Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Sep 10.
Published in final edited form as: J Cancer Educ. 2012 Mar;27(1):165–171. doi: 10.1007/s13187-011-0285-0

Consistency in Attitudes Across Cancer Screenings in Medically Underserved Minority Populations

Susan J Shaw 1,, James Vivian 2, Kathryn M Orzech 3,4, Cristina Huebner Torres 5, Julie Armin 6
PMCID: PMC4160142  NIHMSID: NIHMS624969  PMID: 22105657

Abstract

While a wide range of behavioral and psychosocial literature explores attitudes and beliefs towards cancer screenings, fewer studies examine attitudes across cancer screening types. We draw on quantitative and qualitative findings from a 4-year prospective study based at a community health center serving diverse, low-income patients. Methods included self-report surveys (n=297), medical chart abstraction, and several qualitative methods with a subsample of participants. Participants included white, African–American, Vietnamese, and Latino patients who were diagnosed with diabetes, hypertension, or both. Patients’ attitudes (both positive and negative) towards cancer screening types were remarkably consistent across cancer screening types. These effects were stronger among men than women. Never having had a cancer screening was generally associated with more unfavorable attitudes towards all screenings. Qualitative interviews indicate the importance of information circulated through social networks in shaping attitudes towards cancer screenings. Condensed abstract: In a multi-method study of attitudes towards cancer screening among medically underserved patients in a primary care setting, we found that attitudes (both positive and negative) were remarkably consistent across cancer screening types.

Keywords: Health knowledge, attitudes, practice, Cancer screening tests, Medically underserved, Minority health, Health care disparities, Breast cancer, Prostate cancer, Colon cancer

Background

Ethnic and racial disparities have been identified in access to and utilization of a range of cancer screenings [17, 21, 24, 33]. A wide range of behavioral and psychosocial studies have investigated factors associated with patient attitudes towards cancer screenings [7, 10, 18], such as the way benefits or risks are explained [11, 28, 30], social networks [31], and acculturation and familism [15, 32], among many others [27]. Much of this literature describes patients’ attitudes towards site-specific cancer screenings [2] or individual screening methods [3]. Experience with a cancer screening may lead people to be positively inclined to continue with a schedule of cancer screenings in the future [1, 7, 14, 16, 29]. Less is known about minority patients’ attitudes across cancer types. Racial and ethnic cancer disparities may be exacerbated by community attitudes and other factors that lead minorities and those with limited access to health care to miss recommended screenings [12]. Our mixed-method study aimed to explore attitudes towards screenings for three cancer types among ethnically diverse, low-income patients at a safety-net clinic. To our knowledge, this is the first study that examines attitudes towards breast, prostate, and colon cancer screenings in this population.

Methods

Funded by the National Cancer Institute, the Culture and Health Literacy study employed self-report surveys regarding cancer screening behaviors and attitudes (n=297), medical chart abstraction, and several qualitative methods. The study was based at Caring Health Center in Springfield, Massachusetts, a primary care clinic located in a medically underserved area. Following informed consent, quantitative surveys were orally administered in the language of the patient’s choice (English, Spanish, or Vietnamese) by bilingual, bicultural interviewers. Self-reported experiences with cancer screening were measured using items adapted from the BRFSS [4]. Attitudes towards cancer screenings were measured using items adapted from breast cancer [5, 6] and colorectal cancer [26] screening scales.

For each cancer type (breast, prostate, colorectal), participants were asked about their experience with two screening methods (for example, breast self-exam and mammogram for breast cancer), including whether a test was recommended to them by their health care provider, and whether they had the test. To the extent possible, equivalent questions were asked about each test and were intended to reflect either favorable (e.g., having the test will reduce my chances of dying from cancer, I don’t worry as much about breast/prostate/colon cancer) or unfavorable attitudes (e.g., the test will be painful, embarrassing, take too much time, cost too much) towards the test in question. For breast cancer screenings, we also asked whether participants agreed that either a mammogram or breast self-exam “will help me find a lump” and whether each is still necessary if the other screening is performed.

The categorical format of these questions (yes/no) allowed us to add the number of items endorsed to create indices of positive (range 0–2 for all screenings except breast cancer, where range 0–4) and negative (range 0–5) attitudes toward each screening type. Female and male attitudes were thus assessed for a total of four screening tests, two of which were gender-specific (breast cancer screening in women; prostate cancer screening in men). For women, we thus created a total of eight scales reflecting positive (4) and negative (4) attitudes towards mammography, breast self-exam (BSE), blood stool test (BST), and colonoscopy (COL). Eight additional scales were created for men reflecting their attitudes towards prostate-specific antigen test (PSA), digital rectal exam (DRE), blood stool test (BST), and colonoscopy (COL). These scales served as the dependent measure in analyses presented below.

A subsample of participants also completed in-depth interviews (n=35), focus groups (n=47), chronic disease diaries (n=15), and home observations (n=12). Qualitative interviews were recorded, transcribed, translated into English if required, and analyzed in a two-step coding process using Atlas.ti, a qualitative data analysis program. Intercoder agreement was achieved through regular meetings among the PI, ethnographer, and qualitative data manager, who periodically coded the same transcripts and met to discuss their coding decisions to ensure that all coders understood how to apply particular codes to the data [20].

Results

Our study sample included white (n=40), African–American (n=64), Latino (n=100), and Vietnamese (n=93) participants who had been diagnosed with hypertension and/or diabetes. Nearly three quarters (74%) of our sample estimated their household income to be between $400 and $1,200 a month. Over half (59%) reported that they were disabled, and over two thirds (67%) rated their health as fair or poor. More than one third (34%) had an eighth-grade education or less, with Vietnamese and Latino patients having fewer years of schooling, on average, than white or African–American participants.

For both women and men, stronger unfavorable attitudes were consistently expressed by those who have never had/done the test in question (see Table 1). The most striking differences concerned unfavorable attitudes towards colonoscopy expressed by those who have never had the test vs. those who have (see Table 1). Favorable attitudes were similarly compared according to experience with tests. No significant differences were found between those men or women who have and have not had/done the test in question. The association between attitudes and experience with a test was thus restricted to the negative dimension of the attitudinal measure. In other words, lack of experience with a screening test was associated with negative attitudes toward the test.

Table 1.

Experience with cancer screenings and unfavorable attitudes

Unfavorable attitudes towards cancer screening
  Women Mammogram BSE BST Colonoscopy
Had test 1.34 .62 1.06 1.42
  No test 2.70 1.10 1.82 3.41
    Sig .002 .04 .05 .0001
    Men PSA DRE BST Colonoscopy
Had test 1.24 1.36 .50 .81
  No test 1.76 2.23 1.88 2.40
    Sig n.s. .03 .001 .0001

Cell means reflect a negative attitude towards the various screening tests. Thus, higher numbers reflect more unfavorable attitudes

To assess consistency in attitudes across screening types, we produced separate correlation matrices for men and women that display associations among the four positive attitudinal indices and among the four negative attitudinal indices. As shown in Table 2, below, women’s positive attitudes towards each screening type were positively associated with each other (mean inter-item correlation =.38). In addition, combining the four positive indices yielded an internally consistent scale (α=.70), reflecting a positive disposition towards all screening tests surveyed. A virtually identical pattern of association was found between women’s negative attitudes towards screening types (mean inter-item r=.37; α=.68). This pattern of consistency in positive and negative attitudes across screening types was even stronger for men, with mean inter-item correlations ranging from (r=.57 to .62) for positive and negative attitudes, respectively. Combining men’s positive and negative attitudes also yielded internally consistent scales (α=.84, α=.87). These findings suggest that the more negative (or positive) participants’ attitudes were towards one screening type, the more negative (or positive) their attitudes were towards all screening types.

Table 2.

Consistency in attitudes across cancer screening types

Women (n=147) Men (n=150)
MAM BSE BST COL PSA DRE BST COL
Positive attitudes
Mammogram PSA
BSE .39** DRE .63**
BST .29* .42** BST .56** .50**
Colonoscopy .39** .32* .47** Colonoscopy .52** .64** .56**
α=.70 α=.84
Negative attitudes
Mammogram PSA
BSE .41** DRE .56**
BST .30* .41** BST .51** .69**
Colonoscopy .31* .40** .53** Colonoscopy .52** .78** .70**
α=.68 α=.87
**

p<.0001;

*

p<.001

These correlational data seem to suggest that the range of attitudinal measures capture a more general positive or negative orientation towards cancer screening across screening types. If this were the case, we would expect a factor analysis to identify clusters of items associated with this more general positive or negative orientation. To explore this further, we submitted the eight positive and negative attitudinal indices to principal components analysis with varimax rotation for men and women separately. In each case, a clear two-component solution was obtained accounting for 69.83% of the variance for men and 54.11% for women. As expected, these two components very clearly reflect a favorable and an unfavorable disposition towards cancer screenings on the whole (see Table 3). For both women and men, high component loadings were consistently associated with the positive attitudinal indices for the first component and with the negative indices for the second component. This pattern of loadings suggests that our measures of cancer screening attitudes detected a more general underlying orientation towards screening that is not necessarily test-specific.

