Abstract
In odorant-rich environments, animals must be able to detect specific odorants of interest against variable backgrounds. However, several studies have suggested that both humans and rodents are very poor at analyzing the components of odorant mixtures, leading to the idea that olfaction is a synthetic sense in which mixtures are perceived holistically. We have developed a behavioral task to directly measure the ability of mice to perceive mixture components and found that mice can be easily trained to detect target odorants embedded in unpredictable and variable mixtures. We imaged the responses of olfactory bulb glomeruli to the individual odors used in the task in mice expressing the Ca++ indicator GCaMP3 in olfactory receptor neurons. By relating behavioral performance to the glomerular response patterns, we found that the difficulty of segregating the target from the background was strongly dependent on the extent of overlap between the representations of the target and the background odors by olfactory receptors. Our study indicates that the olfactory system has powerful analytic abilities that are constrained by the limits of combinatorial neural representation of odorants at the level of the olfactory receptors.
Introduction
Rodents, like many other species, rely on olfaction for their survival. It is the primary sense for finding food and mates and avoiding predators 1–3. As natural environments are rich in stimuli from multiple and variable sources, identifying any stimulus of interest necessitates segregating it from the background. Scene analysis and segmentation have been studied extensively in vision 4,5 and audition 6,7, but very little is known about analysis of odorous scenes 8,9. Indeed, while contemporary studies in vision and audition focus on the mechanisms underlying scene segmentation, even the mere behavioral ability continues to be questioned for olfaction 9–11.
The difficulty of a complete analysis of an olfactory scene may arise from overlapping representations of odorants in the olfactory bulb. Odorant identity is thought to be combinatorially encoded in the olfactory bulb by the identity of activated receptors/glomeruli 12,13. Each receptor can bind multiple odorants and a single odorant can bind onto multiple receptors. It follows that different odorants will elicit responses that are overlapping in glomerular space, eventually limiting the number of odorants that can be simultaneously encoded 12,14. Indeed, several studies have suggested that the olfactory system has a very limited capacity to encode multiple odorants simultaneously 15–18 or even to detect a specific target odor within a mixture 8,9,19–21, unless odors are dispersed in time 16,22. These studies have contributed to the widely-held view that olfaction is primarily a synthetic sense in which odorant mixtures elicit emergent perceptions at the expense of perceiving the individual components of the mixture 10,11,22,23.
Here we describe a behavioral task that directly tests the ability of mice to detect target stimuli within variable and unpredictable backgrounds. We further describe the relationship between the glomerular patterns of activity evoked by target and background odorants and the difficulty of segregating them.
Results
Mice can segregate odorants from a background
We designed a behavioral task to answer two questions: 1) how does target detection depend on the number of background odorants? and 2) how does target detection depend on the similarity between the target and the background odorants? Previous experiments in rodents have used fixed mixtures over many trials 21,24 or temporally-jittered binary mixtures 25,26 to study object-background segregation. Here, background odors were randomly varied, so that odor stimuli in different trials were rarely similar to each other. Additionally, unlike earlier studies, head-restraining allowed us to use well-controlled and timed stimuli, monitor responses more precisely and obtain thousands of trials.
Mice were trained on a Go/NoGo task, in which they had to detect a target odorant within a mixture of odorants (Fig. 1), delivered through a custom-built olfactometer (Supplementary Fig. 1). Mice reported the presence of the target odorant (Go trial) by licking a water spout in front of their mouth, and reported the absence of the target odorant from the mixture (NoGo trial) by refraining from licking. A correct lick was rewarded by a water drop and an incorrect trial was punished by a 5 second timeout.
Odorant mixtures were chosen from a pool of 16 odorants (Fig. 2) with varying degrees of similarity – 8 of the odorants contained a common functional group (tiglate). Two of the 16 odorants were designated as targets and 14 were background for each mouse. The two target odorants were never presented together, the number of odorants in the mixture was limited to 14, and the probability of any target odorant being present in any single trial was kept constant at 0.5. These constraints yielded a total of 49,149 possible mixtures (rather than 65,536 without constraints). The use of 2 target odorants for each mouse reduced the risk of obtaining results that are highly dependent on the specific target. Furthermore, to avoid obtaining odorant-specific results, 13 mice were trained on 8 different target pairs with every odorant being the target for at least one mouse. Mice were first trained with mixtures that had few odorants until they reached 80% performance, and the distribution of the number of components in the mixture was then gradually adjusted to become uniform, where the probability of different numbers of odorants in the mixture was equal (Fig. 1a-c, see methods). Mice typically reached criterion level performance (80%) within 1000 trials (Fig. 1c and d). When the target odorant tubes were replaced with empty tubes, or switched with background odorants (while keeping reward dependent on the same olfactometer channel where the target odorant used to be), performance dropped to near chance levels, confirming that mice were using odors as cues for performing the task (Fig. 1d). Importantly, mice performed well on novel mixtures in the asymptotic phase (trial novelty rate of > 60%, Fig. 1b), indicating an understanding of the task rules as opposed to learning of specific mixture-response associations.
