Abstract
Aims
To investigate associations between socio-economic position in early life and later alcohol use and problem use among male and female adolescents.
Design
Birth cohort study.
Setting
South West England.
Participants
A total of 2711 girls and 2379 boys with one or more measures of alcohol use or problem use at age 15 years.
Measurements
Exposure measures were highest parental social class, maternal education and household disposable income (all maternal self-report before school-age); outcome measures were heavy typical drinking, frequent drinking, regular binge drinking, alcohol-related psychosocial problems and alcohol-related behavioural problems.
Findings
Alcohol use and related problems were relatively common amongst adolescent girls and boys. Boys were slightly more likely to report frequent drinking and girls were slightly more likely to drink heavily and to experience alcohol-related psychosocial problems. Higher maternal education appeared protective in relation to alcohol-related problems, particularly among boys. Higher household income was associated with greater risk of alcohol use and problem use, most apparently among girls.
Conclusions
Children from higher-income households in England appear to be at greater risk of some types of adolescent alcohol problems, and these risks appear different in girls compared to boys. Childhood social advantage may not generally be associated with healthier behaviour in adolescence.
Keywords: Alcohol problems, alcohol use, ALSPAC, gender interaction, gender, health inequalities, missing data, social status
Introduction
Alcohol is the most common psychoactive substance used and abused by adolescents world-wide [1–3]. Alcohol use is associated with substantial harms at both the individual and community level [4]. The use of alcohol in childhood and adolescence has been associated with both proximal (e.g. antisocial behaviour, poor academic performance, unintentional injuries) and distal (mental and physical health problems) adverse consequences [5–7]. Adolescent problem alcohol use may also cluster with other adolescent health risk behaviours [7,8]
Problem substance use in adolescence and adulthood shows a relatively consistent general association with childhood disadvantage [9]. A previous systematic review found little consistent evidence to support an association between childhood disadvantage and later use or problem use of alcohol [10]. Although some evidence suggested heavier alcohol use among adolescents from more disadvantaged backgrounds [11], other studies found that less disadvantaged childhood circumstances were associated with greater risk of alcohol use in later life [12,13]. Associations between childhood socio-economic position (SEP) and alcohol use might differ from those between SEP and more problematic use [10].
Childhood social position may influence adolescent substance use through a number of mechanisms. Children from more disadvantaged backgrounds are more likely to have been exposed to a number of early adversities, including parental substance abuse and psychological problems and family dissolution [14]. These exposures may also associate with later problem substance use [8,15]. In particular, childhood externalizing disorders such as conduct problems have been shown to predict later problem substance use [16–18]. Externalizing disorders are also more common among disadvantaged children. However, childhood social advantage may also increase the risk of adolescent substance use, particularly in the case of relatively accessible substances [2,19,20]. Socially advantaged children may be more likely to have the means to purchase alcohol and may be more likely to live in homes where alcohol is available [10]. The particular measure of SEP may also matter regarding the nature of these associations. For example, an inverse relationship between parental education and alcohol use is often assumed, but evidence is weak [10,12,13,21,22]. Evidence on associations between alcohol use and other childhood socio-economic indicators, such as household income or social class, is even more controversial and may suggest a complex picture [10].
Young people tend to consume alcohol for its pleasurable and inebriating effects [2,23,24]. Occurrences of heavy episodic drinking sessions (binge drinking) or drinking to intoxication are common among adolescent drinkers world-wide [2,3,25]. In Britain, too, more positive expectations about alcohol use have been linked to more problem use [2]. Hibell et al. [2] reported that boys aged 15–16 in the United Kingdom consumed on average 6.9 cl of pure alcohol on the latest occasion, and girls of the same age consumed 5.7 cl, exceeding the current recommendations for adults [6]. Although some evidence suggests convergence of male and female drinking patterns [2,25,26], other evidence points to important gender differences [1,2,26,27]. For example, boys in the United States have their first drink earlier than girls, drink more frequently and are more likely to binge drink [1].
In mid-adolescence, girls have been reported to mature earlier and to be exposed to higher contextual influences than boys, which may impact upon substance-related behaviour [15,28–30]. Within and across strata of SEP they may also observe different cultural norms to that of boys [29–31]. Therefore, the association between parental SEP and offspring alcohol use could vary by gender. However, evidence is mixed. Most studies reported SEP–adolescent alcohol associations that appeared consistent across genders [10–12,22,32]. One study tested explicitly for gender differences in the association of alcohol consumption with indicators of paternal/maternal education/occupation and found none [32]. Other studies reported a gender-specific effect of dimensions or constructs of SEP on adolescent alcohol use, with contrasting evidence in their reciprocal direction [19,27]. For example, Richter et al. [19] reported that, across a number of countries, with the exception of one, family affluence was related positively to drunkenness in boys, but not in girls.
A recent systematic review of evidence on these questions [10] noted contradictory findings and further highlighted a general lack of prospective studies where measurement of SEP clearly preceded measurement of alcohol, where different dimensions of SEP were considered, where the distinction was made between problematic and non-problematic alcohol use and where possible gender differences were examined. This review also noted a general lack of evidence related to alcohol use patterns in contemporary adolescents. We used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (a contemporary birth cohort study in South West England) to address these questions. We investigated associations between different dimensions of early childhood SEP and multiple measures of later alcohol use and problem use and whether these associations differed in boys and girls.
Methods
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) recruited 14 541 pregnant women from the former Avon area (southwest UK) in the early 1990s. The mothers and their offspring have been followed-up to date to investigate aspects of their general health and wellbeing, including offspring substance use and detailed socio-demographic data (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/) [20,33,34]. Informed written consent and assent were obtained from the adult participants and the young people. The ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committee approved the study.
Participants
The original birth cohort comprised 13 973 live infants at 1 year of known sex [34]. Those with known addresses and still participating were invited to a clinical assessment at age 15 years (n = 10 040, 71.9%); 5246 (52.3%) eligible subjects attended the clinic. Adolescents with a lower SEP (according to multiple indicators) were less likely to participate (P < 0.001). Girls (2771, 52.8%) were more likely than boys (2475, 47.2%) to participate (P < 0.001) and they were slightly older (P = 0.001). A total of 5090 subjects (2711 girls, 2379 boys, response rate: 50.7%) reported some alcohol consumption at age 15. Given the complexity and longevity of the overall ALSPAC project, the attrition rate and its implications have been described comprehensively elsewhere [20,34].
SEP
All SEP indicators were obtained from self-administered questionnaires to the mother. Based on the Registrar General's classification of occupations [35], parental social class was defined as the highest rank between parents (mainly the father's occupation) and grouped into four categories: V (unskilled) or IV (semi-skilled manual), III (skilled manual or non-manual), II (managerial and technical) and I (professional), and used as an indicator of social capital [14]. The highest maternal educational attainment was categorized as ‘less than O-level’, ‘O-level’, ‘A-level’ and ‘university degree’ as a measure of human capital. Both social class and education were collected in pregnancy. A measure of household disposable income was averaged over two observations at ages 3 and 4 years of the index child and split into quintiles to denote material capital [14]. The measure had been rescaled preliminarily to account for family size and composition, estimated housing benefits and was expressed in 1995 prices for comparability across families [20]. Father's education and maternal social class, as reported by the mother, were also considered for inclusion. Due to relatively higher proportions of missing data, good correlations with the variables included and either consistent or weaker findings in preliminary investigations, they were excluded from further analyses. All SEP indicators included were correlated moderately, as reported previously [20].
Alcohol measures
Adolescents reported detailed information on their alcohol consumption via a computer at age 15 years. We first derived a variable for early alcohol onset (≤11 years) to measure pre-adolescence alcohol use, as both an intermediate outcome and a marker of later problem use [15,16,36]; the earliest available information from six surveys from age 10 to 16 years was used in hierarchical order to minimize recall bias and maximize the sample for the analyses. For the main outcome variables we focused on data from the clinic at 15 years, as this was after the peak incidence of alcohol initiation; data were adequate to derive multiple phenotypes of alcohol use and response rate was relatively high. The following binary variables for alcohol use were defined: heavy typical drinking (more than four drinks per occasion in the previous 6 months), which constitutes sufficient level for intoxication in this age group for both genders [37]; frequent drinking (≥20 times in the previous 6 months), which is akin to weekly drinking compared to other studies [2,19,25]; and regular binge drinking (consuming more than five drinks in any 24-hour period in the previous 2 years on ≥20 occasions), which adapts a common definition for binge drinking [15,21]. If participants reported any binge drinking in the past 2 years, but failed to report on the number of times they did so, they would be classified as ‘no’ (n = 537). Two additional indicators defined alcohol problem use. Alcohol psychosocial problems experienced in the previous 2 years on more than three occasions (to exclude chance occurrences) included any of the following eight items (yes/no): ‘set a limit, drank more’; ‘felt should stop/cut back on drinking’; ‘spent a great deal of day drinking’; ‘not done things because of drinking’; ‘continued to drink despite causing problems’; ‘unable to keep up with other activities’; ‘parents/friends complained’; and ‘had a “blackout” because of drinking’. Alcohol behavioural problems, defined similarly, included any of four dichotomous items: ‘used alcohol in dangerous situations’; ‘been accidentally physically hurt while drinking’; ‘had a problem with the police’ and ‘got into fights because of drinking’. These latter measures are also comparable with other constructs and with criteria for alcohol dependence or harmful use in adults [2,3,38,39].
Additional exposures
Maternal smoking in early childhood: maximum number of cigarettes per day smoked between the child's birth and 4 years (none, ≤20, >20), as reported across five surveys.
Maternal drinking in early childhood: at the same surveys above, mothers were classified as ‘non-drinking’ (0), ‘drinking less than daily’ (1) or ‘drinking daily’ (2). A total score was derived and categorized subsequently as 0–1, 2–4, 5, 6+ to minimize misclassification for non-response and due to small numbers in extreme cells.
Paternal smoking in pregnancy: father's smoking was based on both maternal and own reports across two surveys in pregnancy. If they reported any form of tobacco use or smoking on more than one occasion per day at either survey they were classified as smokers.
Father's absence: measure derived from multiple self-reports asked of the mother about the biological father's presence/absence in the household between birth and age 10 of the child. The father was classified ultimately as either ‘ever absent’ or ‘present’ between birth and 5 years, as a period of particular susceptibility [40].
Conduct problems: both asked of the mother and the teacher at child's age 11. The highest score reported based on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [41] was then ordered as 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5+ due to small counts in extreme cells.
Alcohol onset: as defined previously; this was included as an exposure of alcohol use at 15.
All these childhood exposures were considered as they have been reported previously to predict alcohol use in adolescence and to be associated with SEP.
Statistical analysis
Patterns of drinking are reported by gender. Multivariable logistic regressions were fitted to each alcohol measure at age 15 and included all three indicators of SEP, child's gender and actual age at reporting as explanatory variables. Age was included, as girls were slightly older than boys due to differential response patterns. All SEP indicators entered the analysis as linear terms, as there was no strong evidence of any non-linear relationship. In a second step, interaction terms between each SEP indicator and child's gender were then tested for each outcome. If a likelihood ratio test had a P < 0.05, an interaction term was included and retained in further analyses. Because early SEP may influence early alcohol onset, which can then determine adolescent alcohol use at 15 and possibly conceal other parenting variables associations [16,17,36], each model was adjusted further for all remaining exposures (step 3) and separately for alcohol onset (step 4). Alcohol onset was also analysed as an outcome to aid interpretation of the results.
Missing data
To assess the potential for selection bias we also ran sensitivity analyses after multiple imputations of missing data on the original cohort (n = 13 973). Covariates and outcomes were imputed iteratively by chained equations [42–44], which assume that data are missing at random, given the observed characteristics of the individuals included in the imputation model. We entered 67 variables overall into the model, which included the measures of interest and auxiliary data to predict both their missingness and the missing values [44]. Conditional imputations were stratified by gender to allow for any interaction with other variables [44]. One hundred imputations of 10 cycles each were run through the command ice in Stata version 11.2 [42]. Parameter estimates were then summarized according to Rubin's rules through the command mim [42]. This method has been described and applied similarly elsewhere [20,44]. We restricted the analysis of the associations of interest only to subjects with one or more measures of alcohol available (n = 7921), including early alcohol onset, as outcome imputations across cycles out with this sample were unstable [44].
Results
Patterns of alcohol use
A greater proportion of boys reported early-onset drinking than girls (Table 1). Boys and girls had similar patterns of alcohol use at age 15 (Table 1). However, a greater proportion of girls reported heavy typical drinking, whereas boys were more likely to report frequent drinking. Approximately 10% of boys and girls reported binge drinking regularly. A higher proportion of girls (nearly 28 versus 24% in boys) reported alcohol-related psychosocial problems. The prevalence of alcohol-related behavioural problems was similar to that of binge drinking and similar in boys and girls.
Table 1.
Boys | Girls | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Alcohol measure | na | % | na | % | P |
Early drinkingb | 3757 | 17.4 | 4042 | 15.2 | 0.009 |
Heavy typical drinking | 2289 | 19.7 | 2611 | 22.6 | 0.012 |
Frequent drinking | 2359 | 20.6 | 2691 | 18.1 | 0.024 |
Regular binge drinking | 2371 | 10.6 | 2696 | 10.1 | 0.50 |
Alcohol psychosocial problems | 2336 | 23.8 | 2678 | 27.8 | 0.001 |
Alcohol behavioural problems | 2349 | 10.0 | 2695 | 9.1 | 0.27 |
n represents the total number of respondents for each measure; n may vary due to missing data. From these numbers, row percentages are calculated by gender.
The total observations for this measure are noticeably larger than for other measures, as early drinking onset is derived from multiple surveys of age 10 to 16. P-values are reported by Pearson's χ2 statistics.
Early drinking onset and SEP
Higher maternal education was associated negatively with early drinking onset (Table 2). Risk of early drinking onset did not appear to be related independently to either disposable income or occupational class.
Table 2.
Early drinking onset | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) | (b) | (c) | ||||
(n = 6170) | (n = 5512) | (n = 7921) | ||||
SEP indicatora | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P |
Social class | 1.00 (0.90–1.11) | 0.997 | 1.06 (0.94–1.18) | 0.353 | 1.04 (0.94–1.16) | 0.412 |
Maternal education | 0.91 (0.84–0.99) | 0.029 | 0.91 (0.83–1.00) | 0.046 | 0.90 (0.83–0.98) | 0.012 |
Disposable income | 1.01 (0.95–1.06) | 0.820 | 1.01 (0.95–1.08) | 0.677 | 1.04 (0.98–1.10) | 0.178 |
Girl | 0.88 (0.77–1.01) | 0.062 | 0.94 (0.81–1.09) | 0.399 | 0.88 (0.78–1.00) | 0.044 |
Each socio-economic position (SEP) indicator is represented by a linear term. The reference category is the least advantaged socio-economic position and the odds ratio represents the independent linear effect of a unit increase in the levels of each indicator. (a) also adjusted by gender [odds ratios (ORs) shown]; (b) model (a), also adjusted for multiple exposures; (c) model (b), after multiple imputation by chained equations of missing data. n represents the number of subjects in each analysis; ns may vary due to missing data. 95% CI: 95% confidence interval. P-values are reported by the Wald test for each term in the model.
Alcohol use and SEP
Heavy typical drinking and regular binge drinking were both associated negatively with maternal education (Table 3). However, these associations were attenuated substantially in adjusted models. In contrast, there was evidence for both frequent drinking and regular binge drinking being associated positively with family disposable income, particularly after adjustment for other exposures and after multiple imputation. No interaction of gender with SEP was detected. Associations of gender with measures of alcohol use were generally consistent with those seen in Table 1.
Table 3.
Heavy typical drinking | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) | (b) | (c) | (d) | |||||
(n = 4039) | (n = 3708) | (n = 3708) | (n = 7921) | |||||
SEP Indicatora | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P |
Social class | 0.90 (0.80–1.01) | 0.083 | 0.91 (0.80–1.04) | 0.170 | 0.89 (0.78–1.02) | 0.084 | 0.95 (0.85–1.07) | 0.408 |
Maternal education | 0.91 (0.83–1.00) | 0.053 | 0.96 (0.87–1.06) | 0.454 | 0.98 (0.88–1.09) | 0.684 | 0.94 (0.86–1.02) | 0.113 |
Disposable income | 0.99 (0.93–1.05) | 0.704 | 1.01 (0.94–1.08) | 0.886 | 1.01 (0.94–1.08) | 0.870 | 1.01 (0.95–1.07) | 0.732 |
Girl | 1.20 (1.03–1.40) | 0.023 | 1.20 (1.02–1.42) | 0.027 | 1.22 (1.03–1.44) | 0.020 | 1.13 (0.98–1.30) | 0.099 |
Frequent drinking | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 4151) | (n = 3807) | (n = 3807) | (n = 7921) | |||||
Social class | 0.97 (0.86–1.10) | 0.636 | 0.97 (0.85–1.10) | 0.622 | 0.93 (0.82–1.07) | 0.328 | 1.03 (0.91–1.16) | 0.654 |
Maternal education | 0.97 (0.88–1.07) | 0.531 | 0.98 (0.88–1.08) | 0.654 | 1.00 (0.90–1.11) | 0.991 | 0.99 (0.90–1.09) | 0.820 |
Disposable income | 1.08 (1.01–1.16) | 0.019 | 1.09 (1.02–1.17) | 0.015 | 1.10 (1.02–1.19) | 0.010 | 1.08 (1.02–1.15) | 0.015 |
Girl | 0.84 (0.72–0.98) | 0.027 | 0.82 (0.70–0.97) | 0.021 | 0.82 (0.69–0.98) | 0.025 | 0.81 (0.71–0.94) | 0.004 |
Regular binge drinking | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 4162) | (n = 3817) | (n = 3817) | (n = 7921) | |||||
Social class | 0.99 (0.84–1.15) | 0.852 | 1.02 (0.86–1.21) | 0.826 | 0.99 (0.83–1.18) | 0.944 | 1.04 (0.89–1.20) | 0.636 |
Maternal education | 0.86 (0.76–0.97) | 0.016 | 0.92 (0.80–1.05) | 0.203 | 0.93 (0.81–1.07) | 0.336 | 0.91 (0.81–1.02) | 0.100 |
Disposable income | 1.06 (0.97–1.15) | 0.198 | 1.11 (1.01–1.22) | 0.027 | 1.12 (1.02–1.23) | 0.022 | 1.11 (1.02–1.20) | 0.019 |
Girl | 0.93 (0.76–1.14) | 0.503 | 0.94 (0.76–1.17) | 0.583 | 0.94 (0.75–1.18) | 0.596 | 0.88 (0.73–1.06) | 0.193 |
Each socio-economic position (SEP) indicator is represented by a linear term. The reference category is the least advantaged socio-economic position and the odds ratio (OR) represents the independent linear effect of a unit increase in the levels of each indicator. (a) Also adjusted by gender (ORs shown) and age at reporting; (b) model (a), also adjusted for multiple exposures; (c) model (b), also adjusted by early drinking onset; (d) model (c), after multiple imputation by chained equations of missing data. n represents the number of subjects in each analysis; ns may vary due to missing data. 95% CI: 95% confidence interval. P-values are reported by the Wald test for each term in the model.
Alcohol-related problems, SEP and gender
There was some evidence of an interaction between gender and dimensions of SEP for measures of alcohol-related problems in adolescence (Table 4 and Fig. 1). Among girls, increasing household income was associated with increasing risk of alcohol-related psychosocial problems, whereas an opposite and weaker association was apparent among boys (Fig. 1a). For example, when stratifying the marginal effects by levels of income and gender, girls coming from the top quintile of household income had their average odds of alcohol psychosocial problems 1.7 [odds ratio (OR) 1.68, 95% confidence interval (CI): 1.33–2.13] times higher than boys with comparable familial income. Conversely, among boys there was an average reduction of 0.029 (95% CI: 0.009–0.050) in the odds of alcohol-related behavioural problems for each additional level of maternal education, with no association apparent in girls (Fig. 1b). Fully adjusted and sensitivity analyses after multiple imputation showed similar patterns, although a few associations, particularly with social class and maternal education, were attenuated.
Table 4.
Alcohol psychosocial problems | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) | (b) | (c) | (d) | (e) | ||||||
(n = 4120) | (n = 4120) | (n = 3780) | (n = 3780) | (n = 7921) | ||||||
SEP indicatora | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P |
Social class | 0.84 (0.75–0.93) | 0.002 | 0.84 (0.75–0.94) | 0.002 | 0.86 (0.77–0.97) | 0.017 | 0.84 (0.74–0.95) | 0.005 | 0.87 (0.78–0.97) | 0.010 |
Maternal education | 0.86 (0.79–0.94) | 0.001 | 0.86 (0.79–0.94) | 0.001 | 0.89 (0.81–0.98) | 0.021 | 0.90 (0.82–1.00) | 0.044 | 0.88 (0.81–0.96) | 0.003 |
Disposable income | 1.07 (1.01–1.13) | 0.030 | 0.96 (0.88–1.04) | 0.296 | 0.98 (0.90–1.07) | 0.667 | 0.97 (0.89–1.07) | 0.582 | 1.00 (0.93–1.09) | 0.931 |
Girl × disposable income | – | – | 1.22 (1.10–1.36) | <0.001 | 1.23 (1.10–1.38) | <0.001 | 1.26 (1.12–1.41) | <0.001 | 1.20 (1.10–1.32) | <0.001 |
Girl | 1.18 (1.02–1.36) | 0.023 | 0.75 (0.57–0.99) | 0.042 | 0.74 (0.55–1.01) | 0.055 | 0.71 (0.52–0.97) | 0.033 | 0.78 (0.61–0.99) | 0.040 |
Alcohol behavioural problems | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 4144) | (n = 4144) | (n = 3802) | (n = 3802) | (n = 7921) | ||||||
Social class | 0.84 (0.71–0.98) | 0.031 | 0.83 (0.71–0.98) | 0.029 | 0.86 (0.72–1.02) | 0.090 | 0.82 (0.69–0.99) | 0.035 | 0.90 (0.77–1.05) | 0.177 |
Maternal education | 0.87 (0.77–0.99) | 0.039 | 0.78 (0.66–0.92) | 0.004 | 0.87 (0.72–1.05) | 0.135 | 0.88 (0.73–1.07) | 0.190 | 0.84 (0.71–0.99) | 0.034 |
Girl × maternal education | – | – | 1.26 (1.01–1.57) | 0.042 | 1.26 (1.00–1.59) | 0.053 | 1.29 (1.02–1.64) | 0.037 | 1.26 (1.03–1.55) | 0.028 |
Disposable income | 0.98 (0.90–1.07) | 0.671 | 0.98 (0.90–1.07) | 0.666 | 1.02 (0.93–1.12) | 0.660 | 1.02 (0.93–1.13) | 0.658 | 1.05 (0.97–1.15) | 0.218 |
Girl | 0.82 (0.66–1.01) | 0.057 | 0.60 (0.41–0.86) | 0.006 | 0.61 (0.41–0.92) | 0.017 | 0.58 (0.38–0.89) | 0.011 | 0.62 (0.45–0.87) | 0.005 |
Each socio-economic position (SEP) indicator is represented by a linear term, including the interaction term of gender with selected SEP indicators. The reference category is the least advantaged socio-economic position and the odds ratio (OR) represents the independent linear effect of a unit increase in the levels of each indicator. When an interaction with gender is present the main effect of SEP is the main effect in boys. (a) Also adjusted by gender (ORs shown) and age at reporting; (b) model (a), including relevant interaction terms; (c) model (b), also adjusted for multiple exposures; (d) model (c), also adjusted by early drinking onset; (e) model (d), after multiple imputation by chained equations of missing data. n represents the number of subjects in each analysis; ns may vary due to missing data. 95% CI: 95% confidence interval. P-values are reported by the Wald test for each term in the model.
Discussion
In a large population-based sample of contemporary adolescents in South West England, both alcohol use and problem use were relatively common. There was also evidence that multiple dimensions of childhood SEP had different associations with alcohol use and problem use in adolescence. These associations also differed by gender. Particularly among boys, higher maternal education was associated with lower risk of alcohol-related behavioural problems, whereas among girls higher household income was associated with greater risk of alcohol-related psychosocial problems. Attenuation of some effects after adjustment for established childhood risk factors for problem alcohol use suggested possible mediation through these factors which would need further investigation. Alternative mechanisms are discussed below.
Findings in the context of previous evidence
Our findings challenge the view that childhood social advantage is associated generally with healthier behaviour in adolescence. This adds to evidence both from ALSPAC [20] and elsewhere that aspects of childhood SEP, particularly higher household income, may be associated positively with certain types of unhealthy behaviour [2,12,19,22]. An association between higher parental education and healthier behaviour in adolescent offspring has been reported previously, although in relation to alcohol use this evidence is mixed [10,12,13,20–22]. Moreover, most previous studies investigated limited domains of both SEP and alcohol use. There has been little prior investigation of differential SEP effects on alcohol use by gender. Some studies have suggested a greater vulnerability to alcohol-related problems among adolescent girls due to earlier maturation and the greater influence of contexts and peers [15,27–30]. We are not aware of previous evidence that this phenomenon may differ by SEP. However, there is some evidence that higher social strata may have a more liberal attitude towards drinking, particularly in girls [17,29,30].
Possible mechanisms
Our study was not intended to investigate mechanisms, and any discussion of this based on our findings is necessarily speculative. Increased risk of alcohol use and related psychosocial problems with greater household income may reflect greater availability in such households or greater ‘purchasing power’ among children from such households. There may also be cultural norms to explain different alcohol behaviours across the socio-economic spectrum. The positive association of alcohol problem use with income in girls could be explained partly by their susceptibility to ethanol [37] and heavier drinkers coming from more affluent families. The current findings might also be a reflection of the ‘emotional climate’ that may operate within families in different social strata. For example, one longitudinal study reported that parental disapproval of alcohol use was particularly protective in boys and also, to a lesser extent, in girls [27]. It is also possible that parental disapproval of alcohol misuse is associated with higher parental education or higher social classes. Weaker inverse relationships between a higher maternal education and alcohol problem use in girls compared to boys could also reflect a reduction in maternal closeness at this time [27].
Strengths and limitations
This study has several strengths. Our general population-based sample of contemporary adolescents was large enough for us to examine effects by gender. Our prospective design minimized the risk of recall bias. We had information on different dimensions of social position and were able to derive multiple measures of alcohol use and problem use. We also had information on key childhood exposures likely to confound or mediate an association between SEP and problem alcohol use. The measurement of our main exposures and covariates clearly preceded that of our alcohol use outcomes, thus minimizing the possibility of reverse causation or recall bias. Our study also had limitations. Alcohol problems were self-reported, thus may have been subject to reporting bias. Despite inclusion of the main socially patterned exposures influencing adolescent alcohol problem use, it is still possible that our associations reflect mechanisms we were not able to consider, such as peer-related factors and parenting practices. Like most prospective studies, ALSPAC experienced attrition which was higher among disadvantaged children. We addressed this issue through sensitivity analyses after multiple imputation of missing data which suggested that attrition had not introduced any substantial bias; nevertheless, we cannot exclude the possibility of such bias. Our main alcohol-related outcome measures were collected at age 15 and based where possible on standard criteria. While this is appropriate in the context of the particular issue of adolescent drinking, it is possible that problem alcohol use phenotypes are still emerging at this age, and that at later ages different patterns of association may be observed. Our findings derive from a UK sample and may not generalize to other settings and drinking cultures.
Policy implications
Despite sparse evidence of effective strategies to curb adolescent drinking [30,45,46], interventions towards targeted groups or individuals have yielded more promising findings [45–48]. Our results may have some implications, particularly for parent-orientated and gender-specific prevention strategies. The increased affordability of alcohol among wealthier families and its availability may enhance opportunities for unhealthy drinking among their offspring (particularly girls). Improving communication with the family and favouring ties, particularly mother–daughter bonds, may help to prevent health-damaging behaviours among adolescents [27,49]. Policies to tackle irresponsible drinking among adolescents should focus not only on those from more deprived backgrounds, but include interventions for the more affluent.
Conclusion
Children from higher-income households were at greater risk of some types of adolescent alcohol problems, and these risks appeared different in girls compared to boys. Policy around reducing problematic alcohol use among adolescents should reflect awareness of these issues, and not be predicated on the notion that alcohol problems are predominantly a feature of social disadvantage.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded specifically by the Economic and Social Research Council UK (ESRC, Grant RES-060-23-0011) to study the Impact of Family Socioeconomic Status on Outcomes in Childhood and Adolescence (Principle Investigator Paul Gregg). We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council (Grant ref: 74882) the Wellcome Trust (Grant ref: 076467) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. We thank Liz Washbrook, Paul Gregg and Carol Propper for the use of their disposable income measure. This publication is the work of the authors and John Macleod will serve as guarantor for the contents of this paper.
Declarations of interest
None.
References
- 1.US Department of Health and Human Services. 2007. The Surgeon General's Call to Action to Prevent and Reduce Underage Drinking. Rockville, MD (US): Department of Health and Human Services, Office of the Surgeon General (US) [DOI] [PubMed]
- 2.Hibell BR, Guttormsson U, Ahlström S, Balakireva O, Bjarnason T, Kokkevi A, et al. The 2007 ESPAD Report: Substance Use Among Students in 35 European Countries. Stockholm, Sweden: The Swedish Council for Information on Alcohol and other Drugs (CAN); 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future National Results on Adolescent Drug Use: Overview of Key Findings, 2008. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Nutt DJ, King LA, Phillips LD. Drug harms in the UK: a multicriteria decision analysis. Lancet. 2010;376:1558–1565. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61462-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR. Age at drinking onset and alcohol dependence—age at onset, duration, and severity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160:739–746. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.160.7.739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chief Medical Officer. Guidance on the Consumption of Alcohol by Children and Young People. London: Department of Health; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Newbury-Birch D, Gilvarry E, McArdle P, Venkateswaran R, Stewart S, Walker J, et al. 2008. Impact of Alcohol Consumption on Young People: A Review of Reviews. Newcastle University: Institute of Health and Society.
- 8.Dodge KA, Malone PS, Lansford JE, Miller S, Pettit GS, Bates JE. A dynamic cascade model of the development of substance-use onset. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2009;74:vii–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00528.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Daniel JZ, Hickman M, Macleod J, Wiles N, Lingford-Hughes A, Farrell M, et al. Is socioeconomic status in early life associated with drug use? A systematic review of the evidence. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2009;28:142–153. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-3362.2008.00042.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wiles NJ, Lingford-Hughes A, Daniel J, Hickman M, Farrell M, Macleod J, et al. Socio-economic status in childhood and later alcohol use: a systematic review. Addiction. 2007;102:1546–1563. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01930.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Droomers M, Schrijvers CTM, Casswell S, Mackenbach JP. Occupational level of the father and alcohol consumption during adolescence; patterns and predictors. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2003;57:704–710. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.9.704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Maggs JL, Patrick ME, Feinstein L. Childhood and adolescent predictors of alcohol use and problems in adolescence and adulthood in the National Child Development Study. Addiction. 2008;103:7–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02173.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Piko BF, Fitzpatrick KM. Socioeconomic status, psychosocial health and health behaviours among Hungarian adolescents. Eur J Public Health. 2007;17:353–360. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckl257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;53:371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Masten AS, Faden VB, Zucker RA, Spear LP. Underage drinking: a developmental framework. Pediatrics. 2008;121:S235–251. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lynskey MT. The prevalence and risk factors associated with abusive or hazardous alcohol consumption in 16-year-olds. Addiction. 1995;90:935–946. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1995.9079356.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Zucker RA. Anticipating problem alcohol use developmentally from childhood into middle adulthood: what have we learned? Addiction. 2008;103:100–108. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02179.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rutter M. Substance use and abuse: causal pathway considerations. In: Rutter M, editor. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 4th edn. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing; 2002. pp. 455–463. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Richter M, Leppin A, Nic Gabhainn S, Hurrelmann K. The impact of socio-economic status on adolescent drinking behaviour. In: Margaretha JR, Robin R, editors. Youth Drinking Cultures European Experiences. Abingdon, UK: Ashgate; 2007. pp. 81–99. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Melotti R, Heron J, Hickman M, Macleod J, Araya R, Lewis G. Adolescent alcohol and tobacco use and early socioeconomic position: the ALSPAC birth cohort. Pediatrics. 2011;127:e948–955. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-3450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bachman JG, O'Malley PM, Johnston LD, Schulenberg JE. 2010. Impacts of parental education on substance use: differences among White, African-American, and Hispanic students in 8th, 10th, and 12th grades (1999–2008). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.
- 22.Hanson MD, Chen E. Socioeconomic status and health behaviors in adolescence: a review of the literature. J Behav Med. 2007;30:263–285. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9098-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. J Stud Alcohol. 2002;(Suppl):54–70. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.54. Mar (14) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Szmigin I, Griffin C, Mistral W, Bengry-Howell A, Weale L, Hackley C. Re-framing [‘]binge drinking’ as calculated hedonism: empirical evidence from the UK. Int J Drug Policy. 2008;19:359–366. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2007.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Child and Adolescent Health Research Unit. Inequalities in Young People's Health: HBSC International Report from the 2005/2006 Survey. Copenhagen: WHO Regional Office for Europe; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bates B, Clemens S, Deverill C, Mackenzie H. Fuller E, editor. Drinking Alcohol. 2007. pp. 65–118. Smoking, drinking and drug use among young people in England in 2006. NHS Information Centre for Health and Social Care.
- 27.Kelly AB, O'Flaherty M, Toumbourou JW, Connor JP, Hemphill SA, Catalano RF. Gender differences in the impact of families on alcohol use: a lagged longitudinal study of early adolescents. Addiction. 2011;106:1427–1436. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03435.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Windle M, Spear LP, Fuligni AJ, Angold A, Brown JD, Pine D, et al. Transitions into underage and problem drinking: developmental processes and mechanisms between 10 and 15 years of age. Pediatrics. 2008;121:S273–289. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243C. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gaughan M. The gender structure of adolescent peer influence on drinking. J Health Soc Behav. 2006;47:47–61. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dick DM, Pagan JL, Holliday C, Viken R, Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J, et al. Gender differences in friends’ influences on adolescent drinking: a genetic epidemiological study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:2012–2019. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00523.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zucker RA, Donovan JE, Masten AS, Mattson ME, Moss HB. Early developmental processes and the continuity of risk for underage drinking and problem drinking. Pediatrics. 2008;121:S252–272. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243B. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tuinstra J, Groothoff JW, Van den Heuvel WJA, Post D. Socio-economic differences in health risk behavior in adolescence: do they exist? Soc Sci Med. 1998;47:67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00034-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Golding J, Pembrey M, Jones R. ALSPAC—the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I. Study methodology. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2001;15:74–87. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.2001.00325.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, et al. Cohort Profile: the ‘Children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2012 doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. Apr 16. [Epub ahead of print] PMID: 22507743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Office of Population Censuses and Surveys (OPCS) Standard Occupational Classification. London, UK: HMSO; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pitkanen T, Kokko K, Lyyra AL, Pulkkinen L. A developmental approach to alcohol drinking behaviour in adulthood: a follow-up study from age 8 to age 42. Addiction. 2008;103:48–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02176.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Donovan JE. Estimated blood alcohol concentrations for child and adolescent drinking and their implications for screening instruments. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e975–981. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.World Health Organization (WHO) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 39.American Psychiatric Association. DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—Text Revision. 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000. p. 943. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Joinson C, Heron J, Lewis G, Croudace T, Araya R. Timing of menarche and depressive symptoms in adolescent girls from a UK cohort. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;198(suppl 1–2):17–23. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Royston P. Multiple imputation of missing values. Stata J. 2004;4:227–241. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Rubin D. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York: Wiley; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 44.White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. 2011;30:377–399. doi: 10.1002/sim.4067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Windle M, Zucker RA. Reducing underage and young adult drinking how to address critical drinking problems during this developmental period. Alcohol Res Health. 2010;33:29–44. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Tobler NS, Roona MR, Ochshorn P, Marshall DG, Streke AV, Stackpole KM. School-based adolescent drug prevention programs: 1998 meta-analysis. J Prim Prev. 2000;20:275–336. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Spoth R, Greenberg M, Turrisi R. Overview of preventive interventions addressing underage drinking state of the evidence and steps toward public health impact. Alcohol Res Health. 2009;32:53–66. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Vogl L, Teesson M, Andrews G, Bird K, Steadman B, Dillon P. A computerized harm minimization prevention program for alcohol misuse and related harms: randomized controlled trial. Addiction. 2009;104:564–575. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02510.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Danielsson AK, Romelsjo A, Tengstrom A. Heavy episodic drinking in early adolescence: gender-specific risk and protective factors. Subst Use Misuse. 2011;46:633–643. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2010.528120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]