Summary
Recent studies of behavioral choice support the notion that the decision to carry out one behavior rather than another depends on the reconfiguration of shared interneuronal networks [1]. We investigated another decision-making strategy, derived from the classical ethological literature [2, 3], which proposes that behavioral choice depends on competition between autonomous networks. According to this model, behavioral choice depends on inhibitory interactions between incompatible hierarchically organized behaviors. We provide evidence for this by investigating the interneuronal mechanisms mediating behavioral choice between two autonomous circuits that underlie whole-body withdrawal [4, 5] and feeding [6] in the pond snail Lymnaea. Whole-body withdrawal is a defensive reflex that is initiated by tactile contact with predators. As predicted by the hierarchical model, tactile stimuli that evoke whole-body withdrawal responses also inhibit ongoing feeding in the presence of feeding stimuli. By recording neurons from the feeding and withdrawal networks, we found no direct synaptic connections between the interneuronal and motoneuronal elements that generate the two behaviors. Instead, we discovered that behavioral choice depends on the interaction between two unique types of interneurons with asymmetrical synaptic connectivity that allows withdrawal to override feeding. One type of interneuron, the Pleuro-Buccal (PlB), is an extrinsic modulatory neuron of the feeding network that completely inhibits feeding when excited by touch-induced monosynaptic input from the second type of interneuron, Pedal-Dorsal12 (PeD12). PeD12 plays a critical role in behavioral choice by providing a synaptic pathway joining the two behavioral networks that underlies the competitive dominance of whole-body withdrawal over feeding.
Graphical Abstract
Highlights
-
•
Behavioral choice between mutually exclusive behaviors is hierarchically organized
-
•
Touch-induced whole-body withdrawal inhibits sucrose-driven feeding rhythms
-
•
Two interneurons with asymmetrical connectivity allow withdrawal to override feeding
-
•
Suppression of feeding is due to the enhancement of tonic inhibition
A current model of behavioral choice depends on the reconfiguration of shared interneuronal networks. Pirger et al. provide evidence for the alternative Tinbergen model, which depends on a hierarchically based competition between autonomous networks. An asymmetrical inhibitory interneuronal pathway allows one behavior to dominate the other.
Results
As predicted by the Tinbergen hierarchical model of behavioral choice [2], tactile stimuli that evoke whole-body withdrawal responses in Lymnaea significantly inhibit feeding even in the presence of a strong feeding stimulus (Figure 1). It should be noted that the animals (n = 16) were starved for 2 days so that they could be maximally sensitive to food stimuli.
To investigate the interneuronal mechanisms underlying this behavioral choice, we developed a semi-intact preparation (Figure 2A) that allowed the application of tactile and sucrose stimuli to the lips while recording identified neurons from the feeding and withdrawal networks [7, 8]. We discovered that behavioral choice depends on the interaction between two unique types of interneurons. One type, the Pleuro-Buccal (PlB), is a well-known extrinsic feeding modulatory interneuron that has extensive inhibitory synaptic connections with interneurons and motoneurons of the feeding network [9]. The second type, Pedal-Dorsal12 (PeD12), is a newly discovered interneuron (Figures 2A and S1A available online) that plays a critical role in behavioral choice by providing the synaptic pathway that underlies the competitive interaction between the otherwise autonomous feeding and withdrawal-response networks.
Stimulation of PeD12 activated whole-body withdrawal and simultaneously inhibited rhythmic feeding movements (example in Figure 2B; n = 6). A burst of spikes (Figure 2B) artificially evoked in PeD12 by current injection resulted in a single large contraction of the columellar muscle, which is known to cause touch-induced whole-body withdrawal responses [4]. The same touch inhibited sucrose-driven rhythmic feeding movements of the buccal mass (feeding apparatus [10]). To understand how PeD12 might affect these alternative behaviors, we investigated the interactions of PeD12 with neurons of the withdrawal and feeding networks.
First, we asked how PeD12 drives withdrawal. We found that PeD12 is electrotonically coupled to motoneurons of the withdrawal-response network, and this plays a critical role in causing touch-induced withdrawal. A hyperpolarizing current pulse applied to PeD12 produced corresponding changes in membrane potentials of corecorded withdrawal motoneurons (Figure 2C1) that were located in several different ganglia of the CNS (Figure 2A). In the same preparation, application of lip touch caused a burst of spikes in PeD12 and motoneurons (Figure 2C2). Due to the extensive electrotonic connectivity of PeD12 with the withdrawal-response network, a current-induced burst of spikes in PeD12 depolarized the motoneurons and induced spiking (Figure 2C3) similar to that produced by touch. No other member of the withdrawal circuit was capable of eliciting withdrawal alone [5, 11]. It therefore seems reasonable to conclude that this behavioral response to touch results from a combination of distributed sensory input to all members of the withdrawal network [5] and the strong electrotonically mediated excitatory effects of PeD12 (Figure 2C3).
Next, we asked whether touch-induced burst responses in PeD12 are necessary for the touch-induced suppression of feeding in a sucrose-driven rhythm. Data supporting this necessity were obtained by recording PeD12 together with neurons of the feeding circuit, such as the B3 and B4 motoneurons (Figure 2A). By recording these motoneurons, we were able to monitor sucrose-driven “fictive feeding” activity, an in vitro correlate of behavioral feeding in the intact animal [10]. Motoneuronal bursts in response to sucrose were driven by synaptic inputs from the feeding central pattern generator (Figure 2D1, expanded trace). PeD12 was normally silent (mean resting potential −75 ± 2.3 mV, n = 28), but experiments (n = 12) of the type shown in Figures 2D1 and 2D4 showed that PeD12 was strongly activated by touch. This was accompanied by a significant inhibition of the fictive feeding rhythm recorded in the B3 feeding motoneuron. An artificially induced burst of spikes in PeD12 had the same effect (Figure 2D2). There was no statistical difference in the inhibitory effect on feeding between these two methods of spike activation (Figure 2D3). To determine whether PeD12 is necessary for feeding inhibition, we compared the effects of touch with (Figure 2D5) and without (Figure 2D4) suppression of touch-induced PeD12 spikes. Statistical analysis showed that preventing PeD12 spikes by hyperpolarization removed the inhibition of fictive feeding by touch (Figure 2D6). We conclude that the touch-induced spiking of PeD12 is necessary for inhibition of feeding.
We compared the effects of touch on feeding in the semi-intact preparation in which feeding was monitored in vitro with the behavioral experiments using the same stimuli (Figure 1). There was no significant difference in the inhibitory effects of touch in the two types of experiments, justifying the use of the in vitro preparations for the neural analysis of the Tinbergen choice mechanism (mean difference scores: behavioral, −3.1 ± 0.3, n = 16; in vitro, −2.5 ± 0.2, n = 17; Mann-Whitney test, U = 87, p = 0.07).
It was important to find out how PeD12 inhibited feeding because it was key to understanding how the two behavioral networks interacted. We found that there were no direct synaptic connections from PeD12 to neurons of the feeding network (Figure S2A). Instead, we showed that a PeD12 to PlB synaptic pathway mediated PeD12 inhibition of feeding, with PlB being the primary agent for feeding suppression. Evidence that PeD12 inhibited feeding via the PlB interneuron was obtained by corecording PeD12 and PlB and artificially manipulating their spike activity during a sucrose-driven rhythm (n = 3). Evoking a burst of spikes in PeD12 excited PlB, and this resulted in inhibition of feeding cycles recorded in the B3 motoneuron (Figure 3A1). Suppressing PlB activity by hyperpolarization prevented this inhibition (Figure 3A2), so PeD12 must have been acting via PlB. The ability of PlB alone to suppress feeding activity is shown in Figure 3A3, where a burst of spikes in PlB inhibited feeding in the absence of spike activity in PeD12.
These experiments suggest that PeD12 has an excitatory synaptic connection with PlB, and this was confirmed by showing that an artificially evoked burst of spikes in PeD12 drives an increase in the firing rate of PlB (Figure 3B1, left; n = 11). This connection was asymmetrical because there was no evidence of a corresponding synaptic connection from PlB to PeD12 (e.g., Figure 3B1, right) in the same preparation. More detailed experiments suggested that the PeD12-PlB synapse was chemically mediated and monosynaptic. Thus, calcium was required for transmission (n = 10) (Figure 3B2), and high concentrations of the divalent cations calcium and magnesium (Hi-Di saline), which blocked polysynaptic pathways [12, 13], did not block synaptic transmission (Figure 3B3, left; n = 8). When PlB spikes were suppressed by hyperpolarization in the same Hi-Di experiments, a slow depolarizing synaptic response was revealed (Figure 3B3, middle). Repeated triggering of single PeD12 spikes on a faster time base revealed the presence of short-latency 1:1 excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) on PlB (Figure 3B3, right), also consistent with a monosynaptic connection. Dye-filling experiments revealed the sites of potential synaptic contacts between the two neurons (Figure S1). The arborization of PeD12 (Figure S1A1, red) and PlB (Figure S1A1, green) indicated two areas where the neurites intertwined. These were potential sites of the synaptic interactions. One of these areas was close to the cell body of the PeD12 cell (Figure S1A2), and the other was close to the PlB cell body (Figure S1A3). Together, these experiments provide evidence for a monosynaptic chemical pathway between PeD12 and PlB (Figure 3B4).
To validate the role of the PeD12-PlB synaptic pathway in behavioral choice, we had to show that PlB inhibits a sucrose-driven feeding rhythm. Although interneuron PlB inhibits feeding behavior [9], little is known about its sensory inputs [14], and in particular, about whether its response to strong tactile inputs is sufficient to suppress feeding rhythms. PlB fired tonically during sucrose application, but its baseline activity was insufficient to inhibit fictive feeding. A single touch stimulus produced a maintained depolarization of PlB and an increase in tonic firing (Figure 3C1). These touch-induced increases in PlB tonic firing rate resulted in an inhibition of the fictive feeding rhythm (Figures 3C1 and 3B1; n = 11). Similar inhibition was produced by an artificially evoked burst of spikes in PlB (Figure 3C2), indicating that increased firing in PlB was sufficient to suppress sucrose-induced feeding. A statistical comparison of the effects of touch versus the depolarization of PlB found that there was no difference in the inhibition of the fictive feeding responses produced by the two types of stimulation (Figure 3C3). The necessity for the touch-induced increase in firing of PlB for feeding inhibition was tested. PlB was hyperpolarized, and the effects of touch on fictive feeding were compared with (Figure 3D2) and without (Figure 3D1) hyperpolarization. There was a significantly smaller difference score in the hyperpolarized state (Figure 3D3). These results show that the increase in tonic firing in PlB induced by touch is both sufficient and necessary for the inhibition of feeding.
Finally, we showed that there were no synaptic connections between PlB and motoneurons of the withdrawal-response network (Figure S2B); therefore, PlB has no role in the control of whole-body withdrawal responses.
Discussion
Our results conform to the competitive model for behavioral selection originating in the ethological literature [2, 3] and provide an interneuronal mechanism for it. We propose that behavioral choice in response to conflicting sensory inputs depends on inhibitory synaptic interactions between autonomous networks that control incompatible behaviors. Extensive electrophysiological and anatomical investigations ([4, 5, 6]; Figure S2) show that the feeding and whole-body withdrawal circuits operate as autonomous units, consistent with the Tinbergen model. This type of inhibitory interaction between autonomous networks was suggested to occur in the mollusk Pleurobranchaea to explain the “dominance” of feeding over withdrawal [15, 16]. More recent studies [17, 18] have described the mechanism that mediates another type of competitive behavioral interaction in the same animal. Here, the dominance of swimming over feeding was shown to involve the asymmetrical synaptic inhibition of the feeding central pattern generator (CPG) circuit by a CPG interneuron from the swim circuit [17]. This differs from our example, where interneurons extrinsic to the feeding network are involved (Figure 4). The switch from feeding to defensive withdrawal is mediated by two identified interneurons with asymmetrical synaptic connectivity that allows withdrawal to always override feeding. One of these interneurons (PlB) completely inhibits the feeding rhythm when it is excited by the second of the two neurons (PeD12). Crucially, PeD12 also plays an important role in driving the whole-body withdrawal behavior, and it is responsive to strong tactile stimuli that evoke the defensive behavior. This pivotal neuron therefore has a dual function: in response to a strong aversive stimulus, it simultaneously activates the withdrawal motor circuit, acting as an extrinsic modulatory interneuron, and monosynaptically excites the PlB, which enhances tonic inhibition to the feeding motor circuit to suppress feeding. Thus, this simple asymmetric circuit joins the two motor networks and underlies the dominance of defensive withdrawal. By activating PeD12, the animal can simultaneously shut down grazing and initiate whole-body defensive withdrawal.
Defensive withdrawal of the whole animal is known to be at the top of the behavioral choice hierarchy in Lymnaea [19], so it would be expected that the inhibitory connection is asymmetrical to achieve this dominance. This differs from the original Tinbergen model, where reciprocal inhibition was proposed to prevent two behaviors being coexpressed. The reciprocal inhibition model is more likely to occur when inhibitory interactions between two autonomous behaviors require a more flexible relationship [20]. With high-value defensive behaviors, it is imperative that they override all other behaviors to prevent predation so that asymmetrical inhibitory interactions are present rather than reciprocal inhibition. In our experiments, we used starved animals to increase their responsiveness to food, but, despite this manipulation, food-driven feeding rhythms were still inhibited by touch. Tinbergen [2] considered that behavioral state would be an important determinant of behavioral selection, but our data suggest that aversive sensory stimulation triggering life-preserving behavioral responses overrides the effects of behavioral state.
Our Lymnaea example of behavioral choice is fundamentally different to other systems where the alternative behaviors share elements of one another’s circuits. Here, behavioral choice depends on overlapping combinations of interneurons that are active during different behaviors. In the leech (Hirudo), for example, the selection of one of four different behaviors (swimming, shortening, crawling, or bending) depends on a unique combination of firing in the same interneurons [21].
A key feature of all those systems that involve network configuration is that similar groups of muscles and motoneurons are used in various combinations, so the units of motor control are not unique to a particular behavior. The Tinbergen model occurs when the elements of motor control are autonomous, and selection depends on hierarchically based control mechanisms, where behaviors are selected by inhibition of less-valued behaviors.
In conclusion, two distinctly different models have been proposed to explain how switching between behaviors is achieved at the level of neuronal networks. The selection of different combinations of active neurons in shared wider networks determines which of a limited subset of behaviors is expressed where there is significant overlap in the neuronal machinery and muscles controlling more than one behavior. The inhibition of one circuit by another determines the behavioral outcome when the choice is made between behaviors controlled by dedicated nonoverlapping networks. Our example is an interesting case of the second model. Moreover, it provides insight into the cellular and synaptic details of the way inhibition mediates behavioral choice. For example, our results suggest that there is an important role for the modulation of tonic inhibition in explaining the hierarchical coupling between behavioral responses to aversive and rewarding sensory stimuli. We therefore suggest that the regulation of tonic inhibition by interneurons constitutes a common mechanism that is central to adaptive behavioral switching in other systems [22, 23].
Experimental Procedures
Experimental Animals
Animals from a laboratory-bred stock of Lymnaea stagnalis were used in the experiment. Details of their maintenance are described in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures.
Behavior
Animals were starved for 2 days before the experiments. Sucrose-driven feeding activity was initiated by perfusion of 0.02 mM sucrose. Von Frey hairs (4 g) were used to induce whole-body withdrawal. The procedure was video recorded and analyzed using ImageJ software. Feeding scores were calculated by subtracting the number of feeding cycles in the 20 s after the touch from the number of cycles in the 20 s before.
Preparations
Experiments were performed on semi-intact preparations containing the entire CNS and attached lips and tentacles (Figure 2A) [8, 24, 25, 26]. A modified semi-intact preparation, containing the main feeding muscle (buccal mass) and the columellar muscle, responsible for the whole-body withdrawal was also used to measure contractions induced by neuronal stimulation. A detailed description of preparations, stimulation and recording protocols, explanation of choice of neurons recorded, and data analysis methods are described in the Supplemental Experimental Procedures.
Author Contributions
Z.P. and M.C. carried out the electrophysiological experiments with the assistance of Z.L. M.C. carried out the behavioral experiments. S.N. did the confocal microscopy. G.K. and M.O. were involved in planning the experiments, discussing the results, and writing the manuscript. P.R.B. and I.K. each played a major role in the design of the experiments, analysis of the data, and production of the manuscript.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council for supporting our work.
Published: August 21, 2014
Footnotes
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).
Supplemental Information includes Supplemental Experimental Procedures and two figures and can be found with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.07.044.
Supplemental Information
References
- 1.Kristan W.B. Neuronal decision-making circuits. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:R928–R932. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Tinbergen N. Oxford University Press; New York: 1951. The Study of Instinct. [Google Scholar]
- 3.McCleery R. In: Animal Behaviour. Slater P.J.B., Halliday T.R., editors. Blackwell; Oxford: 1983. Interactions between activities; pp. 132–165. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ferguson G.P., Benjamin P.R. The whole-body withdrawal response of Lymnaea stagnalis. II. activation of central motoneurones and muscles by sensory input. J. Exp. Biol. 1991;158:97–116. doi: 10.1242/jeb.158.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ferguson G.P., Benjamin P.R. The whole-body withdrawal response of Lymnaea stagnalis. I. identification of central motoneurones and muscles. J. Exp. Biol. 1991;158:63–95. doi: 10.1242/jeb.158.1.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Benjamin P.R. Distributed network organization underlying feeding behavior in the mollusk Lymnaea. Neural Syst. Circuits. 2012;2:4. doi: 10.1186/2042-1001-2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kemenes I., Straub V.A., Nikitin E.S., Staras K., O’Shea M., Kemenes G., Benjamin P.R. Role of delayed nonsynaptic neuronal plasticity in long-term associative memory. Curr. Biol. 2006;16:1269–1279. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Marra V., O’Shea M., Benjamin P.R., Kemenes I. Susceptibility of memory consolidation during lapses in recall. Nat. Commun. 2013;4:1578. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Alania M., Sakharov D.A., Elliott C.J. Multilevel inhibition of feeding by a peptidergic pleural interneuron in the mollusc Lymnaea stagnalis. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 2004;190:379–390. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0503-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rose R.M., Benjamin P.R. The relationship of the central motor pattern to the feeding cycle of Lymnaea stagnalis. J. Exp. Biol. 1979;80:137–163. doi: 10.1242/jeb.80.1.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Syed N.I., Winlow W. Coordination of locomotor and cardiorespiratory networks of Lymnaea stagnalis by a pair of identified interneurones. J. Exp. Biol. 1991;158:37–62. doi: 10.1242/jeb.158.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Getting P.A. Mechanisms of pattern generation underlying swimming in Tritonia. I. neuronal network formed by monosynaptic connections. J. Neurophysiol. 1981;46:65–79. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Elliott C.J., Benjamin P.R. Esophageal mechanoreceptors in the feeding system of the pond snail, Lymnaea stagnalis. J. Neurophysiol. 1989;61:727–736. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.4.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Alania M., Dyakonova V., Sakharov D.A. Hyperpolarization by glucose of feeding-related neurons in snail. Acta Biol. Hung. 2004;55:195–200. doi: 10.1556/ABiol.55.2004.1-4.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kovac M.P., Davis W.J. Behavioral choice: neural mechanisms in Pleurobranchaea. Science. 1977;198:632–634. doi: 10.1126/science.918659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kovac M.P., Davis W.J. Neural mechanism underlying behavioral choice in Pleurobranchaea. J. Neurophysiol. 1980;43:469–487. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.2.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jing J., Gillette R. Neuronal elements that mediate escape swimming and suppress feeding behavior in the predatory sea slug Pleurobranchaea. J. Neurophysiol. 1995;74:1900–1910. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.5.1900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jing J., Gillette R. Escape swim network interneurons have diverse roles in behavioral switching and putative arousal in Pleurobranchaea. J. Neurophysiol. 2000;83:1346–1355. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.3.1346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Winlow W., Moroz L.L., Syed N.I. In: Neurobiology of Motor Programme Selection. Kien J., McCrohan C.R., Winlow W., editors. Pergamon Press; Oxford: 1992. Mechanisms of behavioural selection in Lymnaea stagnalis; pp. 52–72. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Anderson D.J. Optogenetics, sex, and violence in the brain: implications for psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71:1081–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kristan W.B., Gillette R. In: Invertebrate Neurobiology. North G., Greenspan R.J., editors. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; Cold Spring Harbor: 2007. Behavioural choice; pp. 533–553. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Grillner S., Hellgren J., Ménard A., Saitoh K., Wikström M.A. Mechanisms for selection of basic motor programs—roles for the striatum and pallidum. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:364–370. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Benjamin P.R., Staras K., Kemenes G. What roles do tonic inhibition and disinhibition play in the control of motor programs? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2010;4:30. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kemenes G., Staras K., Benjamin P.R. In vitro appetitive classical conditioning of the feeding response in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;78:2351–2362. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.5.2351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Staras K., Kemenes G., Benjamin P.R. Cellular traces of behavioral classical conditioning can be recorded at several specific sites in a simple nervous system. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:347–357. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00347.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kemenes I., Kemenes G., Andrew R.J., Benjamin P.R., O’Shea M. Critical time-window for NO-cGMP-dependent long-term memory formation after one-trial appetitive conditioning. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:1414–1425. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-01414.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.