We analyzed 30,400 trials performed by 13 mice in sessions after learning had reached a plateau and where the number of components in the mixture had a flat probability distribution. The first key result from our experiments is that mice performed well above chance level on mixtures with any number of odorant components, starting at 94% for 1 component and declining to 85% for 14 components (Fig. 1e). Each additional mixture component reduced performance by an average of 0.75%. These data indicate that mice are capable of performing olfactory figure-ground segregation, and that performance degrades steadily with increasing number of background odorants.
The reduced performance for mixtures with more odorant components may reflect a greater degree of overlap in the neural representation of target and background odorants. Additionally, more difficult olfactory discriminations may require longer sampling for a decision to be made 27,28. Perhaps, if mice were allowed longer sampling, performance would not degrade with the number of mixture components. Several findings argue against a limitation in sampling time as the explanation for reduced performance. First, mice were allowed 2 seconds to make a decision and yet over 98% of licks had a latency of less than 1 second (Fig. 3a). Second, response latency was only weakly dependent on the number of odorants in the mixture (mean raw latency increased by 1.2 ms per added component – less than 0.25% of the mean, correlation coefficient 0.7, p = 0.004 Fig. 3b top)29,30. And third, the latencies at which asymptotic performance was reached were very similar in mixtures with different numbers of odorant components - that is, the benefit of longer sampling was similar for mixtures with either many components or just a few (Fig. 3c and d). These results indicate that the difficulty of solving the task does not arise from a sampling time limit, but probably reflects an actual overlap in the neural representation of the mixture components.
Effects of figure and background composition
In sensory systems operating on a well-defined stimulus space such as vision and audition, the difficulty of figure-ground segregation depends on the similarity between features of the figure and the background. Whether a similar dependence exists in the olfactory system has been tested using binary odor mixtures and indirect measures of recognition such as odor investigation time 21,31. To test this hypothesis more directly we analyzed separately trials from mice that were trained to detect tiglate targets (n=8 mice) and trials from mice that were trained to detect non-tiglate targets (n=5 mice). The logic was that tiglate targets (molecules containing the tiglate functional group) would have more similar odors in the background (other tiglates) than non-tiglate targets. Although chemical similarity does not necessarily imply perceptual similarity (discussed below), this analysis provides a link between the behavioral difficulty of figure-background segregation and the chemical features of the target and the background. The analysis of 19,300 trials performed by 8 mice that were trained on tiglate targets and 11,100 trials performed by 5 mice trained to detect non-tiglate targets is shown in figures 4a and 4b, respectively (analysis of individual mice is shown in Supplementary Figs. 2 and 3). Performance dropped steadily with increasing number of background odorants for both groups, yet several differences were evident. Mice trained to detect tiglate targets did worse on the task overall (mean ± confidence interval: 89.7 ± 0.4 and 92.4 ± 0.5 % correct, respectively. p < 10-10, two proportion z-test), and were more affected by the number of components in the mixture than mice trained to detect non-tiglate targets (performance reduction of 0.82 ± 0.06 % and 0.6 ± 0.08 % per added odorant respectively, mean ± 95% confidence interval, p < 0.01). This difference is likely due to the greater similarity of tiglate odorants as a group, acting as better distractors or masks when the target is also a tiglate.
We further tested this hypothesis by analyzing how behavioral performance was affected by the presence of members of the two groups in the mixture. We used NoGo trials for further analysis of behavioral performance since >90% of incorrect responses were due to false alarms (Fig. 4a and b). We calculated the performance of mice detecting tiglates and mice detecting non-tiglates for all combinations of the numbers of tiglates and non-tiglates in the background mixture (Fig. 4c and d, respectively). The performance of mice detecting tiglate targets was dependent on the number of tiglate background odorants but was not affected much by non-tiglates (Fig. 4c). Conversely, the performance of mice detecting non-tiglate targets was independent of tiglate background but decreased as the number of non-tiglates increased (Fig. 4d). To quantify the effect of tiglates as background we calculated the slopes of the rows in the matrices in figures 4c and 4d, which allowed us to assess performance as a function of the number of tiglates in the background for a given number of non-tiglates (Fig. 4c&d, bottom). Similarly, the slopes of the columns of these matrices were used to quantify the effect of non-tiglates as background odorants (Fig. 4c and 4d, Right). The distributions of these slopes for both tiglate and non-tiglate targets, are shown in figures 4e and 4f, respectively. The performance of mice detecting tiglate targets decreased by an average of 3.4±0.4% for each added background tiglate but was unaffected by additional non-tiglates (+0.4±0.5%; the two slopes are significantly different, p = 1.6×10-4, Mann-Whitney U-test). Conversely the performance of mice detecting non-tiglate targets decreased by an average of 2.7±0.5% for each non-tiglate added to the background but was unaffected by background tiglates (0.0±0.5%; slope difference significant, p = 1.9×10-4, Mann-Whitney U-test). A similar dependence between the group effect of background odorants and the target was also found when analyzing the behavior of individual mice, separately (Supplementary Fig. 4). The uncertain relationship between chemical similarity of odorants and the corresponding perceptual similarity 32 raises the question whether other groupings may also show a strong behavioral effect. We repeated the analysis of group effect as done for tiglates and non-tiglates, this time with all possible divisions of the 16 odorants into two groups of 8 (there are 12870 possible 8-member groups). The distributions of group effects are shown in figures 4g and 4h, for mice detecting tiglate targets and non-tiglate targets, respectively. Notably, tiglates were among the 2 percentile of groups with the strongest negative effect for mice detecting tiglates. Analyzing the 50 groups with the strongest effects, we found that tiglates were very common members of these groups for mice trained to detect tiglates (Fig. 4i, p = 0.02, Mann-Whitney U-test of fraction member for tiglates vs non-tiglates) but not for mice trained to detect non-tiglates (Fig. 4j, p = 0.39).
Glomerular activity patterns
The analysis described above was based on the similarity of functional groups between the odorants. However, the fact that two odorants contain a similar functional group does not necessarily imply that they are similar from the perspective of the nose. To obtain an independent physiological measure of the similarity between the odorants, we imaged glomerular responses to all odorants on the dorsal surface of the olfactory bulb 33–37. Glomerular input activity directly reflects the olfactory receptors that are activated by the odorants and previous studies have found a correlation between the similarity of glomerular response patterns and the difficulty of discrimination 29. Naïve anesthetized mice were used for these experiments to measure the similarity and overlap of odorant representations before training.
We measured odor-evoked responses in mice expressing the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP3 in olfactory receptor neurons (Fig. 5a) 38. Example responses from the individual glomeruli from one experiment are shown in figures 5b-d. The response to each odorant was represented as a vector of glomerular activity and the cross-correlation between these vectors was used as a measure of response similarity between pairs of odorants (Fig. 5d and 5e). When pooling all pairwise correlations from 6 experiments, tiglate to tiglate correlations (r = 0.49 ± 0.02, n=168) were higher than both non-tiglate to non-tiglate (r = 0.43 ± 0.02, n=168, p < 0.05, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, ks = 0.17) and tiglate to non-tiglate correlations (r = 0.45 ± 0.01, n=384, p < 0.05, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, ks = 0.16, Figs. 5f and 5g). Although the difference between the correlation distributions was significant, these distributions were highly overlapping and therefore could not fully explain the behavioral dependence on the odorant groups. Furthermore, comparison of the pairwise correlations within individual subjects revealed significant similarity of tiglates in only 2 of the 6 experiments. Nevertheless, to assess the significance of the division of the odorants into groups based on the tiglate functional group, we repeated the same analysis for all possible divisions of the 16 odorant pool into two groups of 8. For each group, we calculated the distribution of response correlations within the group and across groups. The separability of each group was measured by the Kolmogorov-Smirnov distance between these distributions (see Methods). Only 6% of odorant groups were more separable than tiglates (Fig. 5i).
Taken together with the analysis of the behavioral data, these results indicate that the difficulty of figure-ground segregation in olfaction cannot be fully explained by the similarity between the representations of figure and background components at the level of olfactory bulb inputs.
Glomerular map overlap predicts performance
The decline in performance with increased similarity between the target and background components can be explained either as reflecting a decreased sensitivity for the target, or as reflecting a perceived similarity of the mixture to the target. Due to the asymmetry of the task (mice were rewarded for hits but not for correct rejections), mice can be expected to lick both when they sense the target (or think they do) and when they realize that the target is masked and cannot be detected. We tested each of these hypotheses by asking whether behavioral performance correlated with the degree of masking of target-evoked glomerular responses by background mixtures, or whether performance correlates with the similarity between target-evoked glomerular responses and mixture-evoked glomerular responses. We based our analysis on a simple model that estimates mixture responses based on the responses to the individual components. Mixture responses can involve non-linear interactions anywhere within the mouse brain, and therefore difficult to predict. We made the simplest assumption, and modeled the mixture response as a linear sum of the responses to the individual components. At least at the level of glomerular inputs, responses to mixtures can be described as the linear sum of responses to the individual components 17,31,34,39,40. Based on the linear summation model, each mixture presented in a behavioral trial was assigned a “masking” value and a “similarity” value. Masking reflected the fraction of target-evoked activity that is covered by the mixture-evoked activity (Fig. 6a, see methods). As a measure of similarity we used the correlation between the target-evoked response and the mixture-evoked response.
Analyzing all NoGo trials, we found that performance significantly degraded with increasing masking (Fig. 6b, correlation coefficient = 0.78, p = 4.7×10-7, n=30, 500-trial bins) but not with similarity (Fig. 6c, correlation coefficient = 0.36 p = 0.05, n=30, 500-trial bins). Although the increase in both masking and correlation was accompanied by an increase in the average number of mixture components (Fig. 6d and e), similar trends were evident even within a fixed number of components for both masking (p < 0.01 for mixtures of more than 3 components, Fig. 6f) and correlation (p < 0.01 for mixtures of more than 10 components, Fig. 6g). Interestingly, masking values of less than 0.7 had no effect on behavioral performance, suggesting redundancy in the coding of the target by multiple glomeruli. To ensure that the above results are not simply due to our assumption of linear summation of glomerular responses for mixture stimuli, we tested another model which assumed no summation. Instead, this model assumed that the mixture response in each glomerulus is the maximum of the individual odorant responses (maximal intensity projection). We found a similar drop in performance with increased masking using the maximal intensity projection model as well (correlation coefficient = 0.68, p = 3.9×10-5, Supplementary Fig. 5), indicating that this drop in performance with increased masking is not sensitive to the exact model of summation.
These data show that the behavioral difficulty of detecting a target odorant within a mixture is closely linked to the constraints of combinatorial coding by glomeruli in the olfactory bulb, and not to similarity of the target and background mixtures. Mixtures components that are similar to the target act as powerful distractors not by making the mixture more similar to the target, but by masking target-evoked activity.
Discussion
We analyzed quantitatively the behavioral ability of mice to detect odorants of interest in the presence of variable backgrounds – a feature that is probably fundamental to the survival of many species. Our main findings are that: (1) mice are highly capable of performing such a task, (2) similarity between mixture components and the target odorant determines the difficulty of the task, and (3) the difficulty of the task depends on the extent to which background odorants and the target odorants overlap in the glomerular activity patterns they elicit. Presumably, the last two findings are related because it is likely that inclusion of background odorants that are similar to the target increases the extent of glomerular activity overlap.
Our finding that mice can detect the presence (or absence) of the target when mixtures with many components are presented, is in contrast with the common view of olfaction as a synthetic sense where mixtures of odorants are perceived as a whole rather than being analyzed to detect and identify their components 8–11,19–23. This view largely relies on the observation that humans are very poor at analyzing mixtures or even detecting target odorants within mixtures 8–10. Studies using rodents also inferred poor detection of single odorants within mixtures, but these were based on either spontaneous non-rewarded discrimination 21, or an assumption that animals generalize task rules 19. The data presented here indicates that macrosmatic mammals can detect single odorants within mixtures. In fact, anecdotal data has already shown that rats can discriminate between two mixtures of 10 odorants that differ by only one component 24. While demonstrating strong analytical abilities, our data should not be taken as evidence against synthesis in olfaction. It is possible that the olfactory system has both analytic and synthetic abilities allowing for both grouping of odorants into objects as well as differentiating objects. Whether a specific odorant should be searched for in a mixture or whether it should be synthetically combined with other odorants to identify an object, may require learning of its behavioral relevance and may be context dependent.
We found that increasing the number of background components gradually and steadily decreased behavioral accuracy. A recent study found that the structure of stimulus presentation can greatly affect decision accuracy and that grouping trials of similar difficulty into blocks within behavioral sessions may increase behavioral accuracy by allowing a more accurate inner representation of the boundary between rewarded and unrewarded stimuli 30. Because trials of varying difficulties were interleaved in our experiments, it is possible that mice can perform even better in more predictable conditions.
Our experimental design allowed us to dissect the features of background mixtures that affected detection of target odorants. We found that odorants that contain the functional group tiglate were strong maskers for targets that contained the same functional group but not for other targets. For the odorant pool used in this set of experiments, a common tiglate functional group was also accompanied by higher similarity of glomerular response patterns, yet this difference was too small to account for the behavioral effect. Taken together these findings imply that not all odorants are equally potent as maskers and that the potency of any odorant is related to its similarity to the specific target it has to mask.
We used a simple model to estimate the glomerular response patterns elicited by all mixtures that were presented in the behavioral task. Using this model we show that performance can be explained by the extent to which mixtures evoke glomerular activity patterns that overlap with the target. Importantly, similarity between mixture- and target-evoked activities does not explain behavioral performance. Glomerular response patterns were recorded from naïve animals to represent the initial input signals the mice have to deal with during training. How exposure and learning related changes in glomerular patterns 41–43contribute to performance on the task will be the subject of future studies.
Our model assumed no lateral interactions. While this has shown to be the case at the input stage 17,31,34,39,40, lateral interactions downstream may certainly play a role in mixture analysis. Because odor-specific lateral interactions are not well characterized in the olfactory bulb of mammals, we chose to keep the model simple. An upper bound and a lower bound to mixture response estimates were yielded by a linear model (Fig. 6) and a ‘maximal projection intensity’ model (Supplementary Fig. 5), respectively. Both models reached similar conclusions. Future experiments will be directed at the mechanism by which the olfactory cocktail party problem is solved. Potential mechanisms include hard-wired strategies analogous to pre-attentive segmentation in the visual system 44. However the fact that behavioral performance only degraded at high values of glomerular masking suggests that learning-related changes in either the olfactory bulb circuits or cortical circuits are involved 45–47. Additionally, cortico-bulbar feedback 48,49 may enable flexible context-dependent classification of stimuli. A temporal jitter between odorant onset has been suggested to contribute to scene segmentation 16,25,26. Similarly, in natural turbulent environments, covariance of components from a common source may also be instrumental for scene segmentation by providing the temporal jitter 50. Careful analysis of combined behavioral and physiological recordings will be critical to uncover the mechanisms by which figure-ground segregation is achieved in the olfactory system.
Methods
All experimental procedures were performed using approved protocols in accordance with institutional (Harvard University IACUC) and national guidelines.
1. Behavior
a. Subjects and surgery
Thirteen c57bl6 adult male mice (Charles River) were trained on the behavioral task. No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample sizes but our sample sizes are similar to those reported in previous publications (refs). Mice were first anesthetized (Ketamine/Xylazine 100 and 10 mg/kg, respectively) and a metal plate was attached to their skull with dental acrylic for subsequent head restraining. Mice were then maintained in a reversed light/dark cycle facility. All behavioral training and testing was done during the subjective night time.
b. Apparatus
The behavioral apparatus was located inside a sound attenuating box (Med Associates, VT USA) and consisted of a head restraining device, an odor delivery system, a lick detector and a water delivery system. Odor delivery, monitoring of licking and water rewards were controlled using computer interface hardware (National Instruments) and custom software written in LabView. The mouse was continuously monitored using a CCD camera during behavior sessions.
c. Mixture presentation
Odorant mixtures were presented using a custom-made olfactometer that was designed to have constant flow (2.5 liters/minute) and to have the concentrations of the different odorants independent of each other (Supplementary Fig. 1). The olfactometer was composed of 16 odor modules. Each module was made of two glass tubes, one containing the odor and one containing only the odor solvent. A 3-way valve (Lee Company, USA) diverted an input flow of filtered air to either the odor tube or the solvent tube, and the output of both tubes was merged to form the module output. This design ensured that each module contributed a constant amount of flow at any time. Input flow to the modules and output flow from the modules were made of FEP-lined Tygon/PVC tubing connected in symmetric pair-wise bifurcations to ensure equal flow on all modules. All odorants were diluted to 2% v/v in diethyl phthalate in the tubes and then further diluted by the flow of other modules 16 fold. The odorant mixture was carried from the point of final odorant convergence to the odor port through a 4 foot long tubing with an inner diameter of 1/16 inch to allow mixing while minimizing the latency from valve opening to odor presentation at the mouse's nostrils. Olfactometer output was analyzed with a photoionization detector (miniPID, Aurora Scientific) to test for latency (170 ms), repeatability (mean coefficient of variation of 6 ± 0.8%), and independence of the modules (see Supplementary Fig. 1).
c. Behavioral training and testing
Following 1 week of recovery from surgery to implant headplates, mice were water deprived. Mice were acclimatized to the behavioral apparatus for two days in which they were allowed 30 minutes of free exploration with free water available at the water port. This was followed by an additional day in which they were head restrained and were allowed free water from the port. After acclimatization, mice were trained on the behavioral task. No randomized process was used to assign target odorants to each mouse. A mixture was presented for 2 seconds every 10 seconds and mice had to respond correctly within the 2 second period. Correct licks were rewarded with a 10μL water drop, correct rejections were not rewarded, and incorrect trials were punished by a 5 second timeout. Training began with easy sessions and as mice reached 80% performance over a whole session, session difficulty was adjusted. The difficulty of the task was controlled by varying the distribution of the number of components in the mixture using the following equation:
where p(x) is the probability of x components (ranging from 1 to 14). The parameter b was first set to 0.25 and was raised in 0.25 increments when mice reached performance of 80% correct until reaching b=1. This allowed varying task difficulty without any changes in task rules. The number of training sessions varied across mice, ranging from 4 to 19, with an average and standard deviation of 9.2 ± 5 sessions. Unless specified otherwise, all data presented were taken from sessions with flat distributions of the number of components in the mixture (b=1). Mice performed one session per day with an average of 346 ± 90 trials per session (lasting typically about an hour), and each mouse performed between 4 and 10 test session (6.7 ± 1.8 mean ± SD). Data collection and analysis were not performed blind to the conditions of the experiments
d. Odor set
Since odorant space is not easily parameterized and defined 32,51, a complete analysis of the behavioral capacity is impossible and odorant choice may affect the conclusions from behavioral experiments. We chose the odorants to be used in the task with two goals in mind: 1) making the task difficult, to allow for failures in performance, and 2) having variable similarities between odorants to study the dependence of difficulty of figure-ground segregation on figure-ground similarity. With the difficulty of defining odorant similarity, we settled on 8 odorants from the tiglate family and 8 other odorants (non-tiglates) for the task (Fig. 2).
Odorants were obtained from Sigma Aldrich (Ethyl tiglate, Allyl tiglate, Hexyl tiglate, Methyl tiglate, Isopropyl tiglate, Ethyl propionate, 2-Ethylhexanal, 4-Allylanisole, ±Citronellal, Isobutyl propionate, Allyl butirate, Propyl acetate), Penta (Benzyl tiglate, Phenylethyl tiglate, Cytronellyl tiglate), and Alfa Aesar (Ethyl valerate). The target odorant pairs and the number of mice trained to detect each pair are as follows: Ethyl tiglate and Allyl tiglate - 2 mice. Hexyl tiglate and Methyl tiglate - 1 mouse. Isopropyl tiglate and Citronellyl tiglate - 1 mouse. Benzyl tiglate and Phenylethyl tiglate - 4 mice. Ethylpropionate and 2-Ethylhexanal – 1 mouse. Propyl acetate and 4-Allylanisole – 1 mouse. Ethylvalerate and Citronellal - 2 mice. Isobutyl propionate and Allyl butyrate – 1 mouse.
e. Data analysis
(i) latency measurement: Latency was defined as the time from odor valve opening to lick-detection. Based on photoionization detector measurements, 170 ms were taken off all latency values to account for the time it takes for the odor to travel from the valve to the mouse nose. To remove intersession variability in response latencies that most likely reflected variability of the positioning of the water port, latency values were normalized to the mean value of their corresponding session. Values (between 0 and 1) were then multiplied by the mean of all trials from all sessions to obtain values that can be compared to the un-normalized values. Data distribution (shown in Fig. 3) was assumed to be normal but this was not formally tested. (ii) Analysis of odorant group effects on behavior (Fig. 4): To estimate the effect of tiglates as a group on behavioral performance we plotted the percent of correct rejections as a function of the number of background tiglates, while holding the number of non-tiglates constant. This was done for all numbers of non-tiglates, yielding 9 such curves (0 to 8 non-tiglate background odorants). Linear fits were made to all 9 curves and the average slope of these curves was taken to be the group effect of tiglates. The same analysis was repeated for other groups of 8 odorants within the 16 odorant pool.
2. Imaging
a. Experimental procedure
Adult OMP-GCaMP3 mice 38 were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine (100 and 10 mg/kg, respectively) the cranial bones over the olfactory bulbs were thinned with a dental drill to allow clear visualization.
Two photo lenses coupled front to front were used to image the olfactory bulb surface onto the sensor of a CMOS camera (DFK 23GPO31, The Imaging Source GmbH). Images (640 × 480 pixels) were acquired at 8 bit resolution and 4-6 frames/sec. Data from the camera was recorded to the computer via data acquisition hardware (National instruments). A blue LED (CBT-90, Luminus, Billerica, Massachusetts), coupled to an optical fiber was used for excitation.
All odorants were diluted to 5% v/v in diethyl phthalate and were delivered using an automated olfactometer 36. The 16 odors and an additional tube containing only diethyl phthalate were presented in random order and each odor was repeated 3-5 times. Odors were presented every 45 seconds for 3 seconds with data acquisition beginning 3 seconds before and ending 3 seconds after odor presentation.
b. Data analysis
The acquired sequence of images was converted to DF/F images (change in fluorescence/ resting fluorescence). We then calculated the maximal response at each pixel for all 16 odorants. These maximum response maps were used to identify putative glomeruli as regions of interest (ROIs) for further analysis (ranging from 55 to 76 ROIs per experiment). The response at each ROI was quantified by integrating the DF/F signal during odor presentation. This procedure yielded responses that were represented as response vectors in a space that is defined by the ROIs.
(i) Analysis of glomerular map similarity
In order to quantify the similarity between the activity patterns of different odors, we calculated the cross-correlation between the response vectors of pairs of odors. We analyzed the separability of odorant groups by calculating the Kolmogorov-Smirnov distances between the distributions of correlation coefficients within a group and across groups. A high Kolmogorov-Smirnov distance would mean that the similarity among members of the group is higher than the similarity between members of the group and other odorants.
(ii) Analysis of mixture masking and similarity to target
Two models were used to estimate the extent to which a mixture masks the target at the level of olfactory bulb glomeruli, a linear model and a maximal intensity projection model. Both models assumed that there are no lateral interactions between glomeruli at the level of inputs. The first model assumed linear summation at each glomerulus to obtain an upper bound to the mixture response. That is – the response to a mixture would be the exact linear sum of the responses to the components. The second model provided an alternative of deriving mixture responses and assumed no summation. Here, the response to a mixture at each glomerulus was taken to be the maximum of the responses at that glomerulus to the mixture components.
Masking at each target-activated glomerulus was calculated using the following equation:
Where Mi(m,T) is the masking of target T by mixture m at the ith glomerulus, ai(m) and ai(T) are the responses of the ith glomerulus to mixture m and target T, respectively. Only glomeruli that were activated by the target above threshold (3 standard deviations away from the baseline) were considered for this analysis to avoid divergence due to small denominators (see Supplementary Fig. 6 for a test of robustness). The masking value for the mixture was then taken to be the average masking of all target-activated glomeruli:
Where M(m,T) is the masking of target T by mixture m and N is the number of target-activated glomeruli. Glomeruli that were not activated by the target were not considered for this analysis. Masking values were calculated for the two targets and the larger of these was taken to be the mixture's masking value, with the assumption that it is enough to mask one target to preclude the mouse from being able to make a decision. The relationship between the masking index and behavioral performance was quantified by first binning trials according to their masking index and plotting the mean performance in each bin against the mean masking index. A decaying logistic function was fit to these data:
where a is the saturated performance and s and b determine the slope of the decay and the shift along the masking axis. M is the masking index value.
The similarity to the target was calculated for each mixture as the cross-correlation between the target and the mixture response vectors. The relationship between the target-mixture similarity and behavioral performance was quantified in the same way as for masking.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Naoshige Uchida, Rachel Wilson and members of our laboratory for comments on the manuscript. Work in VNM's laboratory was supported by grants from the NIH. DR was supported by a fellowship from the Edmond and Lily Safra Center for Brain Sciences, Hebrew University.
Footnotes
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Author contributions: DR and VNM conceived and designed the experiments. DR and VH collected the behavioral data. VK collected the imaging data. DR analyzed the behavioral data. DR and VK analyzed the imaging data. DR and VNM wrote the manuscript.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- 1.Apfelbach R, Blanchard CD, Blanchard RJ, Hayes RA, McGregor IS. The effects of predator odors in mammalian prey species: A review of field and laboratory studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:1123–1144. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Howard WE, Marsh RE, Cole RE. Food detection by deer mice using olfactory rather than visual cues. Anim Behav. 1968;16:13–17. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(68)90100-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Blaustein AR. Sexual Selection and Mammalian Olfaction. Am Nat. 1981;117:1006–1010. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Crouzet SM, Serre T. What are the visual features underlying rapid object recognition? Front Percept Sci. 2011;2:326. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wolfson SS, Landy MS. Examining edge- and region-based texture analysis mechanisms. Vision Res. 1998;38:439–446. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00153-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Elhilali M, Shamma SA. A cocktail party with a cortical twist: How cortical mechanisms contribute to sound segregation. J Acoust Soc Am. 2008;124:3751–3771. doi: 10.1121/1.3001672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McDermott JH. The cocktail party problem. Curr Biol CB. 2009;19:R1024–1027. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Jinks A, Laing DG. A limit in the processing of components in odour mixtures. Perception. 1999;28:395–404. doi: 10.1068/p2898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Laing DG, Francis GW. The capacity of humans to identify odors in mixtures. Physiol Behav. 1989;46:809–814. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90041-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jinks A, Laing DG. The analysis of odor mixtures by humans: evidence for a configurational process. Physiol Behav. 2001;72:51–63. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00407-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wilson DA, Stevenson RJ. Olfactory perceptual learning: the critical role of memory in odor discrimination. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27:307–328. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(03)00050-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hopfield JJ. Odor space and olfactory processing: Collective algorithms and neural implementation. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1999;96:12506–12511. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.22.12506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Polak EH. Multiple profile-multiple receptor site model for vertebrate olfaction. J Theor Biol. 1973;40:469–484. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(73)90005-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Koulakov A, Gelperin A, Rinberg D. Olfactory Coding With All-or-Nothing Glomeruli. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3134–3142. doi: 10.1152/jn.00560.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Giraudet P, Berthommier F, Chaput M. Mitral Cell Temporal Response Patterns Evoked by Odor Mixtures in the Rat Olfactory Bulb. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88:829–838. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shen K, Tootoonian S, Laurent G. Encoding of Mixtures in a Simple Olfactory System. Neuron. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tabor R, Yaksi E, Weislogel JM, Friedrich RW. Processing of Odor Mixtures in the Zebrafish Olfactory Bulb. J Neurosci. 2004;24:6611–6620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1834-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Arnson HA, Holy TE. Robust Encoding of Stimulus Identity and Concentration in the Accessory Olfactory System. J Neurosci. 2013;33:13388–13397. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0967-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Frederick DE, Barlas L, Ievins A, Kay LM. A critical test of the overlap hypothesis for odor mixture perception. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123:430–437. doi: 10.1037/a0014729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Laska M, Hudson R. Discriminating parts from the whole: determinants of odor mixture perception in squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus. J Comp Physiol [A] 1993;173:249–256. doi: 10.1007/BF00192984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wiltrout C, Dogra S, Linster C. Configurational and nonconfigurational interactions between odorants in binary mixtures. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:236–245. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.2.236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kay LM, Crk T, Thorngate J. A redefinition of odor mixture quality. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:726–733. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wilson DA. Pattern Separation and Completion in Olfaction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1170:306–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04017.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Barnes DC, Hofacer RD, Zaman AR, Rennaker RL, Wilson DA. Olfactory perceptual stability and discrimination. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:1378–1380. doi: 10.1038/nn.2217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Linster C, Henry L, Kadohisa M, Wilson DA. Synaptic adaptation and odor-background segmentation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;87:352–360. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Saha D, et al. A spatiotemporal coding mechanism for background-invariant odor recognition. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:1830–1839. doi: 10.1038/nn.3570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Abraham NM, et al. Maintaining accuracy at the expense of speed: stimulus similarity defines odor discrimination time in mice. Neuron. 2004;44:865–876. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rinberg D, Koulakov A, Gelperin A. Speed-accuracy tradeoff in olfaction. Neuron. 2006;51:351–358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Uchida N, Mainen ZF. Speed and accuracy of olfactory discrimination in the rat. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1224–1229. doi: 10.1038/nn1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zariwala HA, Kepecs A, Uchida N, Hirokawa J, Mainen ZF. The Limits of Deliberation in a Perceptual Decision Task. Neuron. 2013;78:339–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Grossman KJ, Mallik AK, Ross J, Kay LM, Issa NP. Glomerular activation patterns and the perception of odor mixtures. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27:2676–2685. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06213.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Secundo L, Snitz K, Sobel N. The perceptual logic of smell. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;25C:107–115. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ma L, et al. Distributed representation of chemical features and tunotopic organization of glomeruli in the mouse olfactory bulb. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109:5481–5486. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117491109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.McGann JP, et al. Odorant Representations Are Modulated by Intra- but Not Interglomerular Presynaptic Inhibition of Olfactory Sensory Neurons. Neuron. 2005;48:1039–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Meister M, Bonhoeffer T. Tuning and topography in an odor map on the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2001;21:1351–1360. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-04-01351.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Soucy ER, Albeanu DF, Fantana AL, Murthy VN, Meister M. Precision and diversity in an odor map on the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:210–220. doi: 10.1038/nn.2262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Uchida N, Takahashi YK, Tanifuji M, Mori K. Odor maps in the mammalian olfactory bulb: domain organization and odorant structural features. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1035–1043. doi: 10.1038/79857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Isogai Y, et al. Molecular organization of vomeronasal chemoreception. Nature. 2011;478:241–245. doi: 10.1038/nature10437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fletcher ML. Analytical Processing of Binary Mixture Information by Olfactory Bulb Glomeruli. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e29360. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lin DY, Shea SD, Katz LC. Representation of Natural Stimuli in the Rodent Main Olfactory Bulb. Neuron. 2006;50:937–949. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kass MD, Moberly AH, Rosenthal MC, Guang SA, McGann JP. Odor-Specific, Olfactory Marker Protein-Mediated Sparsening of Primary Olfactory Input to the Brain after Odor Exposure. J Neurosci. 2013;33:6594–6602. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1442-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Abraham NM, Vincis R, Lagier S, Rodriguez I, Carleton A. Long term functional plasticity of sensory inputs mediated by olfactory learning. eLife. 2014;3 doi: 10.7554/eLife.02109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Jones SV, Choi DC, Davis M, Ressler KJ. Learning-dependent structural plasticity in the adult olfactory pathway. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2008;28:13106–13111. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4465-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Treisman AM, Gelade G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit Psychol. 1980;12:97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chapuis J, Wilson DA. Bidirectional plasticity of cortical pattern recognition and behavioral sensory acuity. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15:155–161. doi: 10.1038/nn.2966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Choi GB, et al. Driving Opposing Behaviors with Ensembles of Piriform Neurons. Cell. 2011;146:1004–1015. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.07.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Haberly LB. Parallel-distributed processing in olfactory cortex: new insights from morphological and physiological analysis of neuronal circuitry. Chem Senses. 2001;26:551–576. doi: 10.1093/chemse/26.5.551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Boyd AM, Sturgill JF, Poo C, Isaacson JS. Cortical feedback control of olfactory bulb circuits. Neuron. 2012;76:1161–1174. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Markopoulos F, Rokni D, Gire DH, Murthy VN. Functional Properties of Cortical Feedback Projections to the Olfactory Bulb. Neuron. 2012;76:1175–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Brody CD, Hopfield JJ. Simple networks for spike-timing-based computation, with application to olfactory processing. Neuron. 2003;37:843–852. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00120-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Koulakov AA, Rinberg D. In search of the structure of human olfactory space. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5:65. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.