hose.” (1998, p. 681)
Despite the central role that theories of the self play in psychology, the rapidly emerging field of behavioral economics (i.e., the application of psychological insights to economics) has had relatively few contributions drawing on theories of the self (exceptions include Babcock & Loewenstein, 1997; Beggan 1992; Diekman, Samuels, Ross, & Bazerman, 1997; and Larrick, 1993). The lack of empirical work connecting the self to economic choices exists even though William James (1890) posited over a century ago that material goods play a crucial role in defining the self. Indeed, James held that losing material possessions has an influence so strong that it results in “a sense of shrinkage of our personality, a partial conversion of ourselves to nothingness.” (James, 1890, p. 293)
James also hypothesized a close connection between the self (broadly construed to include possessions) and emotion (James, 1890). He did not, however, connect the self with specific emotions and specific material decisions, nor did he have the opportunity to present empirical support for his views. In this paper, we examine potential empirical connections among emotion, the self, and material decisions in the context of the “miserly-is-not-miserly” effect.
The “Misery is not Miserly” Effect
The misery-is-not-miserly effect is the tendency for sadness to carry over from past situations to influence normatively unrelated economic decisions, increasing the amount of money that decision makers give up to receive a commodity. In one study, for example, decision makers who received a sadness induction, compared to a neutral induction, subsequently gave up 30% more money to acquire a commodity (Lerner, Small, & Loewenstein, 2004). It is important to emphasize that the misery-is-not-miserly effect, like other emotion carryover effects, involves incidental emotion, i.e., emotion that should be irrelevant to the decision at hand (see Loewenstein & Lerner, 2003).
Perhaps the most curious thing about the misery-is-not-miserly effect is that it runs counter to predictions from valence-based and mood-congruent theories of decision making. According to these theories, negative moods lead people to globally devalue what they perceive. Such devaluation could appear in contexts such as stock market decisions (Hirschleifer & Shumway, 2003), or life satisfaction judgments (Schwarz & Clore, 1983; for a review, see Loewenstein & Lerner, 2003). Empirically, however, the effects of sadness on buying run counter to the predicted pattern. Even though sadness is negatively valenced, it leads people to increase, rather than decrease, valuation of commodities that they might purchase (Lerner et al., 2004).
Misery-is-not-Miserly and Theories of the Self
Why might sadness elicit behaviors that are inconsistent with valence-based predictions? One possibility is that sadness, in contrast to some other negative emotions, has an intimate connection with the self. Previous work demonstrates that sadness inductions can trigger increased self-focus (Salovey, 1992; Wood, Saltzberg, & Goldsamt, 1990) and can do so even without influencing the conscious experience of sadness (Silvia, Phillips, Baumgaertner, & Maschauer, 2006).
The present study investigated the hypothesis that the experience of feeling sad and self-focused leads individuals to pay more for commodities. As shown in Figure 1, our model suggests that a sad event, coupled with self-focus, triggers an implicit devaluation or diminished sense of self (for a possible analogue in depression, see Blumberg & Hokanson, 1983). Self-devaluation, in turn, triggers an implicit desire to enhance the self. Finally, the desire to enhance the self elicits increased valuation of possessions that one might acquire. In sum, we predict that when self-focus is high, sad individuals experience an implicit devaluation of the self, which in turn triggers increased valuation of new commodities.
The Present Study
The primary goal of the present study was to test two main implications of our model. First, the study examined if the misery-is-not-miserly effect depends on one’s level of self-focus (i.e., does self-focus moderate the misery-is-not-miserly effect?) Second, the study tested if self-focus explains the process of the misery-is-not-miserly effect (i.e., does self-focus mediate the misery-is-not-miserly effect?)
A secondary goal was to examine the generalizability of the misery-is-not-miserly effect. Whereas previous research investigated the misery-is-not-miserly effect by observing the amount of cash individuals would forgo to receive a commodity (i.e., their “choice prices”; Lerner, et al., 2004), the current research investigated the effect by observing a more common choice, the amount that participants give up from their own money to receive a commodity (i.e., their “buying prices”; see Kahneman, Knetsch, & Thaler, 1991).
Method
Participants
Thirty-three participants (13 females, 20 males) responded to an advertisement offering $10 for participation. Participants’ ages ranged from 18–30 years (Mage=21.2).
Procedure
Participants received their participation payment upon arrival. They sat in individual cubicles, each equipped with a computer and headphones. For screening purposes, participants first completed an English fluency assessment and baseline emotion measure. They then expected to complete three separate tasks, which they were told had been combined for convenience.
Emotion induction (task 1)
Participants were randomly assigned to a sadness or neutral condition. Sad-condition participants watched a previously validated (Gross & Levenson, 1995) video clip about the death of a boy’s mentor (from The Champ). Neutral-condition participants watched a previously validated video clip (Lerner, et al., 2004) about the Great Barrier Reef (from a National Geographic special). Each clip lasted less than 4 minutes.
Self-focused essay (task 2)
All participants next completed a corresponding essay focused on the self, consistent with previously established emotion induction procedures (see Lerner et al., 2004). Sad-condition participants wrote about how a situation like the one portrayed in the video clip would affect them personally. Neutral-condition participants wrote about their daily activities.
To estimate each individual’s self-focus, two independent coders, blind to hypotheses, counted the frequency that participants wrote the following self-references: I, me, my, and myself (see also Campbell & Pennebaker, 2003). Inter-coder consistency was nearly perfect, r=0.98, p <0.001, prep=0.99.
Buying decisions (task 3)
Participants were shown a sporty, insulated water bottle and told that they could trade any amount up to the total of their experiment payment ($10) to buy the water bottle. Following standard price-elicitation procedures from experimental economics (see Becker, DeGroot, & Marschak, 1964), participants chose between buying the water bottle or not buying the water bottle at prices from $0 to $10 in 50-cent increments. As an incentive for revealing true values, participants were informed that one price would be randomly selected in each session to count for real1 (see Becker et al., 1964) and that if they chose to buy the water bottle at that price, they would pay that price from their experiment payment and receive the water bottle in return.
Manipulation check and debriefing
Immediately after task 2, participants reported how intensely they felt 19 emotions; 6 were of primary interest.2 A sadness composite included the average of “blue,” “sad,” and “depressed” (α=.88). A neutral composite included the average of “indifferent,” “neutral,” and “unemotional” (α=85).
Next, participants completed demand-awareness questions. To encourage truthful responses, participants were told that if they guessed the study’s hypotheses, they would receive a $5 gift certificate to Amazon.com.
Finally, participants learned the randomly selected water bottle price. Based on that price and participants’ indicated preferences, participants kept all participation earnings, or kept a portion of the earnings and exchanged the rest for the water bottle.
Results3
Manipulation Checks
Emotion inductions were effective in magnitude and specificity. Neutral-condition participants reported feeling significantly more neutral than sad, Ms=4.2 versus 1.4 respectively, t(13)=3.29, p<0.01, prep=0.96. Sad-condition participants reported feeling significantly more sad than neutral, Ms=4.6 versus 2.2 respectively, t(16)=5.42, p <0.001, prep=0.99. Sad-condition participants also reported feeling significantly more sad than any other measured negative emotion, including anger, M=2.6, p=0.001, prep=0.99, disgust, M=2.5, p=0.001, p rep=0.99, and fear, M=2.5, p=0.001, p rep=0.99.
Main Analyses
Consistent with prior research, sad-condition participants set higher buying prices than did neutral-condition participants, Ms=$2.11 and $0.56 respectively, t (29)=4.02, p=0.001, prep= 0.99, d=1.41. Thus, the effect of sadness observed using a “buying price” paradigm parallels the effect of sadness observed using the “choice price” paradigm (see Lerner et al., 2004). In fact, the effect of sadness on valuation in the current buying price paradigm was significantly larger (d=1.41) than in the original choice price paradigm, d=0.48; χ2(1)= 4.1, p<0.05.
To test the hypothesized moderating role of self-focus, we conducted a regression analysis predicting buying price with emotion condition, self-focus (centered), and their interaction term. Results revealed an Emotion Condition X Self-Focus interaction, β=1.30, t(27)=2.29, p=0.03, prep=0.91; see Figure 2. Consistent with hypotheses, tests of simple slopes (Aiken & West, 1991) revealed no association between being in the sadness condition and buying prices at low levels (sd=−1) of self-focus, β=0.22, t(27)=0.43, p=0.67, prep=0.38, but a positive association between being in the sadness condition and buying prices at high levels (sd=+1) of self-focus, β=2.19, t(27)=2.55, p<0.02, prep=0.93.
Although the interaction described above is consistent with our theoretical expectations, it is possible that the results arose from an artifact of the experimental conditions. To confirm that the interaction was due to the experience of actual sadness and not an artifact, we calculated a second interaction model, substituting self-reported sadness for emotion condition. Consistent with the idea that the actual experience of sadness drove the effect, results revealed a significant interaction between self-reported sadness and self-focus, β=0.37, t(30)=2.32, p<0.03, prep=0.91. Once again, tests of simple slopes revealed no association between sadness and buying price at low levels of self-focus, β=0.10, t(27)=0.40, p=0.69, prep=0.37, but a positive association between sadness and buying price at high levels of self-focus, β=0.94, t(27)=3.18, p<0.01, prep=0.97.
Next, we conducted analyses examining the mediational, or explanatory, role of self-focus (see Baron & Kenny, 1986). Results revealed that self-focus mediated the relationship between emotion condition and buying price, Sobel test =2.00, p<0.05, prep=0.88. A second analysis confirmed that this relationship held when self-reported sadness was substituted for emotion condition, Sobel test=2.05, p<0.05, prep=0.89. Parameter estimates for both mediational models appear in Figure 3.
As maintained by Judd, Kenny, & McClelland (2001; see also, Judd & Kenny, 1981), a variable can serve as both a moderator and mediator for a single relationship. We observe that self-focus plays both a moderating and mediating role for the relationship between sadness and buying price. However, because we do not observe an effect of sadness on buying price when self-focus is low, the mediation results should be interpreted as holding only for average (and above average) levels of self-focus (C. Judd, personal communication, August 13, 2007).
General Discussion
The current study tested models examining relationships among incidental emotion, the self, and spending decisions. Results demonstrated that the misery-is-not-miserly effect occurs only when self-focus is high. That is, self-focus moderates the effect of sadness on spending. Moreover, mediational analyses revealed that at sufficiently high levels of self-focus, self-focus mediates (explains) the relationship between sadness and spending. Finally, results showed that sadness and self-focus influence individuals’ actual buying prices, not just their choice prices (see Lerner et al., 2004).
By revealing that self-focus plays both a moderating and mediating role in the relationship between sadness and spending, the findings connect William James’s concept of the material self to contemporary theories of emotion and decision making. Whereas spending decisions have been addressed primarily by economic theories, the present results highlight a central role for psychological theories of emotion and the self.
Alternative Explanations
It may appear, contrary to our Jamesian account, that existing theories of mood-repair (e.g., Clark & Isen, 1982) provide an alternative explanation for the present results. According to mood-repair theories, individuals in a negative emotional state are predisposed to engage in potentially mood-improving behaviors such as helping others. These theories make no mention of self-focus, however, and therefore they do not predict the present pattern of data. Moreover, the misery-is-not-miserly effect still occurs even after participants receive an effective happiness induction (Garg & Lerner, 2006), revealing that the effect persists even after a mood-repairing event.
The present findings do, however, allow more than one explanation for the link among sadness, self-focus, and spending. Our working model (see Figure 1) proposes that sad and self-focused individuals spend more on commodities because they seek self-enhancement. Another possible model is that sad and self-focused individuals experience reduced self-value or sense of entitlement, and therefore value other things more by contrast. This contrasting-value model could be tested in future studies by examining if sad and self-focused people value objects more even when they cannot receive the objects (and therefore have no opportunity for self-enhancement). Results supporting either model would have important implications for models of sadness and choice.
Implications for Related Literatures
The present findings have theoretical value for multiple literatures. For example, for theories of emotion, the results demonstrate that valence-based models of affect and decision-making require updating. Valence-based models of emotion predict that negative emotions elicit global devaluation of what one perceives. The misery-is-not-miserly effect, however, shows a different pattern: when self-focus is sufficiently high, sadness increases valuation of commodities that one may purchase. The current findings imply that valence, although a powerful dimension of emotion, needs to be considered in the context of self processes for accurate predictions of behavior.
As another example, the results hold implications for models of clinical depression. Depression, like sadness, is associated with increased self-focus (Ingram, 1990; Watkins & Teasdale, 2004) and a diminished sense of self-worth (Gotlib & Hammen 1992; Watkins & Teasdale, 2001). Future studies could explicitly test the role of self-focus and self-devaluation in both sad and depressed individuals’ decisions. Results from these studies might clarify 1) the relationship between sadness and depression 2) common decision processes underlying sadness and depression and 3) the potential for new clinical interventions.
In sum, the present findings bring William James’s classic concept of the “material self” into modern theorizing at the intersection of psychology and behavioral economics. Combining methods from economics with theories from psychology, the results reveal the benefit of a multi-disciplinary approach to understanding emotional and cognitive influences on decision making.
Acknowledgments
Grants from the National Science Foundation (PECASE SES0239637; awarded to Jennifer S. Lerner) and from the National Institute of Health (MH58147; awarded to James J. Gross) supported this research. We thank Beth Gilden, Hetal Choxi, and Sarah Scholl for help conducting this research. We thank Dan Gilbert for creative title input and Chick Judd for statistical expertise. Additionally, we thank George Lowenstein and Deborah Small for conceptual contributions.
Footnotes
By using a randomly-generated price for the water bottle, instead of using the market price, we discouraged participants from reporting prices lower than market price in attempts to get a “deal” (see Becker et al., 1964).
Emotion manipulation checks occurred after the main dependent variable because labeling one’s feelings after incidental-emotion inductions can reduce the effect of such emotions (Keltner, Locke, & Audrain, 1993; Schwarz & Clore, 1983).
Data analyses excluded two individuals because they misunderstood experiment instructions and asked to re-do the choice task.
Contributor Information
Cynthia E. Cryder, Carnegie Mellon University
Jennifer S Lerner, Harvard University.
James J. Gross, Stanford University
Ronald E. Dahl, University of Pittsburgh
References
- Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Ashmore RD, Jussim L. Toward a second century of the scientific analysis of self and identity: Introduction. In: Ashmore RD, Jussim L, editors. Self and identity: Fundamental issues. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997. pp. 3–19. [Google Scholar]
- Babcock L, Loewenstein G. Explaining bargaining impasse: the role of self-serving biases. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 1997;11:109–126. [Google Scholar]
- Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beggan JK. On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;62:229–237. [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF. The self. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindsey G, editors. Handbook of social psychology. 4th ed. Vol. 1. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998. pp. 680–740. [Google Scholar]
- Becker GM, DeGroot MH, Marschak J. Measuring utility by a single-response sequential method. Behavioral Science. 1964;9:226–232. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830090304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg SR, Hokanson JE. The effects of another person’s response style on interpersonal behavior in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1983;192:196–209. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.92.2.196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell RS, Pennebaker JW. The secret life of pronouns: Flexibility in writing style and physical health. Psychological Science. 2003;14:60–65. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark MS, Isen AM. Towards understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. In: Hastorf AH, Isen AM, editors. Cognitive social psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland; 1982. pp. 73–108. [Google Scholar]
- Diekmann KA, Samuels SM, Ross L, Bazerman MH. Self-interest and fairness in problems of resource allocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72:1061–1074. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.5.1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garg N, Lerner J. Emotion effects on compensatory consumption. The University of Mississippi; 2006. Manuscript in preparation. [Google Scholar]
- Gotlib IH, Hammen CL. Psychological aspects of depression: Toward a cognitive-interpersonal integration. New York: Wiley; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion. 1995;9:87–108. [Google Scholar]
- Hirschleifer D, Shumway T. Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather. The Journal of Finance. 2003;58:1009–1032. [Google Scholar]
- Ingram RE. Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;109:156–176. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James W. Principles of psychology. New York: Holt; 1890. [Google Scholar]
- Judd CM, Kenny DA. Process analysis: Assessing mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review. 1981;5:602–619. [Google Scholar]
- Judd CM, Kenny DA, McClelland GH. Estimating and testing mediation and moderation in within-participant designs. Psychological Methods. 2001;6:115–134. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.6.2.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, Knetsch JL, Thaler RH. Anomalies: The endowment effect, Loss aversion, and status quo bias. Journal of Economic Perspectives. 1991;5:193–206. [Google Scholar]
- Keltner D, Locke KD, Audrain PC. The influence of attributions on the relevance of negative feelings to personal satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1993;19:21–29. [Google Scholar]
- Larrick RP. Motivational factors in decision theories: The role of self-protection. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;113:440–450. [Google Scholar]
- Lerner JS, Small DA, Loewenstein G. Heart strings and purse strings: Carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science. 2004;15:337–340. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00679.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loewenstein G, Lerner JS. The role of affect in decision making. In: Davidson R, Goldsmith H, Scherer K, editors. Handbook of affective science. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press; 2003. pp. 619–642. [Google Scholar]
- Salovey P. Mood-induced self-focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;62:699–707. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.62.4.699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz N, Clore GL. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;25:5213–5223. [Google Scholar]
- Silvia PJ, Phillips AG, Baumgaertner MK, Maschauer EL. Emotion concepts and self-focused attention: Exploring parallel effects of emotional states and emotional knowledge. Motivation and Emotion. 2006;30:225–231. [Google Scholar]
- Watkins E, Teasdale JD. Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:353–357. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.110.2.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins E, Teasdale JD. Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2004;82:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood JV, Saltzberg JA, Goldsamt LA. Does affect induce self-focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;58(5):899–908. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.58.5.899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]