Skip to main content
PLOS Computational Biology logoLink to PLOS Computational Biology
. 2014 Sep 4;10(9):e1003796. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003796

Emergence of Assortative Mixing between Clusters of Cultured Neurons

Sara Teller 1, Clara Granell 2, Manlio De Domenico 2, Jordi Soriano 1,*, Sergio Gómez 2, Alex Arenas 2
Editor: Boris S Gutkin3
PMCID: PMC4154651  PMID: 25188377

Abstract

The analysis of the activity of neuronal cultures is considered to be a good proxy of the functional connectivity of in vivo neuronal tissues. Thus, the functional complex network inferred from activity patterns is a promising way to unravel the interplay between structure and functionality of neuronal systems. Here, we monitor the spontaneous self-sustained dynamics in neuronal cultures formed by interconnected aggregates of neurons (clusters). Dynamics is characterized by the fast activation of groups of clusters in sequences termed bursts. The analysis of the time delays between clusters' activations within the bursts allows the reconstruction of the directed functional connectivity of the network. We propose a method to statistically infer this connectivity and analyze the resulting properties of the associated complex networks. Surprisingly enough, in contrast to what has been reported for many biological networks, the clustered neuronal cultures present assortative mixing connectivity values, meaning that there is a preference for clusters to link to other clusters that share similar functional connectivity, as well as a rich-club core, which shapes a ‘connectivity backbone’ in the network. These results point out that the grouping of neurons and the assortative connectivity between clusters are intrinsic survival mechanisms of the culture.

Author Summary

The architecture of neuronal cultures is the result of an intricate self-organization process that balances structural and dynamical demands. We observe that when the motility of neurons is allowed, these neurons organize into compact clusters. These neuronal assemblies have an intrinsic synchronous activity that makes the whole cluster firing at unison. Clusters connect to their neighbors to form a network with rich spontaneous dynamics. This dynamics ultimately shapes a directed functional network whose properties are investigated using network descriptors. We find that the networks are formed such that preference in connectivity between clusters is based on the similarity between their activity, a property that is called assortative mixing in networks' language. This particular choice of connectivity correlations must be rooted to basic survival mechanisms for the neurons constituting the culture.

Introduction

The theory of complex networks [1][5] has proven to be a useful framework for the study of the interplay between structure and functionality in social, technological, and biological systems. A complex network is no more than a specific representation of the interactions between the elements of the system in terms of nodes (elements) and links (interactions) in a graph. The analysis of such resulting abstraction of the system, the network, provides clues about regularities that can be connected with certain functionalities, or even be related to organization mechanisms that help to understand the rules behind the system's complexity. Particularly, in biological systems, the characterization of the emergent self-organization of their components is of utmost importance to comprehend the mechanisms of life [6][8].

One of the major challenges in biology and neuroscience is the ultimate understanding of the structure and function of neuronal systems, in particular the human brain, whose representation in terms of complex networks is especially appealing [9], [10]. In this case, structural connectivity corresponds to the anatomical description of brain circuits whereas the functional connectivity is related to the statistical dependence between neuronal activity.

Network theory and its mathematical framework have provided, through the analysis of the distribution of links, statistical measures that highlight key topological features of the network under study. These measures have facilitated the comprehension of processes as complex as brain development [11], learning [12] and dysfunction [13], [14]. Particularly, these measures have unfolded new relationships between brain dynamics and functionality. For instance, synchronization between neuronal assemblies in the developing hippocampus has been ascribed to the existence of super-connected nodes in a scale-free topology [15]; efficient information transfer has been associated to circuits with small-world features [16], such as in the the nematode worm C. elegans [17] or the brain cortex [18], [19]; and the coexistence of both segregated and integrated activity in the brain has been hypothesized to arise from a modular circuit architecture [20][22].

A network measure that has recently caught substantial attention is the assortativity coefficient, which quantifies the preference of a node to attach to another one with similar (assortative mixing) or dissimilar (disassortative mixing) number of connections [23], [24]. Assortative networks have been observed in both structural [20] and functional [25] human brain networks. It has been proposed that assortative networks exhibit a modular organization [26], display an efficient dynamics that is stable to noise [27], and manifest resilience to node deletion (either random or targeted) [23], [28]. Resilience is ascribed to the preferred interconnectivity of high-degree nodes, which shape a ‘connectivity backbone’ [29] that preserves network integrity. The existence of such a tightly interconnected community is generally known as the ‘rich-club’ phenomenon [19], [22], [30]. On the other hand, disassortative networks, such as the ones identified in the yeast's protein-protein interaction and the neuronal network of C. elegans [23], are more vulnerable to targeted attacks. However, in these disassortative networks, the tendency of high degree nodes to connect with low degree ones results in a star-like topology that favors information processing across the network.

The assortativty coefficient is usually calculated through the Pearson correlation coefficient between the unweighted degrees of each link in the network [23]. To account for effects associated to large networks, the Spearman assortativity measure was introduced [31] and, later, weighted assortativity measures were proposed to include the weight in degree-degree dependencies [32].

To better understand the importance of these network measures in describing neuronal networks, in vitro preparations in the form of neuronal cultures have been introduced given their accessibility and easy manipulation [6], [33]. Two major types of cultured neuronal networks are of particular interest. In a first type, neurons are plated on a substrate that contains a layer of adhesive proteins. Neurons firmly adhere to the substrate, leading to cultures with a homogeneous distribution of neurons [34][37]. In a second type, neurons are plated without any facilitation for adhesion. Neurons then spontaneously group into small, compact assemblies termed clusters that connect to one another [38][41].

The formation of a clustered architecture from an initially isotropic configuration is an intriguing self-organization process [40], [41]. This feature has made clustered networks attractive platforms to study the development of neuronal circuits as well as the interplay between structural and functional connectivity at intermediate, mesoscopic scales [39], [42][45]. Moreover, the existence of a two-level network, one within a cluster and another between clusters, has made clustered cultures appealing to study dynamical and topological features of hierarchical [40], [41], [45], [46] as well as modular networks [46][49].

In this work we use spontaneous activity measurements in clustered neuronal cultures to render the corresponding directed functional networks and study their topological properties. We introduce a novel theoretical framework that uses the propagation of activity between clusters as a measure of “causality”, although strictly speaking we should refer to as a sequence of delayed activations, giving rise to functional connections that are both directed and weighted. Based on this weighted nature of the network, we propose a new measure of assortativity that explicitly incorporates the weight of the links. We observed that all the studied functional networks derived from clustered cultures show a strong, positive assortative mixing that is maintained along different stages of development. On the contrary, homogeneous cultures tend to be weakly assortative, or neutral. Finally, in combination with experiments that measure the robustness of network activity to circuitry deterioration, we show that the strongly assortative, clustered networks are more resistant to damage compared to the weakly assortative, homogeneous ones. Our work provides a prominent example of the existence of assortativity in biological networks, and illustrates the utility of clustered neuronal cultures to investigate topological traits and the emergence of complex phenomena, such as self-organization and resilience, in living neuronal networks.

Results

Experiments

We used rat cortical neurons in all the experiments. As described in Methods, neurons were dissociated and seeded homogeneously on a glass substrate. Cultures were limited to circular areas Inline graphic in diameter for better control and full monitoring of network behavior. The lack of adhesive proteins in the substrate rapidly favored cell-to-cell attachment and aggregation, giving rise to clustered cultures that evolved quickly (Figure 1A). By day in vitro (DIV) Inline graphic, cultures contained dozens of small aggregates, which coalesced and grew in size as the culture matured. Connections between clusters as well as initial traces of spontaneous activity were observed as early as DIV Inline graphic. Cultures comprised of Inline graphic interconnected clusters by DIV Inline graphic, and were sufficiently stable and rich in activity for measurements. Although the strength of the connections in the network and its dynamics evolved further, we observed that the size and position of the clusters remained stable. We therefore measured dynamics already at DIV Inline graphic, and studied cultures up to DIV Inline graphic.

Figure 1. Experiments in clustered neuronal networks.

Figure 1

A Bright field image of a network at day in vitro Inline graphic. Dark circular objects are aggregates of neurons (clusters), and filaments are visible physical connections between them. B Corresponding fluorescence image, integrated over 50 frames (Inline graphic). Bright clusters at the top-left corner are active ones. C Spontaneous activity in the network (see Movie S1 for the actual recording). The top plot shows the average fluorescence signal of the clustered network shown in B, and along Inline graphic of recording. The sharp peaks in fluorescence correspond to the fast sequential ignition of a group of clusters (burst). The bottom raster plot shows the clusters that ignite along the recording. The yellow bars relate a fluorescence peak with the ignition of a group of clusters, and highlights the tendency for the clusters to activate in specific groups. D Example of a particular ignition sequence in a region of the network containing Inline graphic clusters. From left to right, the progress of cluster's activation is revealed by the increase in fluorescence signal of the downstream connected clusters. E Order of activation (black arrows) according to the analysis of the fluorescence signal. The clusters marked in yellow are those that fire simultaneously within experimental resolution. The ones in grey are clusters that do not participate in the firing sequence, and either fire independently or remain silent. F Detail of the fluorescence traces for the Inline graphic participating clusters along two consecutive bursts, illustrating the accuracy in resolving the time delay in the activation of the clusters. The two bursts contain the same clusters, but the activation sequences are slightly different. Blue dots mark the ignition time, and yellow dots signal the clusters that fired simultaneously. The bottom orange boxes depict the final activation sequences of each burst. In the construction of the directed functional network, the influence of a cluster on another is conditioned by the time span between their activations. Close activations result in strong couplings (green arrows); far activations in weak ones (blue). Any two clusters whose activations are above Inline graphic ms are considered functionally uncoupled (red).

The example shown in Figure 1A corresponds to a culture at DIV Inline graphic. Clusters appear as circular objects with an average diameter of Inline graphic and a typical separation of Inline graphic. Connections between clusters are visible as straight filaments that contain several axons.

We monitored spontaneous activity in the clustered network through fluorescence calcium imaging (Figure 1B). Fluorescence images of the clustered network were acquired at a rate of Inline graphic frames per second, and with an image size and resolution that allowed the monitoring of all the clusters in the network with sufficient image quality (see Movie S1). Activity was recorded for typically Inline graphic hour, which provided sufficient statistics in firing events while minimizing culture degradation due to photo-damage.

The analysis of the images at the end of the measurement provided the variations in fluorescence intensity for each cluster and the corresponding onset times of firing (see Methods). As shown in the top panel of Figure 1C, the average fluorescence signal of the network is characterized by peaks of intense cluster activity combined with silent intervals. The accompanying raster plot reveals that this activity actually corresponds to the collective ignition of a small group of clusters, which fire sequentially in a short time window on the order of few hundred milliseconds. We denote by bursts these fast sequences of clusters' activations. Young cultures (DIV Inline graphic) exhibited an activity of about Inline graphic, while maturer cultures (DIV Inline graphic) displayed about Inline graphic (see also Table 1).