Table 3.

Principal components of cancer screening attitudes

Scale Women Scale Men
Component 1 Positive orientation towards cancer screening
Mammogram (+) .685 PSA (+) .817
Mammogram (−) −.159 PSA (−) −.065
BSE (+) .721 DRE (+) .831
BSE (−) .116 DRE (−) −.103
BST (+) .745 BST (+) .796
BST (−) −.161 BST (−) .010
Colonoscopy (+) .756 Colonoscopy (+) .832
Colonoscopy (−) .056 Colonoscopy (−) −.083
Component 2 Negative orientation towards cancer screening
Mammogram (+) −.159 PSA (+) −.090
Mammogram (−) .587 PSA (−) .726
BSE (+) −.027 DRE (+) −.140
BSE (−) .723 DRE (−) .885
BST (+) .000 BST (+) −.019
BST (−) .786 BST (−) .867
Colonoscopy (+) .035 Colonoscopy (+) .006
Colonoscopy (−) .796 Colonoscopy (−) .889

In sum, we found remarkable consistency in attitudes towards cancer screening insofar as attitudes towards any one screening type were positively associated with attitudes toward any other screening. We now turn to findings from qualitative methods to further contextualize these quantitative findings.

Qualitative Findings

Qualitative, in-depth interviews followed a similar pattern of questions as the survey items, first exploring a participant’s experience with a given cancer screening before turning to a discussion of what the participant learned from the test and what she had heard from others about the test. We specifically asked about each of the six cancer screenings discussed above. While participants described a range of encounters with the health care system, several themes were common across all participants’ discussions of cancer screenings, including fear, anxiety, and a proactive orientation towards learning about their cancer risk (Armin et al., in preparation). Most relevant to the quantitative findings presented above, however, was the importance of social networks in participants’ perceptions of cancer screenings. These themes are presented below.

Participants we interviewed seemed to integrate health information from a variety of sources in their decision making about cancer screenings. Information exchanged within participants’ social networks contributed to their knowledge, beliefs, and attitudes towards cancer screenings. Participants described both giving and receiving information about cancer risk, treatment, and the physical experience of a screening. For example, one Vietnamese participant described her fears about having a colonoscopy based on stories she had heard from friends: “it was so scary when they talked about it.” Once she completed the test, however, she felt these fears were unfounded and now she encourages her friends to have the test, explaining that they do not have to be worried because “it’s nothing.” Chi Nguyen,1 another Vietnamese participant, elaborated conflicted feelings about whether or not to get a colonoscopy because of what she had heard from other people:

Nguyen: Fifty percent one and 50% the other, go or not? After I heard the doctor mention it, I wanted to go. But I heard from other people that the test was painful. So I was like, 50%…go or not.

Interviewer: What kinds of things did you hear from other people who had gone to have the test?

Nguyen: [sigh] Okay, they’re talking, talking and then they sleep. And when they’re done, they don’t know what happened. Something like that. Some people told me the test was painful. Other people said they were put to sleep, so they didn’t know anything.

Interviewer: When your doctor recommended a colonoscopy…what did he or she tell you about it?

Nguyen: They said when I get old, over 50, we must go do the test because it happens to old ladies and old men.

Interviewer: Did the doctor tell you much about the test and how it would work?

Nguyen: I can’t remember what they said.

Note that Nguyen has a fairly detailed recollection of what she learned from others regarding getting a colonoscopy, but she has little recollection of what her doctor told her about colonoscopy beyond, “We must do the test because it happens to old ladies and old men.” This seems to indicate the relative strength—as she puts it, “50–50”—of social network inputs in relation to medical information. Despite concerns raised by information from others in their social networks, both the participants above had positive experiences with cancer screenings that led them to eventually recommend them to others. Similarly, negative experiences may ramify outwards through individuals’ social networks. For example, Alysa Rodriguez, a Latina participant living with hypertension and psoriasis, described her sister’s experience to explain why she declined her doctor’s recommendation to obtain a screening colonoscopy:

Rodriguez: The doctor said that he should have scheduled me for one. But I told him, no, not yet, I didn’t have to. There’s no reason for it… I told him no, because my sister had one, and it was–she was in a lot of pain and stuff and I’ve seen what she went through. And I says, why should I go to one? I mean, I don’t need it right now. I have 50 million things before that, so let me just adjust to this, get used to everything else! So many medications, I mean, don’t keep throwing at me—If you throw too many balls, I only got two hands.

Interviewer: Can you tell me a little more about your sister’s experience? Did you go with her to have the test done?

Rodriguez: Um, yes, and then she was numbed up or whatever, but she says she was still feeling—it was painful and she was still feeling stuff you know. It was really, really uncomfortable. I didn’t want to go through that because I been going through so much with everything else.

For this participant, observing her sister’s experience with a colonoscopy compounded her own reluctance to address what she saw as yet another medical issue when her hands were already full coping with other chronic conditions.

Individual fears or negative experiences may extend from one type of cancer screening to another, leading to the similarities in attitudes across screening types demonstrated in the quantitative data. For example, another Latina participant explained her reluctance to undergo a colonoscopy by describing a bad experience she had after an endoscopy more than 20 years earlier in Puerto Rico. African–American participants in another focus group described how they imagined these fears might spread from one kind of test to another:

P2: Like, [people] might think they might have something, or they scared they might find something, you know. Then they gonna put it off, uh-huh, nobody want to hear that they sick or they got something. Yeah.

P3: In denial! In denial of the truth. When all they doing is making it worse if it is the case.

P4: I mean you hear so much stories about how cancer destroys people.

However, fears may motivate some participants to be adherent, as described by a Latina participant who explained that she does breast self-exam while watching TV “but I know I have to do the mammogram either way… It’s just that I’m fearful, that’s why I do [BSE].”

At the same time, positive experiences also seem to support adherence for future screenings [22]. For example, Latina focus group participants describe their breast cancer screening practices:

P4: I always do both. So annually a mammogram, and thank god I have come out negative.

P5: And monthly I will check myself as well [demonstrating the movement of her hand on her breast].

Q: Monthly, okay.

P4: And me too.

P5: I’ll do the mammogram annually but monthly, well…

P4: And thank God I come out negative and in the self-check also.

Positive experiences such as these and participants’ willingness to circulate their experiences and recommendations through their social networks provide an opening for health care providers to reach medically underserved patients to bring them into regular cancer screening care.

Discussion

Our quantitative results confirm other researchers’ findings that experience with a cancer screening may lead people to continue with a schedule of cancer screenings in the future [7, 22, 29] while negative attitudes are associated with lack of experience with cancer screenings [14]. Our research extends these findings by showing that patients’ positive attitudes towards cancer screenings also seem to be generalized across screening types.

Qualitative interviews reveal a range of perspectives on cancer screenings. The findings reported above indicate that social networks seem to play an important role in patient knowledge, beliefs, and attitudes. Information transmitted through social networks can both reinforce patients’ positive attitudes towards cancer screenings and convey others’ experiences with barriers [3, 27], which may include lack of knowledge, fear, mistrust of health care providers, and fatalism [9, 13]. This research supports the work of others who point to the need for better understanding of the role of social context and relational norms in cancer screening utilization among diverse ethnic groups [23, 24].

Our findings are subject to some limitations. Conducting in-depth interviews and surveys with diverse language groups, including those whose first language is not English, does present barriers between participants and some members of the research staff. To address this, in-depth interviews were conducted with the aid of bilingual research assistants who provided on-the-spot translation into English, and who reviewed transcripts and recordings to ensure that this on-the-spot translation captured the entirety of the participants original comments. In addition, we were unable to track the outcomes of referrals outside the clinic to assess actual adherence to cancer screenings, and were thus forced to rely on participants’ self-reported experience with cancer screenings. In addition, we used breast cancer [5, 6] and colon cancer [26] screening scales to measure attitudes towards prostate cancer screenings. Finally, because we translated our attitudinal questions into Vietnamese and Spanish, we did not have the benefit of psychometric research establishing their reliability and validity in these language groups. But while the reliability of Vietnamese endorsements may be in doubt, discussions of attitudes towards cancer screenings in in-depth interviews and focus groups with Vietnamese speakers corresponded to attitudes presented in quantitative data, increasing our confidence in the validity of these findings.

Conclusions and Recommendations

Consistency in attitudes across cancer screenings is an important finding with broad significance for early identification efforts, especially among medically underserved patients. Since patients who have never had any cancer screening tend to have the most negative attitudes towards all screenings, our findings suggest that primary care providers should refer patients with limited screening experience to the most accessible screening possible to ensure their participation. In addition, like other peer education models to increase cancer screenings [8, 19, 25], primary care clinics should seek to draw on and mobilize social networks where diverse individuals can share their positive cancer screening experience with members of their community. A testimonial from a community member citing a good experience with cancer screenings may lend legitimacy to the screening process. Such a program could also serve as a forum for patients and providers to increase patient understanding of cancer risks and the role of screenings in early detection and survival [28].

Acknowledgments

The research described in this paper was supported by the National Cancer Institute grant number R01 CA128455, Susan J. Shaw, Principal Investigator. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Cancer Institute or the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

1

A pseudonym, as are all names of individuals presented here.

There are no financial disclosures from any author.

Contributor Information

Susan J. Shaw, Email: shaws@email.arizona.edu, School of Anthropology, University of Arizona, P.O. Box 210030, Tucson, AZ 85721-0030, USA.

James Vivian, Email: vivian@hartford.edu, Department of Psychology, University of Hartford, Hartford, CT 06117, USA.

Kathryn M. Orzech, Email: kathryn_orzech@brown.edu, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown University, Box G-BH, Providence, RI 02912, USA; Chronobiology and Sleep Research Laboratory, E. P. Bradley Hospital, Providence, RI, USA.

Cristina Huebner Torres, Email: chuebner@caringhealth.org, Community Programs and Research, Caring Health Center, 1145 Main Street, Springfield, MA 01103, USA.

Julie Armin, Email: jarmin@email.arizona.edu, School of Anthropology, University of Arizona, P.O. Box 210030, Tucson, AZ 85721-0030, USA.

References

  • 1.Allen JD, Stoddard AM, Sorensen G. Do social network characteristics predict mammography screening practices? Health Educ Behav. 2008;35(6):763–776. doi: 10.1177/1090198107303251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Angtuaco TL, Banaad-Omiotek GD, Howden CW. Differing attitudes toward virtual and conventional colonoscopy for colorectal cancer screening: surveys among primary care physicians and potential patients. Am J Gastroenterol. 2001;96(3):887–893. doi: 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2001.03639.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Austin KL, Power E, Solarin I, Atkin WS, Wardle J, Robb KA. Perceived barriers to flexible sigmoidoscopy screening for colorectal cancer among UK ethnic minority groups: a qualitative study. J Med Screen. 2009;16(4):174–179. doi: 10.1258/jms.2009.009080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System Survey Questionnaire. Atlanta: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Champion V. Development of a benefits and barriers scale for mammography utilization. Cancer Nurs. 1995;18(1):53–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Champion V, Scott C. Reliability and validity of breast cancer screening beliefs scales in African American women. Nurs Res. 1997;46(6):331–337. doi: 10.1097/00006199-199711000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Drossaert CH, Boer H, Seydel ER. Does mammographic screening and a negative result affect attitudes towards future breast screening? J Med Screen. 2001;8(4):204–212. doi: 10.1136/jms.8.4.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Erwin D, Thea Spatz R, Stotts C, Hollenberg J. Increasing mammography practice by African American women. Cancer Pract. 1999;7(2):78–85. doi: 10.1046/j.1523-5394.1999.07204.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fernandez M, Wippold R, Torres-Vigil I, Byrd T, Freeberg D, Bains Y, Guajardo J, Coughlin S, Vernon S. Colorectal cancer screening among Latinos from U.S. cities along the Texas–Mexico border. Cancer Causes Control. 2008;19:195–206. doi: 10.1007/s10552-007-9085-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fylan F. Screening for cervical cancer: a review of women’s attitudes, knowledge, and behaviour. Br J Gen Pract. 1998;48(433):1509–1514. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gattellari M, Ward JE. A community study using specified and unspecified scenarios to investigate men’s views about PSA screening. Health Expect. 2004;7(4):274–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-7625.2004.00285.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Goldman RE, Diaz JA, Kim I. Perspectives of colorectal cancer risk and screening among Dominicans and Puerto Ricans: stigma and misperceptions. Qual Health Res. 2009;19(11):1559–1568. doi: 10.1177/1049732309349359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Greiner KA, Born W, Nollen N, Ahluwalia JS. Knowledge and perceptions of colorectal cancer screening among urban African Americans. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20(11):977–983. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2005.00165.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Han Y, Williams RD, Harrison RA. Breast cancer screening knowledge, attitudes, and practices among Korean American women. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2000;27(10) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Harmon MP, Castro FG, Coe K. Acculturation and cervical cancer: knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors of Hispanic women. Women Health. 1997;24(3):37–57. doi: 10.1300/j013v24n03_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Janda M, Stanton WR, Hughes K, Del Mar C, Clavarino A, Aitken JF, Shilu T, Short L, Leggett B, Newman B. Knowledge, attitude and intentions related to colorectal cancer screening using faecal occult blood tests in a Rural Australian Population. Asia Pac J Publ Health. 2003;15(1):50–56. doi: 10.1177/101053950301500109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Janz NK, Wren PA, Schottenfeld D, Guire KE. Colorectal cancer screening attitudes and behavior: a population-based study. Prev Med. 2003;37(6 Pt 1):627–634. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lantz P, Weigers M, House J. Education and income differentials in breast and cervical cancer screening policy implications for rural women. Med Care. 1997;35(3):219–236. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199703000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Larkey L. Las mujeres saludables: reaching Latinas for breast, cervical and colorectal cancer prevention and screening. J Commun Health. 2006;31(1):69–77. doi: 10.1007/s10900-005-8190-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.MacQueen KM. Codebook development for team-based qualitative analysis. Cult Anthropol Methods. 1998;10(2):31–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Murff H, Peterson N, Fowke J, Hargreaves M, Signorello L, Dittus R, Zheng W, Blot W. Colonoscopy screening in African Americans and Whites with affected first-degree relatives. Arch Intern Med. 2008;168(6):625. doi: 10.1001/archinte.168.6.625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Palmer RC, Midgette LA, Mullan ID. Colorectal cancer screening preferences among African Americans: which screening test is preferred? J Cancer Educ. 2010;25(4):577–581. doi: 10.1007/s13187-010-0081-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pasick RJ, Barker JC, Otero-Sabogal R, Burke NJ, Galen J, Guerra C. Intention, subjective norms, and cancer screening in the context of relational culture. Health Educ Behav. 2009;36(5 Suppl):91S–110S. doi: 10.1177/1090198109338919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pasick RJ, Burke NJ. A critical review of theory in breast cancer screening promotion across cultures. Ann Rev Publ Health. 2008;29(4):1–18. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.143420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Passanisi N, Prout M, Holm LJ. The New England division Tell A Friend program implementation evaluation. Cancer Pract. 2001;9(Suppl 1):S64–S71. doi: 10.1046/j.1523-5394.2001.95111.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rawl S, Champion V, Menon U, Loehrer PJ, Vance GH, Skinner CS. Validation of scales to measure benefits of and barriers to colorectal cancer screening. J Psychosoc Oncol. 2001;19(3–4):47–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Robb KA, Solarin I, Power E, Atkin W, Wardle J. Attitudes to colorectal cancer screening among ethnic minority groups in the UK. BMC Publ Health. 2008;8:34. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sarfati D, Howden-Chapman P, Woodward A, Salmond C. Does the frame affect the picture? A study into how attitudes to screening for cancer are affected by the way benefits are expressed. J Med Screen. 1998;5(3):137–140. doi: 10.1136/jms.5.3.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schueler KM, Chu PW, Smith-Bindman R. Factors associated with mammography utilization: a systematic quantitative review of the literature. J Womens Health. 2008;17(9):1477–1498. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2007.0603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Straus W, Mansley E, Gold K, Wang Q, Reddy P, Pashos C. Colorectal cancer screening attitudes and practices in the general population: a risk-adjusted survey. J Publ Health Manag Pract. 2005;11(3) doi: 10.1097/00124784-200505000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Suarez L, Lloyd L, Weiss N, Rainbolt T, Pulley L. Effect of social networks on cancer-screening behavior of older Mexican–American women. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1994;86(10):775–779. doi: 10.1093/jnci/86.10.775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Suarez L, Pulley L. Comparing acculturation scales and their relationship to cancer screening among older Mexican–American women. J Natl Cancer Inst Monogr. 1995;18:41–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Vernon S, Vogel V, Halabi S, Jackson G, Lundy R, Peters G. Breast cancer screening behaviors and attitudes in three racial/ethnic groups. Cancer. 1992;69(1):165–174. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19920101)69:1<165::aid-cncr2820690128>3.0.co;2-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES