Skip to main content
European Journal of Psychotraumatology logoLink to European Journal of Psychotraumatology
. 2014 Aug 27;5:10.3402/ejpt.v5.23870. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v5.23870

Traumatic experiences, alexithymia, and posttraumatic symptomatology: a cross-sectional population-based study in Germany

Svenja Eichhorn 1,*, Elmar Brähler 1,2, Matthias Franz 3, Michael Friedrich 1, Heide Glaesmer 1
PMCID: PMC4149745  PMID: 25206956

Abstract

Objective

Previous studies have established an association between number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia. The present study examines this relationship in a large-scale representative sample of the German general population (N=2,507) and explores the potential mediating effects of posttraumatic symptomatology, particularly avoidance/numbing.

Methods

Alexithymia was assessed with the German version of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20). Posttraumatic symptomatology was operationalized by the symptom score of the modified German version of the Posttraumatic Symptom Scale, and traumatic experiences were assessed with the trauma list of the Munich Composite International Diagnostic Interview. Two mediation analyses were conducted.

Results

Of the total sample, 24.2% (n=606) reported at least one traumatic experience, 10.6% (n=258) were classified as alexithymic, and 2.4% (n=59) fulfilled the criteria of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Participants who had survived five or more traumatic experiences had significantly higher alexithymia sum scores. The PTSD symptom cluster avoidance/numbing mediated the association between the number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia.

Conclusions

Our findings illustrate an association between number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia and the influence of emotional avoidance and numbing within this relationship. The significant relationship between alexithymia and number of traumatic experiences in a general population sample further supports the concept of multiple and complex traumatization as associated with alexithymia. The results suggest the importance of further investigations determining the causal impact of alexithymia both as a potential premorbid trait and as consequence of traumatization. Lastly, future investigations are needed to clarify alexithymia as a distinct trauma-relevant characteristic for better diagnostics and specialized trauma-integrative therapy.

Keywords: PTSD, TAS-20, multiple and complex traumatization, avoidance/numbing, mediation


Alexithymia is defined as the limited ability to identify and describe feelings, including external-oriented thinking (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994a; Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994b). Alexithymia has been investigated in various populations over the last several years. Etiological theories have identified alexithymia as a consequence of environmental influences (Gundel, Ceballos-Baumann, & Von Rad, 2002; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997) and have, in addition, suggested a genetic pathway for developing alexithymia (Jorgensen et al., 2002). Taylor and Bagby recently presented an overview of alexithymia in the context of psychoanalysis and empirical research (Taylor & Bagby, 2013). The impact of alexithymia and an external-oriented thinking on anxiety patients’ motivation for seeking psychosocial treatment was reported by Rufer, Moergeli, Moritz, Drabe, and Weidt (2014).

Traumatization has often been investigated as an environmental factor for alexithymia. To date, traumatization, occurring both in childhood and in adulthood, has been identified as the most important known risk factor for developing alexithymia. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are therefore frequently discussed factors connecting traumatic experiences and alexithymia (Declercq, Vanheule, & Deheegher, 2010; Fukunishi, Sasaki, Chishima, Anze, & Saijo, 1996; McCaslin et al., 2006; Sondergaard & Theorell, 2004; Spitzer et al., 2013; Spitzer, Vogel, Barnow, Freyberger, & Grabe, 2007; Zlotnick, Mattia, & Zimmerman, 2001). So far, this relationship has been poorly investigated in population-based studies. Yehuda et al. found a positive association between PTSD and alexithymia in Holocaust survivors, but no association between traumatic experiences and alexithymia. Because, according to their results, alexithymia seems more connected to posttraumatic symptoms than to trauma exposure itself, they conclude that alexithymia might be a preexisting condition that increases the probability of developing PTSD (Yehuda et al., 1997). Other studies showed that the inability to identify one's own feelings is highly congruent with the construct of PTSD (Evren, Dalbudak, Cetin, Durkaya, & Evren, 2010; Sondergaard & Theorell, 2004). Several studies indicate a phenomenological overlap between emotional numbing as a symptom of PTSD and alexithymia, which is also characterized by disordered affect regulation (Badura, 2003; Declercq et al., 2010; Fukunishi et al., 1996). Frewen, Dozois, Neufeld, and Lanius (2008) point out that the type of trauma experienced is an important factor influencing the probability of developing alexithymia. Complex or multiple traumatization is thought to increase the risk of alexithymia (Cloitre, Scarvalone, & Difede, 1997).

In conclusion, alexithymia is, on the one hand, often considered a premorbid trait that facilitates the development of PTSD as a response to traumatization and a risk factor for a chronic course of PTSD. On the other hand, there has been theoretical discussion, suggesting that alexithymia might be a component of PTSD and therefore one of the consequences of traumatic experiences. The purpose of the current study was to evaluate this idea in a population-based study.

The prevalence of traumatic experiences and PTSD within the general population has been examined internationally in several studies (Breslau, 2009; Kessler, 2005; Kessler, Kammerer, Hoffmann, & Traue, 2010; Van Ameringen, Mancini, Patterson, & Boyle, 2008). In Germany, the percentage of people in the population who have had at least one traumatic experience ranges from 24 to 55% (Hauffa et al., 2011; Maercker, Forstmeier, Wagner, Glaesmer, & Brähler, 2008; Spitzer et al., 2008), much lower than the 90% or more rate reported in US studies (Breslau, 2009). One-month-prevalence of PTSD in the German population is comparable to Canadian findings with rates between 2 and 3% (Maercker et al., 2008; Van Ameringen et al., 2008).

Very few population-based studies have focused on alexithymia. Those that do exist indicate prevalence rates ranging from 10% in the German population (Franz et al., 2008) to 26% in a Finnish study (Tolmunen et al., 2011). The present work constitutes the first international population-based study (N=2,507) examining the association between number of traumatic experiences, alexithymia, and PTSD symptomatology. We were especially interested in the mediating effect of the PTSD symptom cluster avoidance/numbing on this association.

After reporting alexithymia and PTSD prevalences and means, the following hypotheses were tested:

  1. People with a higher number of traumatic experiences will show increased alexithymia manifestation.

  2. Posttraumatic symptomatology will have a mediating effect on the association between the number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia.

  3. The PTSD symptom cluster avoidance/numbing is the main mediating factor affecting the association between number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia.

It should be noted that the data are cross-sectional in nature and do not support a causal analysis.

Figure 1 illustrates total (path c), direct (path c′), and indirect effects (path c minus path c′) (Modell A and B) between the variables tested.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Tested models of total, direct, and indirect effects.

To report a mediation-effect, the following requirements have to be fulfilled (Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004):

  1. Significant relation between predictor and outcome (see path c, Fig. 1)

  2. Significant relation between predictor and mediator (see path a, Fig. 1)

  3. Significant relation between mediator and outcome (see path b, Fig. 1), if controlled for the predictor's effect on the outcome

  4. The strength of the relation between predictor and outcome is significantly reduced when the mediator is added to the model (see path c′, Fig. 1/to be tested with the Sobel test)

Methods

Participants and study protocol

A representative sample of the German general population was selected with the assistance of a demographic consulting company (USUMA, Berlin, Germany). The total area of Germany was separated into 258 sample areas representing the different regions of the country. Households of the respective area and members of these households fulfilling the inclusion criteria (age 14 or above, able to read and understand the German language) were selected randomly. The sample is representative in terms of age and sex. A first attempt to establish contact was made at 4,243 addresses, of which 4,118 were valid. If a candidate could not be reached on the first try, a maximum of three additional attempts were made to contact them. Participants were visited by study assistants, informed about the investigation, and given self-rating questionnaires. A total of 2,549 people agreed to participate and completed the self-rating questionnaires in July and August of 2005 (participation rate: 61.9%). The assistants waited until participants answered all questionnaires, and offered help if the meaning of questions was not clear. After elimination of 42 interview-sets due to missing data, 2,507 (60.9%) participants were included in the analysis (M: 49.2 years; SD: 17.9 years). The survey met the ethical guidelines of the international code of marketing and social research practice by the international chamber of commerce and the European society of opinion and marketing research.

To test the impact of posttraumatic symptomatology on the association between number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia, two mediation analyses were conducted (Model A and Model B) on a subsample of participants who had either experienced at least one traumatic event (n=606) or fulfilled A1-criterion according to the DSM-VI-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). After eliminating missing values, the analyses included n=468 participants. Table 1 gives an overview of the demographic characteristics of the full study sample (N=2,507) and the subsample with trauma experience (n=606). The most important difference between the full study sample and the subsample is age, most likely because of the preconditioned A1-trauma criterion. This means the subsample contains more elderly participants, who lived through World War II and were thus more likely to have experienced traumas associated with the exceptional nature of that situation, including among others, higher than average rates of widowhood.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic characteristics

N=2,507 % N=606 % χ 2 φ
Sex .029
 Male 1,155 46.1 281 46.4
 Female 1,352 53.9 325 53.6
Age 413.620** .41
 14–40 years 862 34.4 93 15.3
 41–60 years 867 34.6 136 22.4
 61–75 years 598 23.9 256 42.2
 Older than 75 years 180 7.2 121 20.0
Partnership status 201.338** .28
 Married/living together 1,375 54.8 312 51.5
 Married/not living together 31 1.2 8 1.3
 Single 551 22.0 67 11.1
 Divorced 234 9.3 48 7.9
 Widowed 316 12.6 171 28.2
School graduation 70.871** .17
 Without graduation/pupil 119 4.7 24 4.0
 Secondary graduation 2,055 82.0 500 82.5
 A-form 158 6.3 32 5.3
 University/technical college 175 7.0 50 8.3
**

p≤.01.

Instruments

Corresponding to the trauma list of the PTSD module of the Munich Composite International Diagnostic Interview (M-CIDI) (Wittchen & Pfister, 1997), eight potential traumatizing events were given (e.g., “you were the victim of rape”; “… of a natural disaster”; “… you received serious bodily threats (as with a weapon), were attacked, injured, or tortured”; “… you had horrible experiences during war service”), and an open question about “another terrible event or catastrophe”. Additionally, an inquiry was made concerning three war-related events (“You were bombed out”; “You were driven out of your homeland”; “You had awful experiences during war effort”). Participants had to answer the trauma items with “yes” or “no”.

Modified PTSD Symptom Scale

PTSD according to DSM-IV was assessed with the German modified and shortened version of the Posttraumatic Symptom Scale (PSS) by Foa and colleagues (Breslau, Peterson, Kessler, & Schultz, 1999; Foa, Riggs, Dancu, & Rothbaum, 1993; Maercker et al., 2008; Stieglitz, Frommberger, Foa, & Berger, 2001), analogue to Maercker et al. (2008). The answers refer to the occurrence of symptoms in the last month on a four-point scale from 0 (“not at all”), 1 (“once a week or more seldom”), 2 (“2–4 times per week/half the time”), to 3 (“several times per week/almost always”). PTSD was determined according to the DSM-IV (with the A1 and A2 criteria, B criteria and at least 4 of 7 symptoms according to Breslau with scale values ≥2 and F criteria) (Maercker et al., 2008).

The PSS by Breslau et al. (1999) has shown satisfactory reliability and validity in both American (Foa et al., 1993) and German studies (Stieglitz et al., 2001). The abbreviated item selection from Breslau et al., which turned out to be the most effective item selection according to receiver operating characteristic analysis, defined positive PTSD cases compared to the complete symptom criteria list with 80% sensitivity, a specificity of 97%, a positive predictive value of 71%, and a negative predictive value of 98% (Breslau et al., 1999). The German version of the PSS shows similar psychometric properties to those reported by Foa et al. (1993). In our study, the modified scale shows very good internal consistency (Cronbach's α=.93). The impairment criterion was excluded from PTSD symptom score calculations because of its single-item character.

Toronto Alexithymia Scale

The German version (Franz, Schneider, & Schafer, 2001; Franz, Schneider, Schafer, Schmitz, & Zweyer, 2001; Popp et al., 2008) of Bagby's 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) (Bagby et al., 1994a, b) was used to assess alexithymia. The scale's subscales measure three dimensions of the construct of alexithymia: “difficulty identifying feelings”, “difficulty describing feelings”, and “external-oriented thinking.” Each of the 20 items has to be answered on a five-point likert-scale (“not at all” to “fully applicable”). The TAS-20 sum score is calculated by adding up all item scores. “High alexithymics” were defined as the subgroup above the 90th percentile of the distribution, according to Franz et al. (2008). Both sum scores and cut-off scores were used in the computations. In our study, the TAS-20 showed good internal consistency (Cronbach's α=.80).

Statistical analyses

The difference in alexithymia sum scores between the categories of traumatic experiences was assessed with univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) while post-hoc p-values were Bonferroni adjusted. Sociodemographic differences between the full and subsample as well as sex and age effects on alexithymia and PTSD symptomatology were tested with χ 2—tests and univariate ANOVA. The postulated mediating effect of PTSD symptomatology within the association of traumatic experiences and alexithymia was examined by two mediation analyses. The hypotheses were analyzed stepwise with regression analyses implemented by macro for SPSS written by Hayes (2012). The calculations were run with bootstrapped estimates (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The analyses included a Sobel test to test if the indirect effects of the mediating variables are significant (Sobel, 1986). Sex and age were included as possible confounders. The analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics version 20 for Windows.

Results

Lifetime prevalence of trauma by sex and age

Table 2 gives an overview of the prevalence of the traumatic experiences in the total sample. Twenty-four percent of the sample reported at least one traumatic experience, 14.4% reported at least one war-related trauma, and 10.9% reported at least one civilian traumatic experience.

Table 2.

Traumatic experiences (N=2,507)

n %
War related (at least one) 360 14.4
 War effort/combat action 206 8.2
 Bombing 177 7.1
 Displacement/eviction 171 6.8
Civilian (at least one) 273 10.9
 Rape 18 0.7
 Childhood abuse 29 1.2
 Prisoner/hostage 40 1.6
 Serious accident 112 4.5
 Physical violence 98 3.9
 Life-threatening illness 73 2.9
 Natural catastrophe 20 0.8
 Witness of trauma 214 8.5
 Other 90 3.6
Number of traumatic experiences (total: M=0.5; SD=1.114)
 0 1,847 75.8
 1 287 11.8
 2–4 259 10.6
 5–8 44 1.8
 total 2,437 100

Alexithymia, PTSD, and posttraumatic symptomatology

Figure 2 illustrates the mean alexithymia sum scores of the four categories of traumatization as well as test statistics of univariate ANOVA. We found a significant difference in alexithymia manifestation between the trauma categories (F(3, 2433)=7.388; p≤.001; η 2=.01). Alexithymia sum scores were significantly lower for those participants with none to four traumatic experiences (M=49.1; SD=9.175) as compared to those with five to eight traumatic experiences (M =55.4; SD=9.834) (F(1, 2435)=20.720; p≤.001; η 2=.01). Pairwise comparisons indicated that there was no significant difference between any groups with none (post-hoc mean difference: −5.845; p≤.001), one (post-hoc mean difference: −6.089; p≤.001), or two to four traumatic experiences (post-hoc mean difference: 6.089; p≤.001).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Alexithymia manifestation according to the number of traumatic experiences.

Table 3 shows the prevalence of PTSD and alexithymia by sex and age as well as means of posttraumatic symptomatology and alexithymia. A total of 10.6% of our sample was identified as “high alexithymic” (threshold=^90th percentile/threshold ≥61). There were no sex differences in the TAS-20 sum scores nor in the proportion of individuals categorized as “high” alexithymics.

Table 3.

PTSD and alexithymia by sex and age

Distribution by sex Distribution by age

Total Women Men 14–40 years 41–60 years 61–75 years >75 years


N (%) N (%) N (%) χ 2 N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) χ 2 ϕ
Alexithymia N=2,437 N=1,309 N=1,128 N=834 N=846 N=582 N=175
 Cut off at 90th 258 (10.6) 127 (9.7) 131 (11.6) 2.338 106 (12.7) 77 (9.1) 61 (10.5) 14 (8.0) 7.186 .05
 Percentile/threshold ≥61a
PTSDb (in the entire N=2,448 N=1,316 N=1,132 N=850 N=848 N=580 N=170
 sample) 59 (2.4) 36 (2.7) 23 (2.0) 1.222 12 (1.4) 19 (2.2) 18 (3.1) 10 (5.7) 12.719** .07
PTSD (in those N=606 N=325 N=281 N=93 N=136 N=256 N=121
 participants with trauma exposure) 59 (9.7) 36 (11.1) 23 (8.2) .0290 12 (13.0) 19 (14.0) 18 (7.0) 10 (8.3) 413.620** .10
M SD M SD M SD F M SD M SD M SD M SD F η 2
PTSD symptomatology
 Reexperiencing 2.07 0.98 2.07 0.98 2.06 0.97 .013 2.09 0.92 2.41 1.10 1.93 0.94 1.91 0.86 6.635** .04
 Avoidance/numbing 1.68 0.78 1.72 0.82 1.62 0.74 1.877 1.66 0.78 1.85 0.83 1.63 0.75 1.59 0.78 2.404 .02
 Hyperarousal 1.84 0.93 1.90 0.97 1.77 0.87 2.424 1.84 0.98 2.05 0.99 1.74 0.89 1.80 0.87 2.619* .02
 Total 1.86 0.90 1.90 0.92 1.82 0.86 3.632 1.86 0.89 2.10 0.97 1.77 0.86 1.77 0.84 4.431** .03
TAS-20 sum score 49.16 9.22 48.73 9.20 49.67 9.23 6.352 49.55 9.57 48.94 9.18 49.07 9.17 48.67 7.90 .850 .00
a

According to Franz et al. (2008).

b

According to Maercker et al. (2008).

**

p≤.01

*

p≤.05.

A total of 2.4% of the sample fit the criteria for full PTSD. Within the subsample (n=606), the prevalence of PTSD was approximately 10%. There was no sex difference in rates of PTSD (Table 3). The prevalence of PTSD increased significantly across the age groups from 1.4% (14–40 years) up to 5.7% (over 75 years). When considering only participants who mentioned at least one traumatic experience, a reverse effect can be observed (14–40 years: 13.0%; over 75 years: 8.3%) (see Table 3).

Considering the means of posttraumatic symptomatology, there are significant differences between the age groups in the total score as well as in the symptom clusters “reexperiencing” and “hyperarousal”.

Mediation analyses: the association of traumatic experiences, posttraumatic symptomatology and alexithymia

Table 4 shows the results of the mediation analyses predicting alexithymia. To test mediating effects of posttraumatic symptomatology on this association, two mediation analyses were conducted (Model A with the mediator PTSD symptom score; Model B with the mediators re-experience, avoidance/numbing, and hyperarousal) (see Fig. 1).

Table 4.

Testing mediation effects using linear regression

Model A (N=468)a Model B (N=468)a
Testing step 1 (total effect/path c) b β (SE) R 2 Testing step 1 (total effect/path c) b β (SE) R 2
 Outcome Alexithymia sum score  Outcome Alexithymia sum score
 Confounder Sex 1.111 .060 (.0473)  Confounder Sex 1.111 .060 (.0473)
Age −.049 −.094 (.0513) Age −.049 −.94 (.513)
 Predictor Number of traumatic experiences 1.110*** .134*** (.0388) .029  Predictor Number of traumatic experiences 1.110*** .134*** (.0388) .029
Testing step 2 (direct effect/path a) Testing step 2.1 (direct effect/path a1)
 Outcome PTSD sum score  Outcome Re-experience
 Confounder Sex .082 .050 (.046)  Confounder Sex .010 .005 (.0458)
Age −.007* −.152* (.050) Age −.010*** −.175*** (.0497)
 Predictor Number of traumatic experiences .070** .095** (.038) .026  Predictor Number of traumatic experiences .085** .097** (.0376) .030
Testing step 2.2 (direct effect/path a2)
 Outcome Avoidance/numbing
 Confounder Sex .105 .067 (.0463)
Age −.005* −.24* (.0502)
 Predictor Number of traumatic experiences .069** .098** (.0379) .024
Testing step 2.3 (direct effect/path a3)
 Outcome Hyperarousal
 Confounder Sex .130 .070 (.0462)
Age −.006* −.115* (.0502)
 Predictor Number of traumatic experiences .057 .068 (.0379) .018
Testing step 3 (direct, indirect effect/paths b and c′) Testing step 3 (direct, indirect effect/paths b and c′)
 Outcome Alexithymia sum score  Outcome Alexithymia sum score
 Confounder Sex .775 .042 (.044)  Confounder Sex .666 .036 (.442)
Age −.0198 −.038 (.049) Age −.022 −.043 (.0484)
 Mediator PTSD sum score 4.124*** .099*** (.036)  Mediator Re-experience .436 .046 (.0740)
 Predictor Number of traumatic experiences .820** b .099** (.037) .153 Avoidance/numbing 3.827** .325*** (.0778)
Hyperarousal .294 .030 (.0742)
 Predictor Number of traumatic experiences .793** b .096** b (.0363) .166
a

Multiple Linear Regression Analysis with bootstrapped estimate (1,000 samples): subsample with trauma exposure, as required for reporting posttraumatic symptomatology.

b

Significant indirect effect (reduction in regression coefficient b) (Sobel test);

***

p≤.001

**

p≤.01

*

p≤.05.

Because the total effect was significant (path c), the requirement for mediation step 1 was met. In step 2 (Model A), the relationship between the predictor and the mediator was tested. Because the regression coefficient b was significant, the condition for step 2 was met. The coefficient related to alexithymia and posttraumatic symptomatology (path b) was significant when controlling for the number of traumatic experiences (step 3). Step 3 also provides an estimate of path c′, the relation between the number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia, controlling for posttraumatic symptomatology. The coefficient of this relation was significant and significantly smaller than for path c, which was tested by a Sobel test. For Model A, there is an explained variance of 15% (see Hypothesis 2).

Model B includes the three symptom clusters of PTSD as mediators (re-experience, avoidance/numbing, hyperarousal). The same mediation steps were tested for this model as for Model A. Path c (step 1) is identical. That is why it has the same coefficient and was significant. The relations in path a were tested for each mediator (step 2j). Path a1 and path a2 were significant, proving the direct effect of the predictor (number of traumatic experiences) on both mediators. The requirements for mediation in step 23 (path a3) were not met. A significant indirect effect was only shown for the mediator avoidance/numbing in step 3 (path b). The increase of the predictor's b coefficient (path c′) compared to the total effect (path c) was significant, as proven by a Sobel test. The posttraumatic symptom cluster avoidance/numbing remains as the only significant mediator in this analysis. Model B includes an explained variance of 17% (see Hypothesis 3).

Possible confounder variables (sex and age) were added to both models, whereas only age was significant in step 2 (Model A) and step 21–3 (Model B). Increased age is associated with a lower total posttraumatic symptomatology or fewer reexperiencing, avoidance/numbing and hyperarousal symptoms, whereas the effect of the number of traumatic experiences remains significant in each analysis.

Discussion

The present study examines the association between number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia within a large-scale representative sample of the German general population. A partially mediating effect of posttraumatic symptomatology, and the meaning of the PTSD symptom cluster avoidance/numbing in the context of alexithymia is demonstrated with two four-step mediation analyses.

Almost one-fourth of the sample reported having experienced at least one traumatic event in their lifetime. While this prevalence is much lower than those reported in studies from the United States (Breslau, 2009), it is in the lower range of the available findings from Germany (Hauffa et al., 2011; Spitzer et al., 2008). The most frequent traumatic experience reported was “war effort/combat action”, underlining the importance of traumatic experiences from World War II. The least frequent traumatic experience reported was “rape”, whereas we assume that this low prevalence results partly from a reporting bias due to taboo. A similarly low prevalence applies to PTSD, which is lower than reported in studies from the United States (Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, & Nelson, 1995) but in line with other population-based studies from Germany (Spitzer et al., 2008). Of the subgroup of people who had experienced at least one traumatic experience in their lifetime, the prevalence of PTSD is lower in the German elderly than in younger age groups. This finding might be attributable to a selection effect, insofar as most of the traumatic experiences go back to World War II. Because traumatic experiences are related to limited physical health and mortality (Glaesmer, Brähler, Gundel, & Riedel-Heller, 2011), many of the most severely traumatized people in older age brackets have either already died, were unable to participate, or live in nursing homes, etc. This found age effects for posttraumatic symptomatology did not appear for alexithymia.

Consistent with previous studies, our analyses indicate that alexithymia is associated with the number of traumatic experiences a person has undergone (Badura, 2003; Declercq et al., 2010; Frewen et al., 2008a; Fukunishi et al., 1996; Sondergaard & Theorell, 2004; Spitzer et al., 2007; Yehuda et al., 1997). Because of the high rates of alexithymia among those participants who had been repeatedly traumatized, the concept of multiple and complex traumatization as associated with alexithymia is supported by the analyses (see Hypothesis 1). The association between the number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia still remains, when the mediating effect of posttraumatic symptomatology is considered (see Hypothesis 2). Additionally, the importance of the symptom cluster avoidance/numbing was shown (see Hypothesis 3). This is in line with findings from several other studies (Badura, 2003; Declercq et al., 2010; Fukunishi et al., 1996), which have reported an association or a lack of independence between the PTSD symptom numbing and alexithymia.

When analyzing possible confounder variables, only age remains a significant negative predictor for posttraumatic symptomatology. This result is congruent with the earlier reported negative relation between PTSD prevalence and age across the lifespan and might demonstrate the discussed selection bias.

Given the results from this and other studies that alexithymia might be a consequence of multiple or complex traumatization (Frewen et al., 2008a; Sondergaard & Theorell, 2004; Zeitlin, Mcnally, & Cassiday, 1993) we consider that alexithymia might be then theoretically linked to another consequence of complex traumatization, complex PTSD or DESNOS (disorder of extreme stress, not otherwise specified (Ford, 1999; Van der Kolk & Courtois, 2005). Complex PTSD is proposed for listing in the upcoming WHO International Classification System of Diseases (ICD-11) as an independent clinical picture within the spectrum of trauma and stress-related disorders. We assume that complex PTSD symptomatology might be closer to alexithymia than simple PTSD symptomatology because of preceding multiple and/or long-term traumatization (Maercker, 2003) and the characterizing affect dysregulation (McLean, Toner, Jackson, Desrocher, & Stuckless, 2006). Cloitre and colleagues characterized complex PTSD as high in PTSD symptoms, as well as in affective and interpersonal problems and a negative self-concept (Cloitre, Garvert, Brewin, Bryant, & Maercker, 2013). Furthermore, people with symptoms of complex PTSD show a greater long-term functional impairment than those with simple PTSD (Cloitre et al., 2013). In this context, our findings underscore the necessity of future prospective research, which should focus on the type and extent of traumatization experienced by alexithymic or non-alexithymic people, with the aim of describing how alexithymia and traumatization are linked. If future longitudinal studies show alexithymia more clearly as a consequence of traumatization or multiple traumatization, specific trauma-integrative therapeutic approaches for individual settings should be developed.

Although our study has strengths such as a large sample size and a population-based approach, some factors limit the interpretation and generalizability of our findings. As mentioned in the introduction, the cross-sectional design of the study does not allow us to draw directional causal inferences concerning the association between posttraumatic symptomatology and alexithymia. Moreover, alexithymia as well as PTSD symptomatology were measured by self-report instruments. Consequentially, the validity of the results is reduced.

Traumatic experiences were assessed with the trauma list of the M-CIDI, which might be too restrictive and possibly lead to an underestimation of the prevalence of traumatic experiences. The divergence between the prevalence of traumatic experiences in different studies is at least partly attributable to different methodological approaches (e.g., type and number of traumatic experiences under study, open questions vs. list of traumatic experiences), and cultural and historical characteristics. Furthermore, the reported number of traumatic experiences does not allow conclusions about the relationships between types of traumatization and alexithymia. Future research should focus on specific types of traumatic experiences to explore their associations with alexithymia in more detail.

Conclusions

Our study constitutes the first international population-based evidence for the mediating effect of posttraumatic symptomatology on the association between number of traumatic experiences and alexithymia. The strong association between alexithymia and posttraumatic symptomatology highlights the lack of conceptual and diagnostic consideration of alexithymia as it relates to PTSD (Badura, 2003) and complex PTSD formulations.

Acknowledgements

We thank the German Research Foundation (DFG) and Leipzig University within the program of Open Access Publishing for their support. We are grateful to Amber McPherson and Mark Martin, who did proofreading and linguistic revision of the manuscript as native English speaker and Lena Spangenberg for her helpful proofreading.

For the abstract or full text in other languages, please see Supplementary files under Article Tools online

Conflict of interest and funding

There is no conflict of interest in the present study for any of the authors.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (4th ed., text revision) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. Badura A. S. Theoretical and empirical exploration of the similarities between emotional numbing in posttraumatic stress disorder and alexithymia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2003;17(3):349–360. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00201-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bagby R. M, Parker J. D. A, Taylor G. J. The 20-Item Toronto-Alexithymia-Scale—1. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1994a;38(1):23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bagby R. M, Taylor G. J, Parker J. D. A. The 20-Item Toronto-Alexithymia-Scale—2. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1994b;38(1):33–40. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Breslau N. The epidemiology of trauma, PTSD, and other posttrauma disorders. Trauma Violence & Abuse. 2009;10(3):198–210. doi: 10.1177/1524838009334448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Breslau N, Peterson E. L, Kessler R. C, Schultz L. R. Short screening scale for DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156(6):908–911. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.6.908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cloitre M, Garvert D. W, Brewin C. R, Bryant R. A, Maercker A. Evidence for proposed ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD: A latent profile analysis. European Journal of Psychotraumatology. 2013;4 doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.20706. 20706, doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.20706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cloitre M, Scarvalone P, Difede J. Posttraumatic stress disorder, self- and interpersonal dysfunction among sexually retraumatized women. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1997;10(3):437–452. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490100309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Declercq F, Vanheule S, Deheegher J. Alexithymia and posttraumatic stress: Subscales and symptom clusters. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2010;66(10):1076–1089. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Evren C, Dalbudak E, Cetin R, Durkaya M, Evren B. Relationship of alexithymia and temperament and character dimensions with lifetime post-traumatic stress disorder in male alcohol-dependent inpatients. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2010;64(2):111–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.02052.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Foa E. B, Riggs D. S, Dancu C. V, Rothbaum B. O. Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing posttraumatic-stress-disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1993;6(4):459–473. [Google Scholar]
  12. Ford J. D. Disorders of extreme stress following war-zone military trauma: Associated features of posttraumatic stress disorder or comorbid but distinct syndromes? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67(1):3–12. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Franz M, Popp K, Schaefer R, Sitte W, Schneider C, Hardt J, et al. Alexithymia in the German general population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2008;43(1):54–62. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0265-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Franz M, Schneider C, Schafer R. Reliability testing of the German version of TAS-20—Validity and factorial structure. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie. 2001;51(2):90–91. doi: 10.1055/s-2001-10755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Franz M, Schneider C, Schafer R, Schmitz N, Zweyer K. Factorial structure and psychometric properties of the German version of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) of psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie. 2001;51(2):48–55. doi: 10.1055/s-2001-10755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Frazier P. A, Tix A. P, Barron K. E. Testing moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2004;51(1):115–134. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.51.1.115. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Frewen P. A, Dozois D. J, Neufeld R. W, Lanius R. A. Meta-analysis of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2008;21(2):243–246. doi: 10.1002/jts.20320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Frewen P. A, Lanius R. A, Dozois D. J, Neufeld R. W, Pain C, Hopper J. W, et al. Clinical and neural correlates of alexithymia in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008a;117(1):171–181. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fukunishi I, Sasaki K, Chishima Y, Anze M, Saijo M. Emotional disturbances in trauma patients during the rehabilitation phase—Studies of posttraumatic stress disorder and alexithymia. General Hospital Psychiatry. 1996;18(2):121–127. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(95)00121-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Glaesmer H, Brähler E, Gundel H, Riedel-Heller S. G. The association of traumatic experiences and posttraumatic stress disorder with physical morbidity in old age: A German population-based study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2011;73(5):401–406. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31821b47e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gundel H, Ceballos-Baumann A. O, Von Rad M. Psychodynamic and neurobiological influences in the etiology of alexithymia. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie. 2002;52(11):479–486. doi: 10.1055/s-2002-35278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hauffa R, Rief W, Brähler E, Maertin A, Mewes R, Glaesmer H. Lifetime traumatic experiences and posttraumatic stress disorder in the German population results of a representative population survey. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2011;199(12):934–939. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182392c0d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hayes A. F. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling; 2012. Retrieved October 12, 2013, from http://www.afhayes.com. [Google Scholar]
  24. Jorgensen M. M, Ornbol E, Toft T, Olesen F, Zachariae R, Fink P. The association between alexithymia and prevalence of ICD-10 somatoform disorders. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2002;64(1):170. [Google Scholar]
  25. Kessler H, Kammerer M, Hoffmann H, Traue H. C. Regulation of emotions and alexithymia: A correlative study. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie. 2010;60(5):169–174. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1234046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kessler R. C. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kessler R. C, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson C. B. Posttraumatic-stress-disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1995;52(12):1048–1060. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Maercker A, editor. Therapie der Posttraumatischen Belastungsstörungen [Therapy of posttraumatic stress disorder] Berlin: Springer; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  29. Maercker A, Forstmeier S, Wagner B, Glaesmer H, Brähler E. Post-traumatic stress disorder in Germany. Results of a nationwide epidemiological study. Der Nervenarzt. 2008;79(5):577–586. doi: 10.1007/s00115-008-2467-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. McCaslin S. E, Metzler T. J, Best S. R, Liberman A, Weiss D. S, Fagan J, et al. Alexithymia and PTSD symptoms in urban police officers: Cross-sectional and prospective findings. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2006;19(3):361–373. doi: 10.1002/jts.20133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. McLean L. M, Toner B, Jackson J, Desrocher M, Stuckless N. The relationship between childhood sexual abuse, complex post-traumatic stress disorder and alexithymia in two outpatient samples: Examination of women treated in community and institutional clinics. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse. 2006;15(3):1–17. doi: 10.1300/J070v15n03_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Popp K, Schaefer R, Schneider C, Brähler E, Decker O, Hardt J, et al. Factor structure and reliability of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) in the German population. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie. 2008;58(5):208–214. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-986196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Preacher K. J, Hayes A. F. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;40(3):879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rufer M, Moergeli H, Moritz S, Drabe N, Weidt S. Alexithymia and non-treatment: An Internet based study of 312 people with chronic anxiety. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2014;55(1):179–187. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sobel M. E. Some new results on indirect effects and their standard errors in covariance structure models. Sociological Methodology. 1986;16:159–186. [Google Scholar]
  36. Sondergaard H. P, Theorell T. Alexithymia, emotions and PTSD; findings from a longitudinal study of refugees. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;58(3):185–191. doi: 10.1080/08039480410006214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Spitzer C, Barnow S, Volzke H, John U, Freyberger H. J, Grabe H. J. Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the elderly: Findings from a German community study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2008;69(5):693–700. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v69n0501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Spitzer C, Schilling L, John U, Völzke H, Appel K, Schulz A. Alexithymie, traumatischer stress und posttraumatische Belastungsstörung—Befunde aus der Allgemeinbevölkerung [Alexithymia, traumatic stress and posttraumatic stress disorder: findings from the general population] Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie, Psychologie und Psychotherapie. 2013;61(1):27–36. doi: 10.1024/1661-4747/a000137. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Spitzer C, Vogel M, Barnow S, Freyberger H. J, Grabe H. J. Psychopathology and alexithymia in severe mental illness: The impact of trauma and posttraumatic stress symptoms. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2007;257(4):191–196. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-0669-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Stieglitz R. D, Frommberger U, Foa E. B, Berger M. Evaluation of the German version of the PTSD Symptom Scale (PSS) Psychopathology. 2001;34(3):128–133. doi: 10.1159/000049295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Taylor G. J, Bagby R. M. Psychoanalysis and empirical research: The example of alexithymia. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association. 2013;61(1):99–133. doi: 10.1177/0003065112474066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Taylor G. J, Bagby R. M, Parker J. D. A, editors. Disorders of Affect Regulation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  43. Tolmunen T, Heliste M, Lehto S. M, Hintikka J, Honkalampi K, Kauhanen J. Stability of alexithymia in the general population: An 11-year follow-up. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2011;52(5):536–541. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Van Ameringen M, Mancini C, Patterson B, Boyle M. H. Post-traumatic stress disorder in Canada. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics. 2008;14(3):171–181. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2008.00049.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Van der Kolk B. A, Courtois C. A. Editorial comments: Complex developmental trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2005;18(5):385–388. doi: 10.1002/jts.20046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wittchen H.-U, Pfister H, editors. DIA-X-Interviews: Manual für Screeningverfahren und Interviews, Interviewheft [DIA-X-interviews: Manual for screening methods and interviews, interview book] Frannkfurt/Main: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  47. Yehuda R, Steiner A, Kahana B, BinderBrynes K, Southwick S. M, Zemelman S, et al. Alexithymia in Holocaust survivors with and without PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1997;10(1):93–100. doi: 10.1023/a:1024860430725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Zeitlin S. B, Mcnally R. J, Cassiday K. L. Alexithymia in victims of sexual assault—An effect of repeated traumatization. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150(4):661–663. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.4.661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Zlotnick C, Mattia J. I, Zimmerman M. The relationship between posttraumatic stress disorder, childhood trauma and alexithymia in an outpatient sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2001;14(1):177–188. [Google Scholar]

Articles from European Journal of Psychotraumatology are provided here courtesy of Taylor & Francis

RESOURCES