Table 1. Network measures of clustered and homogeneous cultures.

Culture type Network DIV burst rate (minInline graphic) Number of nodes Number of edges Average degree Average strength Assortativity Pearson (wh) Assortativity Spearman (wh)
Clus.
A 5 7.35 38 544 14.32 33.71 0.642 Inline graphic 0.044 0.605 Inline graphic 0.036
B 6 6.80 34 1044 30.71 131.94 0.404 Inline graphic 0.040 0.449 Inline graphic 0.035
C 6 6.55 29 762 26.28 142.45 0.440 Inline graphic 0.036 0.425 Inline graphic 0.041
D 7 2.99 27 471 17.44 32.53 0.442 Inline graphic 0.064 0.414 Inline graphic 0.063
E 7 0.87 29 660 22.76 22.83 0.402 Inline graphic 0.043 0.396 Inline graphic 0.045
F 8 0.85 32 750 23.44 12.89 0.317 Inline graphic 0.064 0.287 Inline graphic 0.057
G 8 0.79 35 395 11.29 4.99 0.528 Inline graphic 0.086 0.553 Inline graphic 0.063
H 9 1.17 32 722 22.56 30.99 0.355 Inline graphic 0.061 0.377 Inline graphic 0.052
I 10 3.19 27 486 18.00 81.88 0.460 Inline graphic 0.046 0.478 Inline graphic 0.046
J 12 2.42 24 456 19.00 97.32 0.326 Inline graphic 0.049 0.322 Inline graphic 0.046
K 13 1.28 19 252 13.26 38.27 0.729 Inline graphic 0.062 0.699 Inline graphic 0.051
L 14 3.40 17 116 6.82 28.25 0.586 Inline graphic 0.076 0.552 Inline graphic 0.077
M 14 1.40 25 205 8.20 16.71 0.356 Inline graphic 0.084 0.309 Inline graphic 0.087
N 14 1.86 26 437 16.81 30.63 0.698 Inline graphic 0.060 0.664 Inline graphic 0.061
O 14 4.91 29 391 13.48 38.77 0.372 Inline graphic 0.069 0.364 Inline graphic 0.061
Hom.
P 6 3.30 814 453812 557.51 41.24 0.059 Inline graphic 0.005 0.055 Inline graphic 0.005
Q 8 4.10 589 243606 413.59 27.66 0.038 Inline graphic 0.007 0.036 Inline graphic 0.007
R 10 0.27 562 35379 62.95 2.46 0.112 Inline graphic 0.023 0.125 Inline graphic 0.023
S 15 1.05 1107 239517 216.37 9.47 0.111 Inline graphic 0.008 0.107 Inline graphic 0.008
T 16 0.47 694 274278 395.21 24.89 0.077 Inline graphic 0.007 0.067 Inline graphic 0.006
U 16 0.78 703 155643 221.40 10.34 0.040 Inline graphic 0.009 0.037 Inline graphic 0.009

The table shows the major topological descriptors of the functional networks investigated, Inline graphic corresponding to a clustered neuronal organization and Inline graphic to a homogeneous one. Average degree refers to the mean number of connections per node, and the average strength to the mean weight per node. All the cultures were maintained and studied identically (see Methods). Experiments cover almost Inline graphic days of development in vitro (DIV). All clustered cultures are strongly assortative, while the homogeneous ones tend to be weakly assortative, or neutral.

We observed that the time spanned between two consecutively firing clusters typically ranged between Inline graphic and Inline graphic (see Methods), as also observed by others [47],[48]. These times are fairly large compared to the eventual scale of signal integration-propagation between single neurons (Inline graphic), and is related to the large time scales associated to integration of the intra-clusters information. No consecutive activations were observed above Inline graphic, signaling the termination of a burst. We therefore use this value of Inline graphic as a cut-off to separate a given burst form the preceding one. Then, two clusters that fired above 200 ms cannot be influenced by one another and therefore are not causally connected.

Bursts occurred every 30 s on average for the experiment shown in Figure 1C and, as illustrated by the yellow bands in this figure, each burst typically encompassed a subset of clusters rather than the entire network. In general, however, the number of participating clusters within a burst depended on the details of the culture. Although in a typical experiment the collective firing comprised between Inline graphic and Inline graphic clusters (see Movie S1), in some experiments the entire cluster population lighted up in a single bursting episode.

The analysis of the onset times of firing provides the cluster's activation sequence within each burst. As an example, Figure 1D depicts a highly active region of the network shown in Figure 1B. This region contains Inline graphic clusters, and Inline graphic of them form a subset that regularly fires together. The series of frames show the progress in clusters' activation, revealed by the changes in fluorescence. Activity starts at the top-left cluster and progresses downwards. The time-line of sequence activation after image analysis is shown in Figure 1E, and the actual fluorescence traces are shown in Figure 1F. With our Inline graphic temporal resolution we could resolve well the propagation of activity from a cluster to its neighboring ones (black arrows in Figure 1E). However, and for about Inline graphic of the cases, the time delay between clusters' activation was either too short for detection or activation occurred simultaneously. The clusters associated to these ‘simultaneous’ events are marked in yellow in Figure 1E, and their inter-relation was treated as a bi-directional link (yellow arrow), since no causality can be inferred.

A typical recording provided on the order of Inline graphic bursting episodes. Some of them included the same group of clusters, although the precise sequence of activation could vary. An illustrative example is shown in Figure 1F, which depicts the fluorescence traces of Inline graphic clusters along two consecutive bursts. The first sequence corresponds to the sketch of Figure 1E. The orange box at the bottom of the plot indicates the relative activation time of each cluster within the window, with two clusters treated as simultaneous. To introduce the construction of the directed functional network that is described later, we note that, intuitively, the firing of cluster #9 is most likely caused by #8 and therefore both clusters are (functionally) strongly coupled. At the other extreme, cluster #1 most likely did not trigger #9, and therefore their mutual coupling is very weak. For the second burst, we note that the activation sequence is very similar, but the relative delay times differ, therefore modifying the cluster's coupling strengths. Indeed, cluster #1 and #9 are now functionally disconnected given their long temporal separation.

We carried out measurements in Inline graphic different clustered networks, and labeled them with capital letters as networks ‘A’Inline graphic‘O’. In order to compare their properties with the ones from cultures with a distinct structure, we applied the same measuring protocols and data analysis to Inline graphic cultures characterized with a homogeneous distribution of neurons (see Methods and Figure S1), and labeled them as networks ‘P’Inline graphic‘U’.

Construction of the directed functional networks

The above sequences of clusters' activations, extended to all the clusters and bursting episodes of the monitored culture, convey information on the degree of causal influence between any pair of clusters in the network. For instance, cluster #5 in Figures 1E-F can fire because of the first order influence of clusters #3 and #4, but also because of the second and third order influences of clusters #2 and #1, respectively. Hence, a realistic functional network construction should take into account these possible influences from the upstream connected clusters to build a network whose links are not only directed, but also weighted by the time delays in activation. This weighted treatment of the interaction between clusters is the major novelty of our work and the backbone of our model.

More formally, the interaction between any two clusters follows the principle of causality, i.e. the firing of cluster Inline graphic immediately after cluster Inline graphic eventually implies that cluster Inline graphic has induced the activity of Inline graphic at that particular time. The likelihood of this relation between clusters is weighted according to its frequency along the full observational time, allowing to a statistical validation. Indeed, cluster Inline graphic could induce the activity of various clusters, if all of them activate in a physically plausible short time window after cluster Inline graphic. Such a construction is illustrated in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Sketch of the construction of the directed functional network.

Figure 2

A Schematic representation of the experimental data, with Inline graphic firings of four different clusters (or neurons in the case of homogeneous cultures). B Stages of the method to construct the directed functional network. (1) The first step consists in detecting the different bursts in the whole sequence. Firings that are separated by more than Inline graphic for clustered cultures (Inline graphic for homogeneous ones) are not considered part of the same burst. (2) Calculation of the time lags Inline graphic between consecutive firings inside the bursts, for the whole sequence. The frequency distribution of time lags is next fitted to a Gaussian distribution Inline graphic, finally providing the variance Inline graphic that will be specific for each culture. (3) Weighting procedure example for the first burst. Cluster #1 can activate #2 and #3. The weight of the links 1Inline graphic2 and 1Inline graphic3 depends on the time differences between clusters' activations, and is given by the function Inline graphic determined by the previous fitting. Hence, weight Inline graphic, and Inline graphic. Cluster #3 can be activated as well by cluster #2, with Inline graphic. (4) Schematic representation of the resulting directed functional connectivity network. The width of the connections is proportional to the weight of the links for clarity.

To construct the directed functional networks for each studied culture we proceed as follows. First of all, we divide the entire firing sequence into the bursts of clusters' activity (Figure 2A) using the cut-off of Inline graphic introduced in the previous section. Once the bursts have been detected, we compute the frequency distribution of time lags between pairs of consecutive firings (Figure 2B). This frequency distribution informs about the characteristic times expected between two consecutive firings within the same burst, and hence it is a good proxy of the causal influence of a cluster on another. We will use this information to weight the causal influence of firing propagation. The frequency distribution Inline graphic presents a good fit to a universal Gaussian decay (Inline graphic) in all the analyzed cultures, although the variance Inline graphic is specific for each culture. We indeed observed (Figure S2) that Inline graphic decreases with the culture age in vitro (correlation coefficient Inline graphic, significance Inline graphic), and increases with the number of clusters present in the network (Inline graphic, Inline graphic).

The last step in the construction of the directed functional networks consists in linking the interactions within each burst, and weighting them according to the previous frequency distribution (Figure 2B). The rationale behind this process is as follows: we hypothesize that every cluster influences other clusters (posterior in time) within a burst and, the larger the time after a cluster has fired the lower the influence we expect in the activation of another cluster (simply because the signal fades out). Then, the weighting of the interaction by the expected frequency observed in the distribution conveys the functional influence between clusters. The weights are reinforced every time the same pair of clusters' sequence is observed. After processing the full sequence we obtain a peer-to-peer activation map that is our proxy of the functional network.

We proceeded identically to construct the directed functional networks for homogeneous cultures (see Methods), with the only difference that the cut-off time corresponds to Inline graphic. We tested for both clustered and homogeneous cultures that the obtained functional networks were stable upon variations of the cut-off times (see Figure S3 and Discussion).

Analysis of the functional networks

We computed the functional networks of the Inline graphic (‘A’ to ‘O’) realizations of clustered cultures, as well as the Inline graphic (‘P’ to ‘U’) homogeneous ones, and analyzed some major topological traits. Firstly, for each culture we obtained the number of nodes, the number of edges, the average degree of the networks, and its average strength (see Methods). The investigated networks and their topological measures are summarized in Table 1. Although young cultures display a richer activity, in general all networks present a similar number of nodes and a comparable functional connectivity, which is described by the number of edges, the average degree and the average strength.

Representative examples of the investigated functional networks for the clustered configuration are shown in Figure 3 (see Figure S1 for an example of the homogeneous ones). The position of the nodes and their size are the same as the actual clusters for easier comparison. Edges in the directed network are both color and thickness coded to highlight their importance, with darker colors corresponding to the highest weights. This representation reveals those pairs of clusters that maintain a persistent causality relationship over time. Nodes are also color coded according to their strength, i.e. the total weight of the in- and out-edges.

Figure 3. Neuronal cultures and functional networks.

Figure 3

Top: Bright field images of Inline graphic representative neuronal cultures at different days in vitro. Bottom: Corresponding functional networks obtained from the directed and weighed construction described in Figure 2. From left to right, the pictures correspond to the cultures labeled D, H and O in Table 1. Only active clusters are used in the construction of the functional network. The size of the nodes is similar to the ones observed in the cultures, and facilitates the comparison of the functional network with the real culture. In the functional networks, the edges are both color and thickness coded according to their weight, while the nodes are only color coded according to their strength. The darker the color, the higher the value.

The functional networks exhibit some interesting features. First, there are groups of nodes that form tightly connected communities. These communities actually reflect the most frequent bursting sequences. Second, nodes preferentially connect to neighboring ones with some long-range connectivity, and often following paths that are not the major physical connections. This indicates that the structural connectivity of the network cannot be assessed from just an examination of the most perceivable processes. And third, as shown in Figure S4A, we observed that there is no correlation between the width of the physical connections and their weight (Inline graphic, Inline graphic), or the size of the nodes and their strength (Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Figure S4B), and indicates that the dynamical traits of the network cannot be inferred from its physical configuration, stressing the importance of the functional study.

We also observed that the size of the clusters did not correlate with their average activity (Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Figure S4C), i.e. small and big clusters displayed similar firing frequencies, and of Inline graphic on average. However, since some clusters are initiators of activity and others just followers, we also computed the relative contribution of a given cluster size to initiate activity in the network. We found no significant correlation between initiation and cluster size (Inline graphic, Inline graphic, Figure S4D). These results strengthen the conclusion that one cannot predict the clusters that will initiate activity, or the most persistent sequences, by just a visual inspection of cluster sizes and their distribution over the network.

These analyses are important in the context of the work by Shein-Idelson and coworkers [41], who studied the dynamics of isolated clusters similar to ours, and observed that their firing rate increased from Inline graphic to Inline graphic as the clusters' radii escalated from Inline graphic to Inline graphic. This remarkable difference in the dynamics between ‘isolated’ and ‘networked’ clusters reflects the dominant role of the network circuitry in shaping its dynamics.

Finally, to crosscheck that the results found for the functional networks presented here are robust to the inference method, we have also performed a classical mutual information analysis to construct functional networks for the same cultures (see Methods). The results obtained with the mutual information analysis are totally in agreement with the constructed functional networks using time delays.

Assortativity and rich-club properties

We determined the values of the weighted formulation of assortativity, both for the Pearson Inline graphic and Spearman Inline graphic correlations, with values in Inline graphic (see Methods for the generalization of assortativity to directed weighted networks). Positive values of the weighted assortativity indicate that nodes with similar strength tend to connect to one another, while negative values mean the preferred interconnectivity of nodes with different strength. In Table 1 we can observe that all clustered networks (labeled ‘A’-‘O’) exhibit a positive weighted assortativity, in the range Inline graphic for the Pearson construction and Inline graphic for the Spearman one. Although the values fluctuate across different cultures, the two assortativity measures provide the same value within statistical error, and reflect that network size corrections provided by the Spearman's treatment have little influence in strongly assortative networks.

To assess the importance of the measured assortativity values, we have also computed the weighted rich-club [50]. The rich-club phenomenon refers to the tendency of nodes with high degree to form tightly interconnected communities, compared to the connections that these nodes would have in a null model that preserves the node's degree but otherwise is totally random. Given the positive assortativity found, we analyzed whether this finding is also reinforced by the existence of rich-club structures.

The weighted formulation for the rich-club takes into account the node's strength instead of the degree, and is particularly useful in situations in which the weights of the links can not be overlooked [51]. The evaluation of the rich-club Inline graphic is performed by computing the ratio between the connectivity strength of highly connected nodes and its randomized counterpart, and for gradually higher values of the strength threshold Inline graphic. The detailed calculation is described in the Methods section, and the results of the analysis for representative networks is shown in Figure S5. Ratios larger than Inline graphic indicate that higher strength nodes are more interconnected to each other than what one would expect in a random configuration. On the contrary, a ratio less than Inline graphic reveals an opposite organizing principle that leads to a lack of interconnectivity among high-degree nodes. After the calculation of the ratios for all the studied clustered networks, we found a positive tendency towards the creation of rich-clubs in all of them (Figure S5), which is in good agreement with the observed values of assortativity.

The above network measures were also analyzed in experiments with a homogeneous distribution of neurons (labeled ‘P’Inline graphic‘U’). The results are summarized in Table 1. Interestingly, the assortativity values are much lower (by an order of magnitude on average) than the ones for clustered cultures, in the range Inline graphic for Pearson's and Inline graphic for Spearman's. Accordingly, the rich-club phenomenon for the homogeneous cultures vanishes (Figure S5).

Network resilience

Several studies highlight the importance of assortative features for network resilience to damage. Given the strong assortativity of our clustered cultures, we carried out a new set of experiments to investigate the concurrent presence of resilient traits. As described in Methods, we considered two major ‘damaging’ actions to the network. In a first one, we gradually weakened the excitatory network connectivity by means of the AMPA-glutamate antagonist CNQX, and measured the decay in spontaneous activity as connectivity failed. In a second one, we continuously exposed a culture to strong fluorescence light, therefore inducing photo-damage to the neurons. This action resulted in random neuronal death across the network and hence a progressive failure of its spontaneous dynamics. The rate of activity decay upon radiation damage provided an estimation of the resistance of the network to node deletion. These investigations were carried out at the same time in clustered cultures (strongly assortative) and in homogeneous ones (weakly assortative or neutral). Their comparison provided a first reference to relate assortativity, network topology and resistance to damage.

Figure 4A shows the results for the application of CNQX to clustered cultures. We first monitored each cluster individually in the unperturbed case, and measured its average firing activity Inline graphic along Inline graphic. We then applied a given drug concentration, measured the firing activity Inline graphic for another Inline graphic, and computed the relative changes in activity respect to the unperturbed case, as Inline graphic. The protocol was repeated until activity ceased. Two illustrative examples of the action of CNQX on network activity are provided in Figure 4. In a clustered cultured and for weak CNQX applications (Inline graphic) the activity in some clusters increases, while in some other decreases, and on average the network firing rate remains stable (Inline graphic). As [CNQX] is increased to Inline graphic, we observe that most of the clusters have reduced their activity, although there are still some that maintain a high activity or even increase it. This different behavior from cluster to cluster suggests that clustered networks are highly flexible, and that they may have mechanisms to preserve activity even with strong weakening of the connectivity. Conversely, homogeneous cultures (Figure 4B) lose activity in a more regular and faster way. These networks are characterized by a highly coherent dynamics [36], [37], and therefore all neurons in the network reduce activity similarly as CNQX is applied. Interestingly, for Inline graphic the shown homogeneous culture has almost completely silenced (Inline graphic), while the clustered culture is still highly active. We repeated this study on Inline graphic different realizations of each culture type and observed that, on average, the critical concentration Inline graphic at which activity complete stopped was Inline graphic for clustered and Inline graphic for homogeneous networks (Figure 4C).

Figure 4. Network resilience to damage.

Figure 4

A-B Examples of the degradation of neuronal activity in clustered and homogeneous cultures due to the gradual weakening of excitatory connectivity. Both culture types were investigated at the same day in vitro Inline graphic and contained a similar density of neurons. The weakening of connections is achieved by gradually increasing the concentration of CNQX, an AMPA-glutamate receptor antagonist in excitatory neurons. Network response upon weakening is quantified through the relative change in activity Inline graphic between a given CNQX application and the unperturbed state. Activity variations are indicated separately for each cluster, and shown according to the cluster labeling number. A Clustered cultures show a mixed response upon weakening, with some clusters increasing activity and others reducing it. Only for relatively high concentrations of CNQX (Inline graphic) the activity systematically decays up to the full silencing of the network. B In homogeneous cultures, activity is analyzed in Inline graphic regions that cover in a regular manner the entire network. Activity decays almost equally in all regions. Relatively small drug concentrations of [CNQX]Inline graphic practically suffice to fully stop activity. C Average critical concentration [CNQX]Inline graphic at which spontaneous activity completed ceases, about Inline graphic for clustered networks and Inline graphic for homogeneous ones. Data is averaged over Inline graphic network realizations of each type of culture. D Photo-damage experiments. Spontaneous activity is measured in cultures that are continuously exposed to strong fluorescence light, causing gradual neuronal degradation and ultimately the death of the entire network. The total radiation received by the neurons is calculated as the duration of the exposure times the area covered by the neurons in the culture (Inline graphic and Inline graphic on average for clustered and homogeneous clusters, respectively). The spontaneous activity in homogeneous cultures decays at a much faster rate than in the clustered counterparts. Data is averaged over Inline graphic network realization of each type. Error bars show standard deviation.

Figure 4D shows the results for the resistance of the networks to node deletion as a consequence of direct photo-damage to the neurons. As can be observed, homogeneous cultures decay in activity much faster than the clustered ones, pinpointing the general resistance of clustered cultures to structural failure.

Discussion

Clustered neuronal cultures have a unique self-organizing potential. An initially isotropic ensemble of individual neurons quickly group to one another to constitute a stable configuration of interconnected clusters of tightly packed neurons. The formation of the clustered network is primarily a passive process governed by the pulling forces exerted by the neurites. Interestingly, aggregation occurs even in the absence of glial cells and neuronal activity [40], and is maintained up to the degradation of the culture [40], [52], [53]. Our work shows that this self-organizing process drives the network towards specific dynamic states, which shape a topology of the functional network that is distinctively assortative. We note that the number of clusters and their distribution are initially random. Therefore, a wide spectrum of physical circuitries and functional topologies are in principle attainable. However, in all the studied cultures, the network drives itself towards markedly assortative topologies with a ‘rich-club’ core. The emergence of these distinct topological traits, concurrently with a stronger network's resilience to activity deterioration, pictures a self-organizing mechanism that enhances network survival by procuring a robust architecture and dynamic stability.

We remark that the link between assortativity and resilience is based on the comparison between the response of clustered and homogeneous cultures upon the same perturbation. To obtain conclusive evidences that assortativity confers resilience traits exclusively from topology, we would require an experimental protocol in which we could arbitrarily ‘rewire’ the connectivity between clusters, or shape in a control manner different circuitries while preserving the number of nodes in the network. Although these strategies are certainly enlightening, they are of difficult development and a major experimental challenge.

We infer the functional connectivity maps of the clustered networks from their spontaneous dynamics. We considered small-sized networks to simultaneously access the entire population (Inline graphic clusters). The approach that we have used to characterize this functional connectivity is based on the analysis of the time delays between consecutive clusters' activations. The uniqueness of our approach is to use these time delays to provide a direct measure of causality, giving rise to a functional network that is both directed and weighted, with the weights given by a decaying function that follows the frequency of the delay between pairs of clusters. Our formulation is simple and naturally derives from the intrinsic dynamics of the network.

We used two main parameters to quantitatively construct the directed functional network, namely the cut-off time for causality, and the variance Inline graphic of the Gaussian-like weighting function. The cut-off time is set to Inline graphic, two times the maximum measured time delay between consecutive activations. The importance of the cut-off is first to discriminate two successive bursting episodes, and second to exclude individual firing events from an actual cascade of activations. Although these individual firings account for less than 2% of the total activations, they may occur in regions of the culture that are physically distant -though temporary close- from an actual sequence, and therefore they would add spurious, long-range functional connections to the network.

On the other hand, the variance Inline graphic is obtained from a Gaussian fit of the distribution of consecutive activation delays within bursts. The value of Inline graphic is specific for each culture to take into account particular differences in the dynamics of the network, specifically the culture days in vitro or the number of clusters (Figure S2), parameters that could affect the delay times of activation. Young cultures for instance exhibit longer time delays between pairs of clusters, leading to a distribution Inline graphic shifted towards higher values and therefore a larger Inline graphic.

We tested that the obtained functional networks were stable upon variation of the above parameters. In particular, to examine whether the choice of the cut-off does or does not substantially affect the features of the generated functional network, we performed a sensitivity analysis on this parameter. As the process of generating the network from the sets of bursts is deterministic, we analyzed the influence of the cut-off value on the formed groups of firings. To quantify the variation on the bursts generated for different values of the cut-off, we calculated the variation of information [54] between the grouping of bursts at a certain cut-off value and the previous one as a measure to assess their difference (Figure S3). In the case of clustered cultures, we found that for values of cut-off of Inline graphic the variation of information is, on average, on the order of Inline graphic. In the homogeneous case, for cut-off values of Inline graphic ms, this value is on the order of Inline graphic. This means that varying the cut-off values in these regions does not substantially change the grouping of the bursts, and therefore the generated networks are equivalent.

To assess the goodness of our construction in inferring the functional connectivity of the clustered networks, we compared our connectivity maps with those procured by information theoretic measures, such as Mutual Information or Transfer Entropy, applied to the original fluorescence recordings. These approaches have been used to draw the topological properties of neuronal networks in vitro, both in electrode recordings [55], [56] and calcium fluorescence imaging [57], [58]. The comparison of our method with these theoretic measures showed that the identified functional links were fundamentally the same, with small quantitative differences associated to the particular weighting procedures.

Our functional networks consistently maintained high assortativity values, and along a wide range of days in vitro. We also observed that, by contrast, the assortativity analysis in homogeneous cultures procured neutral or low assortativity values, a result that is supported by other studies in homogeneous networks similar to ours [55]. In our study, we have seen that the clustered, assortative networks exhibit a higher resilience of the network to damage compared to the homogeneous, non-assortative ones. Different studies also highlighted the importance of assortativity and the ‘rich-club’ phenomenon on higher-order structures of the network, in particular resilience, hierarchical ordering and specialization [10], [30].

Several studies in brain networks advocate that the functional connectivity reflects the underlying structural organization [59][61]. To shed light on this interrelation in our cultures, we would need the identification of all the physical links between clusters. The top images of Figure 3 indeed suggest that some structural connections could be delineated by a simple visual inspection. However, we observed by green fluorescence protein (GFP) transfection that physical connections have long extensions and may easily link several clusters together, and not just in a first-neighbor manner as seen in the images. Since the images provide a very poor subset of the entire structural layout, a complete description of the physical circuitry must be carried out before comparing the structural and functional networks. Such a detailed identification is difficult, and requires the use of a number of connectivity-labeling techniques. Nevertheless, for the connections that we could visualize, we draw two major conclusions. First, that neither the width of the physical connections nor the size of the clusters were related to a particular trait of the functional links, such as the weight of the connections or the strength of the nodes (Figure S4). And, second, that our construction inferred strong functional links between clusters that were not directly connected in a physical manner, highlighting the importance of indirect paths as well as long-range coupling in the flow of activity.

The identification of the full set of structural connections would certainly provide invaluable information to investigate the interplay between structure and function in our networks. In this context, the recent work by Santos-Sierra et al [52] is enlightening. They analyzed some major structural connectivity traits in clustered networks similar to ours, and observed that the networks were strongly assortative as well. Assortativity emerged at early stages of development, and was maintained throughout the life of the culture. Hence, in clustered cultures, the combined evidences of this study and ours hints at the existence of assortative properties in both structure and function.

An interesting peculiarity of our experiments is that, in most of the studied clustered cultures, the spontaneous bursting episodes comprised of a small subset of clusters rather than the entire network. This activation in the form of groups or moduli is often referred as conditional activity. It contrasts with the coherent activity of homogeneous cultures, where the entire network lights up in a short time window during a bursting episode. Given the acute differences in assortativity between clustered and homogeneous cultures, we hypothesize that the modular dynamics by itself increases or reinforces assortative traits in the functional network.

We finally remark that our neuronal cultures are spatial, i.e. embedded in a physical substrate, which imposes constraints to the layout of connections and, in turn, their assortative characteristics [52], [62]. Spatial networks have caught substantial interest in the last years to understand the restrictions—or advantages—that metric correlations impose on the structure and dynamics of complex networks [63], in particular brain circuits [64]. Vértes et al showed that spatial constraints delineate several topological properties of functional brain networks [65], and Orlandi et al showed that the initiation mechanisms of spontaneous activity are governed by metric correlations inherited by the network during its formation [36]. Strong spatial constraints in clustered networks can be attained by anchoring the neuronal aggregates in specific locations, for instance through carbon nanotubes [39]. The comparison of the functional maps of such a forced organization with our free one is enlightening, and would shed light on the importance of structural constraints in shaping functional connectivity.

To conclude, we have presented a simple yet powerful construction to draw the directed functional connectivity in clustered neuronal cultures. The construed networks present assortativity and ‘rich-club’ features, which are present concurrently with resilience traits. Our analysis has been based on spontaneous activity data, and may certainly vary from evoked activity. Hence, the combined experimental setup and functional construction can be viewed as a model system for complex networks studies, specially to understand the interplay between structure and function, and the emergence of key topological traits from network dynamics. Also, the spatial nature of our experiments may also procure invaluable data to understanding the role of short- and long-range connections in network dynamics; or to investigate the targeted deletion of the high degree nodes that shape the backbone of the network. The latter is a powerful concept that may assist in a detailed exploration of resilience in neuronal circuits, for instance to model the circuitry-activity interrelation in neurological pathologies.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

All procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee for Animal Experimentation of the University of Barcelona, under order DMAH-5461.

Clustered neuronal cultures

In our experiments we used cortical neurons from Inline graphic day old Sprague-Dawley rat embryos. Following standard procedures [36], [66] dissection was carried out in ice-cold L-Inline graphic medium enriched with Inline graphic glucose and gentamycin (Sigma-Aldrich). Cortices were gently extracted and dissociated by repeated pipetting.

Cortical neurons were plated onto Inline graphic glass coverslips (Marienfeld-Superior) that incorporated a poly-dimethylsiloxane (PDMS) mold. The PDMS restricted neuronal growth to isolated, circular cavities Inline graphic in diameter. Prior plating, glasses were washed in Inline graphic nitric acid for 2 h, rinsed with double-distilled water (DDW), sonicated in ethanol and flamed. In parallel to glass cleaning, and following the procedure described by Orlandi et al. [36], several Inline graphic diameter layers of PDMS Inline graphic thick were prepared and subsequently pierced with Inline graphic diameter biopsy punchers (Integra-Miltex). Each pierced PDMS mold typically contained Inline graphic to Inline graphic cavities. The PDMS molds were then attached to the glasses and the combined structure autoclaved at Inline graphic, firmly adhering to one another. For each dissection we prepared Inline graphic identical glass-PDMS structures, giving rise to about Inline graphic cultures of Inline graphic in diameter. Neurons were plated in the PDMS cavities with a nominal density of Inline graphic, and incubated in plating medium at 37°C, Inline graphic CO2 and Inline graphic humidity. Plating medium consisted in Inline graphic of foetal calf serum (FCS, Invitrogen), Inline graphic of horse serum (HS, Inivtrogen), and 0.1% B27 (Sigma) in MEM Eagle's-L-glutamate (Invitrogen). MEM was enriched with gentamicin (Sigma), the neuronal activity promoter Glutamax (Sigma) and glucose.

Upon plating, the absence of adhesive proteins in the glass substrate rapidly favored cell-cell attachment and, gradually, the formation of islands of highly compact neuronal assemblies or clusters that minimized the surface contact with the substrate. Clustered cultures formed quickly. By day in vitro (DIV) 2 the culture encompasses dozens of small aggregates that coalesce and grow in size as the culture matures. Spontaneous activity and connections between clusters were observed by DIV Inline graphic. Clusters at this stage of development also anchored at the surface of the glass and, although they continued growing and developing connections, their number and position remained practically stable along the next Inline graphic weeks. At the moment of measuring, each PDMS cavity contained an independent culture formed by Inline graphic interconnected clusters.

Clustered cultures were maintained for about Inline graphic weeks, as follows. At DIV Inline graphic the medium was switched from plating to changing medium (containing Inline graphic FUDR, Inline graphic Uridine, and Inline graphic HS in enriched MEM) to limit glial cell division. Three days later, the medium was replaced to final medium (enriched MEM with Inline graphic HS), which was then refreshed periodically every three days.

Homogeneous neuronal cultures

Overnight exposure of the glass coverslips to poly-l-lisine (PLL, Sigma) provided a layer of adhesive proteins for the neurons to quickly anchor upon seeding, leading to cultures with a homogeneous distribution of neurons over the substrate. The remaining steps in the preparation and maintenance of the cultures were identical as the clustered ones, i.e. we used the same nominal neuronal density for plating, we included PDMS pierced molds to confine neuronal growth in cavities Inline graphic in diameter, and we refreshed the culture mediums in the same manner.

Experimental setup and procedure

Standard experiments

To measure the spontaneous activity in the clustered networks we used cultures at day in vitro (DIV) Inline graphic, i.e. covering about two weeks of development. Cultures started to degrade by DIV Inline graphic, and therefore we did not use cultures older than Inline graphic weeks in our experiments.

Activity in neuronal cultures was monitored through fluorescence calcium imaging [67], [68], which allows the detection of neuronal activity by the binding of Inline graphic ions to a fluorescence probe upon firing. Prior to recording, the cultures under study were incubated for Inline graphic in External Medium (EM, consisting of Inline graphic NaCl, Inline graphic CaCl2, Inline graphic MgCl2, Inline graphic sucrose, Inline graphic glucose, and Inline graphic Hepes; pH 7.4) in the presence of Fluo-4-AM (Invtrogen). We used Inline graphic Fluo4 in a volume of Inline graphic EM. We incubated a glass coverslip containing Inline graphic cultures within the PDMS cavities at once, allowing for the simultaneous recording of different cultures or the selection of cultures with specific traits. After incubation, the cultures were washed with fresh EM and placed in the observation chamber, consisting of a standard glass bottom culture dish, filled with Inline graphic ml EM, and with its wall and cover screened from external light. To minimize accidental damage to the aggregates during the manipulation of the cultures, the PDMS pierced mold was left in contact with the glass during both incubation and the actual experiment.

The observation chamber was mounted on Zeiss Axiovert inverted microscope equipped with a high-speed CMOS camera (Hamamatsu Orca Flash 2.8). We used an objective of Inline graphicX combined with a Inline graphicX optical zoom. These settings provided a final field of view of Inline graphic (Inline graphic)Inline graphic that supported the recording of Inline graphic or Inline graphic PDMS-confined cultures simultaneously. Individual frames were acquired as 8-bit grey-scale images, a size of Inline graphic pixels, and a spatial resolution of Inline graphic. All experiments were carried out at room temperature.

The fluorescence signal of the clusters' spontaneous activity was recorded with the software Hokawo 2.5, provided by the camera vendor (Hamamatsu Photonics). We used acquisition speeds in the range Inline graphic frames per second (fps), corresponding to a time interval of Inline graphic between consecutive frames. These acquisition speeds were selected to optimize the balance between image quality, sufficient time resolution, and minimum light intensity. The latter was particularly important to minimize photo-damage and photo-bleaching, and allowed neuronal cultures to be studied with optimal conditions for at least 3 h. However, the combination of high acquisition speeds and high resolution images resulted in large data files—of at least Inline graphic GB per hour of recording—that had to be stored and analyzed. We therefore limited most of our experiments to about 1 h of recording, which was sufficient to reliably build the functional networks, as described in the ‘Control experiments’ section.

Measurements in homogeneous cultures were carried out in the same way, with the only difference that the recording speed was increased to Inline graphicInline graphic fps to take into account the fast propagation of activity fronts in these preparations, as observed for instance in the study of Orlandi et al. [36].

Resilience experiments

We considered two groups of resilience experiments. In a first group, we monitored the gradual degradation of network activity due to photo-damage. In a second group, we measured the decay in activity as a consequence of the gradual weakening of the excitatory connectivity.

In the first group of measurements, we first considered a clustered culture and measured its spontaneous activity uninterruptedly along 2 hours, with neurons continuously exposed to a light radiation Inline graphic times stronger than normal. We then divided the sequence in blocks of 30 min, and determined, for each block, the average network activity by counting the number of bursting episodes within the block. Next, we switched to a homogeneous culture from the same batch (i.e. identical nominal density and age) and carried out the same protocol. In total we carried out Inline graphic measurements for each kind of culture, and finally analyzed the decay in activity as a function of time. Although the radiation over the culture was homogeneous, the actual area occupied by the neurons was different between homogeneous and clustered cultures. Neurons in homogeneous cultures formed a monolayer that covered Inline graphic of the available area, corresponding to about Inline graphic for the Inline graphic diameter wells. In clustered cultures, neurons were tightly packed in slightly three-dimensional structures, giving rise to a lower spatial coverage of Inline graphic, i.e. about Inline graphic. Hence, homogeneous cultures were effectively more exposed to light than their clustered counterparts. To take this spatial variability into account, the ‘total radiation’ received by the neurons in a given experiment was quantified as the duration of the light exposure times the area occupied by the neurons in the studied culture. We then averaged the results over the Inline graphic different culture realizations of each type, and binned nearby values for clarity. The comparison of the activity-radiation plots between the two networks (Figure 4D) indicated which topology exhibited higher resistance to degradation in neuronal activity.

In the second group of measurements, we compared the change in activity between a clustered and a homogeneous culture during gradual weakening of neuronal connectivity. The weakening was achieved by progressive application of CNQX [37], [69], an AMPA-glutamate receptor antagonist in excitatory neurons (see also ‘Pharmacology’). We first measured the clustered network and thereafter the homogeneous one. In both cases, we first recorded spontaneous activity at Inline graphic, and used the average firing rate Inline graphic as reference for the subsequent steps. We then increased the concentration of the drug to a preset value, waited Inline graphic min for the drug to take effect and measured again for Inline graphic, computing the new average firing rate Inline graphic. We switched to a second preset values, and repeated the procedure until activity ceased. The relative decay in activity Inline graphic is used to illustrate the gradual fall of activity in Figure 4C. The critical concentration [CNQX]Inline graphic at which activity is absent along the Inline graphic of recording (Inline graphic) hints at the robustness of network dynamics to a global failure of its connectivity.

Pharmacology

The pharmacological protocols described below were used identically in clustered and homogeneous cultures.

Inhibitory connections

The in vitro networks contain both excitatory and inhibitory connections. However, for sake of simplicity in the comparison between experiments and model, in the experiments at DIV Inline graphic and above we completely blocked γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) inhibitory synapses with Inline graphic of the antagonist bicuculine methiodide (Sigma). The drug was applied Inline graphic before the actual recordings for the drug to take effect. Spontaneous activity in our experiments is therefore solely driven by excitatory connections. Although the balance between excitation and inhibition shapes the major traits of spontaneous activity [36], [37], such as the average firing rate of the network, we verified that the presence of inhibition did not qualitatively modify the results presented here.

We left active inhibitory synapses for experiments at DIV Inline graphic since at this early stages of development GABA has a depolarizing effect and therefore an excitatory action [37], [69], [70]. Its blockade would effectively reduce excitation and silence the network.

Network connectivity weakening through CNQX

In the studies of network resilience to the weakening of connectivity, we studied the decay in spontaneous activity as a result of the gradual application of 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX, Sigma), an AMPA-glutamate receptor antagonists in excitatory neurons. For Inline graphic the connectivity strength between neurons is maximum. As [CNQX] is administered, the efficacy of excitatory connections steadily diminishes, which is accompanied by a reduction in spontaneous activity (see e.g. [37] for illustrative data). High CNQX concentrations lead to a complete halt in activity. In the measurements we used CNQX concentrations in the range Inline graphic, in quasi-logarithmic steps. We left the culture unperturbed for 5 min after each CNQX application for the drug to reach steady-state effects.

Data analysis for clustered cultures

The acquired images (recorded at a typical speed of Inline graphic fps) were first analyzed with the Hokawo 2.5 software to extract the fluorescence intensity of each cluster as a function of time. The regions of interest (ROIs) were chosen manually and typically covered an area of Inline graphic pixels, each ROI corresponding to a single cluster. As illustrated in Figure 1C and Figure 1F, activity is characterized by a stable baseline (resting state) interrupted by peaks of fluorescence that correspond to clusters' firings. At the onset of firing, the fluorescence signal increases abruptly due to the fast intake of Inline graphic ions. Fluorescence then reaches a maximum, and slowly decays back to the baseline in Inline graphic s.

The algorithm that we used to detect the onset of firing for each cluster was as follows. We first corrected the fluorescence signal Inline graphic from small drifts, and calculated the resting fluorescence level Inline graphic by discarding the data points with an amplitude two times above the standard deviation (SD) of the signal. The corrected signal was then expressed as Inline graphic. We next took Inline graphic and computed its derivative Inline graphic in order to detect fast changes in the fluorescence signal. Finally, the onset of ignition in cluster was defined as the time where a maximum in Inline graphic was accompanied by values of Inline graphic two times above the SD of the background signal, and for at least 5 frames.

Reliability in detecting the clusters' ignition times

Three major tests were carried out to assess the reliability of our analysis. In a first one, we measured spontaneous activity at Inline graphic fps, i.e. twice the standard recording speed, but used stronger light to compensate for the lower exposure time. We next analyzed the data, re-sampled the image sequence down to Inline graphic fps and compared the results with the original acquisition. We observed that the detection of the onset times improved only by about 15%, which did not justify the excess of light and the associated damage to the neurons.

In a second test, we measured spontaneous activity in a culture using identical light settings but considering different acquisition rates, namely Inline graphic, Inline graphic, and Inline graphic fps. We then selected ignition sequences that were as similar as possible in all three measurements, and compared the results. We observed that only in the few cases where the clusters fired with strong amplitudes the increased speed enhanced detection, and again by Inline graphic. For the rest of the cases, the higher speeds actually worsened the analysis due to the poorer signal-to-noise ratio.

Finally, in a third test, we used sub-frame resolution analysis tools to evaluate the importance of finer ignition times. Following Orlandi et al. [36], we considered the approach of fitting two straight lines at the vicinity of each initially detected firing. A first fit included the Inline graphic points of the background signal that preceded ignition, and a second one extended to the Inline graphic points that correspond to the fast rise in fluorescence. The crossing value of the two lines provided an onset time that refined the initially measured value. The better accuracy effectively increased the discrimination of sequences that were initially identified as simultaneous events. However, since these events are rare (by Inline graphic), the finer temporal resolution had practically no effect in the construction of the functional networks and the derived analysis.

Activity propagation times

The time delay Inline graphic in the propagation of activity between two connected clusters was measured in control experiments with high acquisition rates. We concluded that Inline graphic varied in the range Inline graphic ms, with an average value Inline graphic. Other studies in clustered networks provided similar results [47]. With the detection algorithm described above and standard experiments at Inline graphic fps, we could appraise the activation sequence in Inline graphic of the cases. The remaining Inline graphic corresponded to clusters that ignited in the same frame or time bin, and were treated as simultaneous events.

Data analysis for homogeneous cultures

Recordings in homogeneous cultures provided the activity of Inline graphic neurons in an circular area Inline graphic in diameter. Neurons were marked individually as regions of interest in the images and the corresponding fluorescence time traces extracted using custom-made software. Ignition times for each neuron were next obtained by using the sub-frame resolution method described above (detailed in Ref. [36]), and that consisted in fitting two straight lines to the fluorescence data, a first fit encompassing the Inline graphic points in the background region prior to firing, and a second fit including the Inline graphic points during the fast rise in fluorescence that follows ignition. The crossing point of the two lines provided the onset of firing.

The extension of this analysis to all the active neurons within a burst, and along all the bursts, finally provided the entire set of ignition sequences. The construction of the directed functional networks for the homogeneous cultures was then carried out identically as the clustered ones.

Additional control experiments

Recordings in clustered cultures typically lasted for Inline graphic h and contained between Inline graphic bursts in the quietest networks and Inline graphic bursts in the most active ones. To test whether Inline graphic bursts sufficed to draw the functional networks, we carried out a control experiment in which we monitored spontaneous activity along Inline graphic h in a standard clustered culture, measured at DIV Inline graphic and containing Inline graphic nodes (Figure S6). We then analyzed the data using two different procedures. In the first one we drew the functional connectivity using the data extracted from the entire recording, and determined its assortativity values. In the second procedure, we separated the recorded sequence in three blocks, each Inline graphic long, built the functional connectivity for each block, and computed the respective assortative values. The studied culture fired in a sustained manner at a rate of Inline graphic, and procured a total of Inline graphic bursts. Thus, each block typically contained about Inline graphic bursts.

The results (Figure S6) led to two major conclusions. First, that the functional connectivity is very similar among the blocks, and between any of the blocks and the entire recording, providing assortativity values that are compatible within statistical error. And second, that the first 40 min of recording (with 45 bursts only) sufficed to shape the major traits of the functional network, therefore validating our strategy of using Inline graphich of acquisition to procure a reliable estimate of the functional connectivity of the network and its assortative traits.

Network assortativity and rich-club

Here we describe the calculation of the assortativity coefficients, assortativity errors and the rich-club distributions. In the process, we have to define the assortativity for directed weighted networks.

Generalization of assortativity to directed weighted networks

Newman [23] defined assortativity Inline graphic as the Pearson correlation between the degrees of every pair Inline graphic of linked nodes in the network. More precisely, in the case of directed networks, if Inline graphic is the output degree from node Inline graphic, Inline graphic the input degree to node Inline graphic, and Inline graphic scans all the edges in the network, then

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e311.jpg (1)

where

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e312.jpg (2)
graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e313.jpg (3)
graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e314.jpg (4)

After some algebra, assortativity may also be written as

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e315.jpg (5)
graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e316.jpg (6)

This definition of assortativity is applicable to all kind of networks, either undirected, directed, weighted or unweighted. For weighted networks as in our case, the strength of the nodes carries important information about the structure of the network, and thus it would be useful to know the correlation between the strengths instead of the degrees. Since in this case each edge carries a weight, it seems logical that edges with higher weight should have a larger contribution to the correlation. Therefore, we define the weighted assortativity Inline graphic as the Pearson weighted correlation between the strengths of the nodes. In mathematical terms, if Inline graphic is the weight of the link from node Inline graphic to node Inline graphic (zero if there is no link), then Inline graphic and Inline graphic are the output and input strengths, then

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e323.jpg (7)

where

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e324.jpg (8)
graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e325.jpg (9)

with Inline graphic the total strength of the network, i.e.

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e327.jpg (10)

Litvak et al. [31] showed that in disassortative networks the magnitude of the standard assortativity decreases with network size, a problem that was solved by replacing the Pearson correlation Inline graphic with the Spearman correlation, thus obtaining a Spearman assortativity Inline graphic. Spearman rank correlation is calculated in the same way that the Pearson correlation but substituting the values (in this case, the degrees of the nodes) by their respective ranks, i.e. their position when the values are sorted in ascending order. This leads us to define the Spearman weighted assortativity Inline graphic as the Spearman weighted correlation between the strengths of the nodes at both ends of each edge in the network.

The estimation of the error in the assortativity value (for any of the previous four variants) can be computed in several ways, for instance through the jackknife method, the bootstrap algorithm, or by using the Fisher transformation [24], [71]. We used in our work the bootstrap algorithm, and considered Inline graphic random samples of the data.

Rich-club analysis

The rich-club analysis computes the degree-degree (or weight-weight) correlation distributions, and respect to a null case of non-correlated degrees (or weights). It allows us to reinforce the assortativity analysis presented before. Assortative mixing, in principle, will induce a rich-club effect that should be clearly detectable for a wide range of degrees (or weights). We will first introduce the formulation for the calculation of rich-club in weighted networks as presented in [51], and afterwards we will extend it to the case of weighted directed networks.

The rich-club score is calculated as follows:

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e332.jpg (11)

where Inline graphic is the rich-club score relative to the uncorrelated null case, Inline graphic is the sum of the weights of the links of the subgraph formed only by those nodes whose strengths are higher than Inline graphic,

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e336.jpg (12)

and Inline graphic is the corresponding value in the case of uncorrelated strengths,

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e338.jpg (13)

The term Inline graphic designates the subset of nodes Inline graphic such that Inline graphic. Inline graphic is the total number of nodes in the network, and Inline graphic and Inline graphic are the first and second moments of the strength distribution. The ratio Inline graphic is calculated for all values of Inline graphic, and ranges from the minimum value of strength in the network to the maximum. This ratio indicates the presence or absence of a rich-club in the network: a network shows a rich-club effect when the high values of Inline graphic give a ratio above 1.

To calculate this ratio on our functional networks, we have to consider that the network is not only weighted but also directed. Therefore, we need to adapt the former formulation for the case of weighted directed networks. For this reason we will consider the in- and out-strength of each node, expressed as Inline graphic and Inline graphic respectively. In the directed formulation, Eqs. (11) and (12) remain unchanged. However, Inline graphic must be redefined as

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e351.jpg (14)

and the term Inline graphic becomes

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e353.jpg (15)

where the averages are calculated as

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e354.jpg (16)

This formulation allows us to calculate the rich-club coefficient for weighted directed networks. Note that for an undirected network (where Inline graphic) the latter formulation reduces to the original one.

Alternative construction of the functional network based on mutual information

Mutual information [56], [72] is a particular case of the Kullback-Leibler divergence [73], an information-theoretic measure of the distance between two probability distributions. In fact, the mutual information between two stochastic variables Inline graphic and Inline graphic provides an estimation of the amount of information gained about Inline graphic when Inline graphic is known.

Let us indicate by Inline graphic the time series corresponding to the Inline graphic-th cluster, with Inline graphic and Inline graphic the total number of time frames involved in the observation process. The time series adopted for the successive analysis are obtained by mapping the observed train of cluster activations to another time series termed walk, defined by

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e364.jpg (17)

In the specific case of our analysis, the mutual information between two time series Inline graphic and Inline graphic, corresponding to two different clusters, is interpreted as the amount of correlation between the dynamics of cluster Inline graphic and Inline graphic. In general, the time scale of the correlation between two time series is not known a priori. Such a time scale corresponds to the time delay required to maximize the gain of information. Therefore, in the spirit of Fraser and Swinney [74], we define the time delayed mutual cross information between Inline graphic and Inline graphic by

graphic file with name pcbi.1003796.e371.jpg (18)

where Inline graphic and Inline graphic are indices running over some partition of the observed time series. In Eq. (18), Inline graphic indicates the probability to find a value of time series Inline graphic in the Inline graphic-th interval, Inline graphic is the probability to find a value of time series Inline graphic in the Inline graphic-th interval, whereas Inline graphic denotes the joint probability to observe a firing from the Inline graphic-th cluster falling in the Inline graphic-th interval and a firing from the Inline graphic-th cluster falling in the Inline graphic-th interval exactly Inline graphic time frames later.

For the sake of simplicity, in the following we will adopt the more concise notation Inline graphic to indicate the time delayed mutual cross information. Finally, in order to gain the highest amount of information about the dynamics of cluster Inline graphic by observing cluster Inline graphic, we consider only the maximum value Inline graphic of Inline graphic with respect to the time delay Inline graphic.

We estimate the importance of the observed amount of correlation by performing the above analysis on surrogate data. Surrogates adopted in this study are time series generated by randomly reshuffling the temporal observations of the firing series Inline graphic, for each cluster separately. Such a procedure destroys any correlation between pairs of time series while preserving the empirical probability distribution, thus allowing to test the null hypothesis that the observed correlation is obtained by chance.

We indicate by Inline graphic the walk corresponding to the surrogate obtained from time series Inline graphic and with Inline graphic the time delayed mutual cross information between Inline graphic and Inline graphic. We perform 200 independent random realizations of surrogates for each pair Inline graphic and we estimate the corresponding expected value Inline graphic of the maximum mutual cross-information, as well as the root mean square Inline graphic of the underlying distribution.

Hence, we fix a priori the significance Inline graphic of the hypothesis testing and we estimate the Inline graphic-score corresponding to each pair Inline graphic by Inline graphic. Therefore, the observed correlation between cluster Inline graphic and Inline graphic is said to be statistically significant if Inline graphic, where Inline graphic is the standard error function. Finally, we obtain the functional network of clusters by building the weight matrix Inline graphic whose elements are defined by Inline graphic if Inline graphic, and Inline graphic if Inline graphic.

Supporting Information

Figure S1

Homogeneous cultures. A The seeding of neurons in cover glasses previously coated with poly-l-lysine gives rise to neuronal cultures with a quasi-homogeneous distribution of neurons. A typical circular culture Inline graphic in diameter contains about Inline graphic neurons that can be well identified either as bright spots in the fluorescence recordings or as circular objects in bright-field images (small panel on the right). Spontaneous activity in these homogeneous cultures is typically recorded at Inline graphic frames/s, which suffices to extract the time delays between consecutive neuronal activations. The particular experiment shown here corresponds to network ‘P’ of the main text, with a total of Inline graphic neurons manually selected over the images and monitored along Inline graphic. The analysis of their spontaneous activity traces is analyzed in the context of our model, finally procuring the functional connectivity network and its topological properties. B Given the large number of nodes analyzed and the high average degree of the resulting network (Inline graphic functional connections per neuron), a representation of the complete functional network is unpractical. As an example of the obtained functional networks, we here show all the functional links within a small region placed in the center containing Inline graphic neurons. Connections are both color and thickness coded according to their weight. Nodes are color coded according to their strength. C As an alternative representation, we show here a Inline graphic of the population (Inline graphic neurons randomly chosen), each neuron showing the Inline graphic of its links. Nodes and links are color coded according to their strength and weight, respectively. D A ring graph of the same neurons shows that most of them display a similar connectivity, in contrast to the strong modularity and variability in connectivity exhibited by the clustered cultures.

(PDF)

Figure S2

Variance Inline graphic and culture properties. A The variance Inline graphic is obtained from the Gaussian fit of the activation delays between pairs of clusters. The plot shows that Inline graphic decreases with the culture age in vitro, indicating that young cultures display a slower dynamics (larger delay times and therefore larger variance) than mature cultures. B The variance Inline graphic increases with the number of clusters in the culture, indicating a broader and richer distribution of time delays as more clusters participate in the dynamics of the network. Errors bars in A show standard deviation.

(PDF)

Figure S3

Sensitivity of the functional network construction to the cut-off times. The cut-off determines the end of a sequence and therefore its variation modifies the set of burst chosen. The cut-off is set to Inline graphic for clustered cultures and Inline graphic for homogeneous ones. To assess the sensitivity of the grouping of bursts to the cut-off, we have computed the Variation of Information (VI) between the grouping of bursts at a certain cut-off value and the previous one. It is computed as Inline graphic, where Inline graphic and Inline graphic are two partitions, Inline graphic is the entropy and Inline graphic is the mutual information. In our case, each partition is the set of burst found at a certain value of the cut-off. We have screened the cut-off values from Inline graphic to Inline graphic. The analysis shows that Inline graphic for homogeneous and Inline graphic for clustered cultures are values for which the variation of information is already stabilized. Thus, the modification of the cut-off within the stabilization region does not change the grouping of clusters in each burst, and therefore the derived functional networks, as well as the corresponding network measures, remain the same.

(PDF)

Figure S4

Relation between the structural traits of the network and the functional connectivity. A Dependence of the weight of the functional connections on the width of physical connections between directly connected clusters. The sketch conceptually shows the comparison between structural and functional links. The plot represents the analysis of Inline graphic pairs of clusters, with data binned for similar widths. No significant correlation is observed. B The dependence of the node strength on cluster size shows no correlation, indicating that the functional connectivity cannot be assessed from the size of the clusters. Data is based on the analysis of Inline graphic clusters. C For the same clusters, this plot shows that the activity of a cluster is independent of its size. D Activity within a burst is always initiated by a particular cluster, which triggers the sequential activation of all the downstream clusters. To quantify the importance of these ‘initiators of activity’ in network dynamics we computed the number of times that a cluster of a given size initiates a sequence of activations. The plot shows that there is no a significant correlation between initiation and size. The analysis is based on the study of Inline graphic bursts. All these results indicate that the functional connectivity cannot be drawn from a visual inspection of the neuronal culture. Errors bars show standard deviation.

(PDF)

Figure S5

‘Rich-club’ analysis. The evaluation of the rich-club Inline graphic is performed by computing the ratio between the connectivity strength of highly connected nodes and its randomized counterpart, Inline graphic, and for gradually larger values of the strength threshold Inline graphic. The figure shows the rich-club analysis for 3 representative clustered and homogeneous cultures. Clustered networks exhibit values of Inline graphic systematically higher than Inline graphic for large values of the strength threshold Inline graphic, evidencing the existence of a rich-club core of highly connected clusters in the network. On the contrary, homogeneous cultures display a mixture of positive and negative values, and with an average around Inline graphic, ruling out the existence of the rich-club property.

(PDF)

Figure S6

Control experiment. A Bright field image of a clustered network whose spontaneous activity has been recorded for Inline graphic. The average bursting rate of the network is Inline graphic. B Corresponding functional network. The size of the nodes is proportional to the size of the actual clusters, and their color is proportional to their strength. The weights of the links are both color and thickness coded. The darker the color, the higher the value of the observable. C Analysis of the 2 h recording in three blocks, Inline graphic in duration each, and containing Inline graphic bursts. The blocks show very similar traits between them, as well as with the entire recording. The blocks exhibit similar assortativity values, and share both the most important links and nodes’ strengths. Inline graphic indicates the assortativity value of the depicted network, averaged over the Pearson and Spearman formulations.

(PDF)

Movie S1

Representative recording in clustered networks. The movie shows the spontaneous activity in the network depicted in Figure 1, showing the first Inline graphic of recorded activity. The original sequence was acquired at Inline graphic frames/s. It is reproduced here 15 times faster. The ignition of a cluster is revealed by a sharp increase in the fluorescence signal. Bursts of activity correspond to the ignition of Inline graphic clusters in a short time window. It is worth remarking the rich activity, the strong modularity, and the repetition of some sequences. Activity was driven solely by excitatory connectivity, with inhibition blocked with Inline graphic bicuculline.

(AVI)

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge S. Rüdiger and J. G. Orlandi for fruitful discussions and insight, and to E. Tibau for technical assistance.

Funding Statement

Research was supported by the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovaci\'on (Spain, www.idi.mineco.gob.es) under projects FIS2010-21924-C02-02, FIS2011-28820-C02-01, FIS2012-38266-C02-01, and FIS2010-09832-E. We also acknowledge the Generalitat de Catalunya (www10.gencat.cat/agaur_web) under projects 2009-SGR-00014 and 2009-SGR-838, and the EU FET (MULTIPLEX 317532). A.A. also acknowledges partial financial support from the ICREA Academia and the James S.\ McDonnell Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Dorogovtsev SN, Mendes JFF (2003) Evolution of Networks: From Biological Nets to the Internet and WWW. Oxford University Press.
  • 2. Alon U (2007) Network motifs: theory and experimental approaches. Nat Rev Genet 8: 450–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barrat A, Barthélemy M, Vespignani A (2008) Dynamical processes on complex networks. New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.
  • 4.Newman M (2010) Networks: an Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • 5.Estrada E (2011) The Structure of Complex Networks. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • 6. Eckmann JP, Feinerman O, Gruendlinger L, Moses E, Soriano J, et al. (2007) The physics of living neural networks. Phys Rep 449: 54–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Sun Y, Huang Z, Yang K, Liu W, Xie Y, et al. (2011) Self-organizing circuit assembly through spatiotemporally coordinated neuronal migration within geometric constraints. PLoS ONE 6: e28156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Gross GW, Kowalski JM (1999) Origins of activity patterns in self-organizing neuronal networks in vitro. Journal of Intelligent Material Systems and Structures 10: 558–564. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2009) Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10: 186–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Sporns O (2011) The human connectome: a complex network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1224: 109–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Sporns O, Chialvo DR, Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC (2004) Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 418–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Bassett DS, Wymbs NF, Porter MA, Mucha PJ, Carlson JM, et al. (2011) Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain networks during learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108: 7641–7646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Seeley WW, Crawford RK, Zhou J, Miller BL, Greicius MD (2009) Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron 62: 42–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Zhou J, Gennatas ED, Kramer JH, Miller BL, Seeley WW (2012) Predicting regional neurodegeneration from the healthy brain functional connectome. Neuron 73: 1216–1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Bonifazi P, Goldin M, Picardo MA, Jorquera I, Cattani A, et al. (2009) Gabaergic hub neurons orchestrate synchrony in developing hippocampal networks. Science 326: 1419–1424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Latora V, Marchiori M (2001) Efficient behavior of small-world networks. Phys Rev Lett 87: 198701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Watts D, Strogatz S (1998) Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks. Nature 393: 440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Sporns O, Zwi J (2004) The small world of the cerebral cortex. Neuroinformatics 2: 145–162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Harriger L, van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O (2012) Rich club organization of macaque cerebral cortex and its role in network communication. PLoS ONE 7: e46497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, et al. (2008) Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol 6: e159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Meunier D, Lambiotte R, Bullmore ET (2010) Modular and hierarchically modular organization of brain networks. Frontiers in Neuroscience 4: 200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Zamora-López G, Zhou C, Kurths J (2010) Cortical hubs form a module for multisensory integration on top of the hierarchy of cortical networks. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics 4: 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Newman M (2002) Assortative mixing in networks. Phys Rev Lett 89: 208701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Newman M (2003) Mixing patterns in networks. Phys Rev E 67: 26126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Eguíluz VM, Chialvo DR, Cecchi GA, Baliki M, Apkarian AV (2005) Scale-free brain functional networks. Phys Rev Lett 94: 018102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Avalos-Gaytán V, Almendral JA, Papo D, Schaeffer SE, Boccaletti S (2012) Assortative and modular networks are shaped by adaptive synchronization processes. Phys Rev E 86: 015101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. de Franciscis S, Johnson S, Torres JJ (2011) Enhancing neural-network performance via assortativity. Phys Rev E 83: 036114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Rubinov M, Sporns O (2010) Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage 52: 1059–1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Achard S, Salvador R, Whitcher B, Suckling J, Bullmore E (2006) A resilient, low-frequency, small-world human brain functional network with highly connected association cortical hubs. The Journal of Neuroscience 26: 63–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Colizza V, Flammini A, Serrano MA, Vespignani A (2006) Detecting rich-club ordering in complex networks. Nature physics 2: 110–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Litvak N, van der Hofstad R (2013) Uncovering disassortativity in large scale-free networks. Phys Rev E 87: 022801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Leung C, Chau H (2007) Weighted assortative and disassortative networks model. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 378: 591–602. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Wheeler B, Brewer G (2010) Designing neural networks in culture. Proceedings of the IEEE 98: 398–406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Wagenaar DA, Pine J, Potter SM (2006) An extremely rich repertoire of bursting patterns during the development of cortical cultures. BMC Neurosci 7: 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Cohen E, Ivenshitz M, Amor-Baroukh V, Greenberger V, Segal M (2008) Determinants of spontaneous activity in networks of cultured hippocampus. Brain Research 1235: 21–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Orlandi JG, Soriano J, Alvarez-Lacalle E, Teller S, Casademunt J (2013) Noise focusing and the emergence of coherent activity in neuronal cultures. Nature Physics 9: 582–590. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Tibau E, Valencia M, Soriano J (2013) Identification of neuronal network properties from the spectral analysis of calcium imaging signals in neuronal cultures. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 7: 199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Teller S, Soriano J (2013) Experiments on clustered neuronal networks. AIP Conference Proceedings 1510: 244–246. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Gabay T, Jakobs E, Ben-Jacob E, Hanein Y (2005) Engineered self-organization of neural networks using carbon nanotube clusters. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 350: 611–621. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Segev R, Benveniste M, Shapira Y, Ben-Jacob E (2003) Formation of electrically active clusterized neural networks. Phys Rev Lett 90: 168101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Shein Idelson M, Ben-Jacob E, Hanein Y (2010) Innate synchronous oscillations in freely-organized small neuronal circuits. PLoS ONE 5: e14443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Soussou W, Yoon G, Brinton R, Berger T (2007) Neuronal network morphology and electrophysiologyof hippocampal neurons cultured on surface-treated multielectrode arrays. Biomedical Engineering, IEEE Transactions on 54: 1309–1320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Sorkin R, Gabay T, Blinder P, Baranes D, Ben-Jacob E, et al. (2006) Compact self-wiring in cultured neural networks. Journal of Neural Engineering 3: 95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Macis E, Tedesco M, Massobrio P, Raiteri R, Martinoia S (2007) An automated microdrop delivery system for neuronal network patterning on microelectrode arrays. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 161: 88–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Shein M, Greenbaum A, Gabay T, Sorkin R, David-Pur M, et al. (2009) Engineered neuronal circuits shaped and interfaced with carbon nanotube microelectrode arrays. Biomedical Microdevices 11: 495–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Shein-Idelson M, Ben-Jacob E, Hanein Y (2011) Engineered neuronal circuits: A new platform for studying the role of modular topology. Frontiers in Neuroengineering 4: 10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Tsai CY, Chang MC, I L (2008) Robustness and Variability of Pathways in the Spontaneous Synchronous Bursting of Clusterized Cortical Neuronal Networks In vitro. Journal of the Physical Society of Japan 77: 084803. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Yvon C, Rubli R, Streit J (2005) Patterns of spontaneous activity in unstructured and minimally structured spinal networks in culture. Experimental Brain Research 165: 139–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Berdondini L, Chiappalone M, van der Wal P, Imfeld K, de Rooij N, et al. (2006) A microelectrode array (mea) integrated with clustering structures for investigating in vitro neurodynamics in confined interconnected sub-populations of neurons. Sensors and Actuators B: Chemical 114: 530–541. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Zlatic V, Bianconi G, Díaz-Guilera A, Garlaschelli D, Rao F, et al. (2009) On the rich-club effect in dense and weighted networks. The European Physical Journal B 67: 271–275. [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Serrano MA (2008) Rich-club vs rich-multipolarization phenomena in weighted networks. Phys Rev E 78: 026101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. de Santos-Sierra D, Sendiña Nadal I, Leyva I, Almendral JA, Anava S, et al. (2014) Emergence of small-world anatomical networks in self-organizing clustered neuronal cultures. PLoS ONE 9: e85828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Shefi O, Golding I, Segev R, Ben-Jacob E, Ayali A (2002) Morphological characterization of in vitro neuronal networks. Physical Review E 66: 021905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Meilă M (2003) Comparing clusterings by the variation of information. In: Schölkopf B, Warmuth MK, editors, Learning Theory and Kernel Machines, Springer Berlin Heidelberg, volume 2777 of Lecture Notes in Computer Science. pp. 173–187. [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Bettencourt LMA, Stephens GJ, Ham MI, Gross GW (2007) Functional structure of cortical neuronal networks grown in vitro . Phys Rev E 75: 021915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Garofalo M, Nieus T, Massobrio P, Martinoia S (2009) Evaluation of the performance of information theory-based methods and cross-correlation to estimate the functional connectivity in cortical networks. PLoS ONE 4: e6482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Stetter O, Battaglia D, Soriano J, Geisel T (2012) Model-Free Reconstruction of Excitatory Neuronal Connectivity from Calcium Imaging Signals. PLoS Comput Biol 8: e1002653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Orlandi JG, Stetter O, Soriano J, Geisel T, Battaglia D (2014) Transfer entropy reconstruction and labeling of neuronal connections from simulated calcium imaging. PLoS ONE 9: e98842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR (2010) Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12: 43–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran JP, et al. (2009) Predicting human restingstate functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 2035–2040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Honey CJ, Kötter R, Breakspear M, Sporns O (2007) Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 10240–10245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Schmeltzer C, Soriano J, Sokolov IM, Rüdiger S (2014) Percolation of spatially constrained erdösrényi networks with degree correlations. Phys Rev E 89: 012116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Barthélemy M (2011) Spatial networks. Physics Reports 499: 1–101. [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2012) The economy of brain network organization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13: 336–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Vértes PE, Alexander-Bloch AF, Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL, et al. (2012) Simple models of human brain functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 5868–5873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Segal M, Manor D (1992) Confocal microscopic imaging of [Ca2+]i in cultured rat hippocampal neurons following exposure to n-methyl-d-aspartate. The Journal of Physiology 448: 655–676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Takahashi N, Takahara Y, Ishikawa D, Matsuki N, Ikegaya Y (2010) Functional multineuron calcium imaging for systems pharmacology. Analytical and Bioanalytical Chemistry 398: 211–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Grienberger C, Konnerth A (2012) Imaging calcium in neurons. Neuron 73: 862–885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Soriano J, Rodríguez Martínez M, Tlusty T, Moses E (2008) Development of input connections in neural cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 13758–13763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Ganguly K, Schinder AF, Wong ST, ming Poo M (2001) GABA itself promotes the developmental switch of neuronal GABAergic responses from excitation to inhibition. Cell 105: 521–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Efron B (1979) Computers and the theory of statistics: Thinking the unthinkable. SIAM Review 21 : pp. 460–480. [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Singh A, Lesica NA (2010) Incremental mutual information: A new method for characterizing the strength and dynamics of connections in neuronal circuits. PLoS Comput Biol 6: e1001035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Kullback S, Leibler RA (1951) On information and sufficiency. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics 22: 79–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Fraser AM, Swinney HL (1986) Independent coordinates for strange attractors from mutual information. Physical review A 33: 1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure S1

Homogeneous cultures. A The seeding of neurons in cover glasses previously coated with poly-l-lysine gives rise to neuronal cultures with a quasi-homogeneous distribution of neurons. A typical circular culture Inline graphic in diameter contains about Inline graphic neurons that can be well identified either as bright spots in the fluorescence recordings or as circular objects in bright-field images (small panel on the right). Spontaneous activity in these homogeneous cultures is typically recorded at Inline graphic frames/s, which suffices to extract the time delays between consecutive neuronal activations. The particular experiment shown here corresponds to network ‘P’ of the main text, with a total of Inline graphic neurons manually selected over the images and monitored along Inline graphic. The analysis of their spontaneous activity traces is analyzed in the context of our model, finally procuring the functional connectivity network and its topological properties. B Given the large number of nodes analyzed and the high average degree of the resulting network (Inline graphic functional connections per neuron), a representation of the complete functional network is unpractical. As an example of the obtained functional networks, we here show all the functional links within a small region placed in the center containing Inline graphic neurons. Connections are both color and thickness coded according to their weight. Nodes are color coded according to their strength. C As an alternative representation, we show here a Inline graphic of the population (Inline graphic neurons randomly chosen), each neuron showing the Inline graphic of its links. Nodes and links are color coded according to their strength and weight, respectively. D A ring graph of the same neurons shows that most of them display a similar connectivity, in contrast to the strong modularity and variability in connectivity exhibited by the clustered cultures.

(PDF)

Figure S2

Variance Inline graphic and culture properties. A The variance Inline graphic is obtained from the Gaussian fit of the activation delays between pairs of clusters. The plot shows that Inline graphic decreases with the culture age in vitro, indicating that young cultures display a slower dynamics (larger delay times and therefore larger variance) than mature cultures. B The variance Inline graphic increases with the number of clusters in the culture, indicating a broader and richer distribution of time delays as more clusters participate in the dynamics of the network. Errors bars in A show standard deviation.

(PDF)

Figure S3

Sensitivity of the functional network construction to the cut-off times. The cut-off determines the end of a sequence and therefore its variation modifies the set of burst chosen. The cut-off is set to Inline graphic for clustered cultures and Inline graphic for homogeneous ones. To assess the sensitivity of the grouping of bursts to the cut-off, we have computed the Variation of Information (VI) between the grouping of bursts at a certain cut-off value and the previous one. It is computed as Inline graphic, where Inline graphic and Inline graphic are two partitions, Inline graphic is the entropy and Inline graphic is the mutual information. In our case, each partition is the set of burst found at a certain value of the cut-off. We have screened the cut-off values from Inline graphic to Inline graphic. The analysis shows that Inline graphic for homogeneous and Inline graphic for clustered cultures are values for which the variation of information is already stabilized. Thus, the modification of the cut-off within the stabilization region does not change the grouping of clusters in each burst, and therefore the derived functional networks, as well as the corresponding network measures, remain the same.

(PDF)

Figure S4

Relation between the structural traits of the network and the functional connectivity. A Dependence of the weight of the functional connections on the width of physical connections between directly connected clusters. The sketch conceptually shows the comparison between structural and functional links. The plot represents the analysis of Inline graphic pairs of clusters, with data binned for similar widths. No significant correlation is observed. B The dependence of the node strength on cluster size shows no correlation, indicating that the functional connectivity cannot be assessed from the size of the clusters. Data is based on the analysis of Inline graphic clusters. C For the same clusters, this plot shows that the activity of a cluster is independent of its size. D Activity within a burst is always initiated by a particular cluster, which triggers the sequential activation of all the downstream clusters. To quantify the importance of these ‘initiators of activity’ in network dynamics we computed the number of times that a cluster of a given size initiates a sequence of activations. The plot shows that there is no a significant correlation between initiation and size. The analysis is based on the study of Inline graphic bursts. All these results indicate that the functional connectivity cannot be drawn from a visual inspection of the neuronal culture. Errors bars show standard deviation.

(PDF)

Figure S5

‘Rich-club’ analysis. The evaluation of the rich-club Inline graphic is performed by computing the ratio between the connectivity strength of highly connected nodes and its randomized counterpart, Inline graphic, and for gradually larger values of the strength threshold Inline graphic. The figure shows the rich-club analysis for 3 representative clustered and homogeneous cultures. Clustered networks exhibit values of Inline graphic systematically higher than Inline graphic for large values of the strength threshold Inline graphic, evidencing the existence of a rich-club core of highly connected clusters in the network. On the contrary, homogeneous cultures display a mixture of positive and negative values, and with an average around Inline graphic, ruling out the existence of the rich-club property.

(PDF)

Figure S6

Control experiment. A Bright field image of a clustered network whose spontaneous activity has been recorded for Inline graphic. The average bursting rate of the network is Inline graphic. B Corresponding functional network. The size of the nodes is proportional to the size of the actual clusters, and their color is proportional to their strength. The weights of the links are both color and thickness coded. The darker the color, the higher the value of the observable. C Analysis of the 2 h recording in three blocks, Inline graphic in duration each, and containing Inline graphic bursts. The blocks show very similar traits between them, as well as with the entire recording. The blocks exhibit similar assortativity values, and share both the most important links and nodes’ strengths. Inline graphic indicates the assortativity value of the depicted network, averaged over the Pearson and Spearman formulations.

(PDF)

Movie S1

Representative recording in clustered networks. The movie shows the spontaneous activity in the network depicted in Figure 1, showing the first Inline graphic of recorded activity. The original sequence was acquired at Inline graphic frames/s. It is reproduced here 15 times faster. The ignition of a cluster is revealed by a sharp increase in the fluorescence signal. Bursts of activity correspond to the ignition of Inline graphic clusters in a short time window. It is worth remarking the rich activity, the strong modularity, and the repetition of some sequences. Activity was driven solely by excitatory connectivity, with inhibition blocked with Inline graphic bicuculline.

(AVI)


Articles from PLoS Computational Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES