Abstract
Intrusive parenting has been positively associated with child anxiety, although examinations of this relationship to date have been largely confined to middle to upper middle class families and have rarely used longitudinal designs. With several leading interventions for child anxiety emphasizing the reduction of parental intrusiveness, it is critical to determine whether the links between parental intrusiveness and child anxiety broadly apply to families of all financial means, and whether parental intrusiveness prospectively predicts the development of child anxiety. This study employed latent growth curve analysis to evaluate the interactive effects of maternal intrusiveness and financial means on the developmental trajectory of child anxiety from 1st grade to age 15 in 1,121 children (50.7% male) and their parents from the NICHD SECCYD. The overall model was found to provide good fit, revealing that early maternal intrusiveness and financial means did not impact individual trajectories of change in child anxiety, which were stable from 1st to 5th grade, and then decrease from 5th grade to age 15. Cross-sectional analyses also examined whether family financial means moderated contemporaneous relationships between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety in 3rd and 5th grades. The relationship between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety was moderated by family financial means for 1st graders, with stronger links found among children of lower family financial means, but not for 3rd and 5th graders. Neither maternal intrusiveness nor financial means in 1st grade predicted subsequent changes in anxiety across childhood. Findings help elucidate for whom and when maternal intrusiveness has the greatest link with child anxiety and can inform targeted treatment efforts.
Keywords: parenting, child, anxiety, intrusiveness, income, latent growth modeling
Anxiety disorders are among the most common mental disorders experienced by children and adolescents, affecting up to 32% of youth by adolescence (Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler & Angold, 2003; Merikangas, Nakamura, & Kessler, 2009; Merikangas et al., 2010). Child anxiety disorders are highly debilitating and negatively affect children's functioning in school and recreational activities, as well as their relations with family members and peers (Essau, Conradt, & Petermann, 2000; Ezpeleta, Keeler, Erkanli, Costello, & Angold, 2001; Strauss, Frame, Forehand, 1987). Furthermore, the presence of an anxiety disorder in childhood places the child at increased risk for anxiety disorders, depression, and substance abuse in adulthood (Kim-Cohen et al., 2003; Kovacs, Gatsonis, Paulauskas, & Richards, 1989; Kushner, Sher, & Beitman, 1990). Anxiety disorders persisting into adulthood are associated with considerable occupational impairments and reduced health-related quality of life (Comer et al., 2011; Merikangas et al., 2007). Accordingly, understanding factors associated with the development and maintenance of anxiety across childhood is critical.
Empirical work shows parenting behaviors may be linked to the development and maintenance of anxiety in school-aged and preschool-aged children. Specifically, intrusive/overinvolved and overcontrolling parenting have been associated with the presence and severity of child anxiety (e.g., Bayer, Sanson, & Hemphill, 2006; Dumas, LaFreniere, & Serketich, 1995; Hudson & Rapee, 2002; Rubin, Cheah, & Fox, 2001; Siqueland, Kendall, & Steinberg, 1996; Wood, McLeod, Sigman, Hwang, & Chu, 2003). Such parenting entails excessive parental regulation of children's activities and routines, discouragement of children's autonomy and independence, encouragement of children's dependence on parents, and instruction to children on how to think or feel (Barber, 1996; McLeod, Wood, & Weisz, 2007; Steinberg, Elmer, & Mounts, 1989). The relationship between such parenting and child anxiety has been found most reliably when parenting constructs have been assessed via observational methods (Wood et al., 2003), such as coded parent-child interaction tasks. During these tasks parenting behavior is commonly defined as intrusive when the behavior restricts the child and prevents him or her from experiencing a developmentally appropriate degree of autonomy (Hudson, Comer, & Kendall, 2008; Wood, 2006).
Meta-analytic examination across studies documents the largest effect with regard to the association between child anxiety and parenting that does not grant developmentally appropriate child autonomy (pooled ES = .42; McLeod et al., 2007). Such parenting is believed to encroach upon children's autonomous functioning and independence, which can foster a sense of hopelessness that is associated with increased anxiety (Chorpita & Barlow, 1998; Wood et al., 2003). Further, intrusive parenting behaviors when the child is experiencing negative affect may serve to inadvertently maintain anxiety by preventing the child from fully participating in anxiety-provoking situations—experiences that would otherwise afford critical opportunities to mastery anxiety in situ and modify maladaptive cognitions regarding fear-eliciting stimuli (Dadds & Barrett, 2001; Wood, Piacentini, Southam-Gerow, Chu, & Sigman, 2006). Accordingly, evidence-based treatments for childhood anxiety often take a parenting-focused approach, and specifically target the reduction of intrusive/overinvolved and overcontrolling parenting as a core treatment objective (e.g., Barrett, Dadds, & Rapee, 1996; Comer et al., 2012; Kendall, Hudson, Gosch, Flannery-Schroeder, & Suveg, 2008).
Although a growing body of literature supports a link between intrusive parenting behavior and the presence and severity of child anxiety, empirical work to date in this area has focused almost exclusively on relatively small samples of youth from clinic-based samples in academic settings. Findings from youth from clinics in academic settings may not generalize to the broader spectrum of youth in the general population, particularly with regard to economic diversity, as the majority of work in this area has been conducted with families of middle to high socioeconomic status (SES), or families of unreported SES (Bayer et al., 2006; Hudson & Rapee, 2002; Rubin et al., 2001; Siqueland et al., 1996). Further, research examining relationships between intrusive parenting and child anxiety in economically diverse community samples (e.g., Edison et al., 2011; Gray, Carter, and Silverman, 2010) has focused on geographically restricted regions, limiting the broad generalizability of these findings.
Although research has examined SES as it relates to broad-based composites of parenting dimensions (e.g., negative parenting and maternal sensitivity) and their relationships with broad classes of child symptoms (e.g., internalizing problems) (Belsky, Bell, Bradley, Stallard, & Stewart-Brown, 2006; Dumas et al., 1995; Feng, Shaw, & Moilanen, 2011; NICHD, 2005), such work does little to inform the specific relationships between parental intrusiveness and child anxiety. Given that higher rates of anxiety and other emotional and behavioral problems are found in low-income children (Dumas et al., 1995; Essex et al., 2006; Johnson, Cohen, Dohrenwend, Link, Brook, 1999; McLoyd, 1998), and some parenting work suggests that too little involvement (i.e., poor parental monitoring) is associated with internalizing symptoms (Bacchini, Miranda, & Affuso, 2011; Buckner, Mezzacappa, & Beardslee, 2003; Grant et al., 2003; Nebbitt & Lambert, 2009; Whittaker, Harden, See, Meisch, & Westbrook, 2011), to optimally inform treatment efforts for the broad spectrum of families needing care it is critical to clarify how relationships between parental overinvolvement and child anxiety outcomes are affected—or unaffected—by differences in financial means. Without a clear understanding of how parenting behaviors interact with financial means to develop or maintain maladaptive child outcomes, it remains unclear whether treatment-seeking families at economic disadvantage would be expected to benefit from the same degree of parent-focused intervention showing support in middle- to upper-class families, or whether interventions with a less prominent parenting focus may be better suited to families at economic disadvantage.
The relationship between intrusive/overprotective parenting and the development of child anxiety has yet to be evaluated as a function of family financial means. As current early interventions for child anxiety stress the importance of reducing intrusive parenting behaviors (Comer et al., 2012; Pincus, Ehrenreich, Santucci, & Eyberg, 2008; Rapee, Kennedy, Ingram, Edwards, & Sweeney, 2010), it is essential to determine whether findings linking parental intrusiveness to child anxiety can be generalized to families of varying financial means. To optimally inform interventions to be disseminated to families across a range of financial means, the present study examined household income relative to the poverty threshold as a moderator of the relationship between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety across childhood in a large multiphase sample of U.S. community youth. Specifically, we hypothesized that this income-to-needs ratio would moderate the relationship between maternal intrusiveness in 1st grade and the trajectory of child anxiety from 1st grade to age 15. In addition to prospective analyses examining longitudinal growth, we examined the contemporaneous relationships between maternal intrusiveness financial means and child anxiety in 3rd and in 5th grades.
Methods
Participants
Participants included 1,121 children and their mothers who participated in phases II-IV of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD; a large, national multiphase study of parenting, childcare experiences, and child outcomes). Participants were recruited in 1991 into Phase I of this study when the child was 1 month old. Families were recruited through 10 data collection sites across the United States: Arkansas, California, Kansas, Massachusetts, Pennsylvania (Pittsburgh and Philadelphia), Virginia, Washington, North Carolina, and Wisconsin. Children and their mothers were excluded if: 1) the mother was younger than 18 years at the time of the child's birth, 2) the mother was not able to converse in English, 3) the child had obvious disabilities at birth or remained in the hospital > 7 days after birth, and 4) if the family did not plan to remain within the catchment area for at least 3 years. As reported elsewhere, study procedures were approved by the institutional review boards at the 10 NICHD-SECCYD study sites and informed consent was obtained from all participating families (Duckworth, Quinn, & Tsukayama, 2012). The 1,121 children included in the present analyses were 50.67% boys and were on average 6.45 years old (SD=0.50) at the time of their 1st grade assessment. The racial breakdown of children was: 81.45% Caucasian, 11.86% African-American, 1.52% Asian/Pacific Islander, 0.27% Native American/Aleutian, and 4.91% other. The racial breakdown of mothers was: 83.76% Caucasian, 11.78% African-American, 2.14% Asian/Pacific Islander, 0.62% Native American/Aleutian, and 1.69% other. The majority of children and their mothers identified as Non-Hispanic, 93.84% and 95.45%, respectively. Among the families included in the present analyses, 8.55% were characterized as living below the poverty line, 18.64% near poverty/working poor, 47.35% low-middle to middle class, and 25.46% relatively affluent (NICHD, 2009). More details regarding the SECCYD sample can be found elsewhere (see Allhusen et al., 2001).
Measures
Child anxiety
Child anxiety was assessed through the Anxiety Problems Scale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991) at six developmental time points (1st grade, 3rd grade, 4th grade, 5th grade, and 6th grade, and age 15). The CBCL is a 113 item parental report used extensively and designed to assess a broad range of children's behavioral and emotional functioning. This measure provides six DSM-IV oriented scales, including the Anxiety Problems Scale. The Anxiety Problems scale is comprised of questions assessing a broad range of anxiety related behaviors that are associated with a variety of child anxiety disorders, including clinging to adults, worrying, fearfulness and nervousness. The reliability and validity of the Anxiety Problems DSM-IV oriented scale has been demonstrated in previous samples of children and adolescents (Nakamura, Ebesutani, Bernstein, & Chorpita, 2009). Further, the Anxiety Problems scale has been found to discriminate well between children with and without anxiety disorders, and discriminates between children with anxiety and depressive disorders better than the empirically derived anxious/depressed scale of the CBCL (Ebesutani et al., 2010). Participating mothers completed the CBCL at each time point in the laboratory or via mail. In the present sample, the Anxiety Problems Scale of the CBCL demonstrated adequate internal consistencies across all time points: 1st grade (α= .57), 3rd grade (α= .65), 4th grade (α= .67), 5th grade (α= .66), and 6th grade (α= .66), and at age 15 (α= .65).
Financial means
An income-to-needs ratio was computed to capture family financial means. This ratio was calculated by dividing the total reported family household income, including government assistance, by the poverty threshold, as set by the U.S. Census bureau each year, which takes into account the number of people in the household and number of children. An income-to-needs ratio score of < 1.0 = poverty, 1.0 = at the poverty index, 1.0-2.0 = near poverty “working poor”, 2.0-5.0 low-middle to middle class, and > 5.0 = relatively affluent (NICHD, 2009). Financial means values included in the current analyses reflect the demographic information reported by the family when their child was in 1st, 3rd, and 5th grade.
Maternal intrusiveness
Maternal intrusiveness was measured through a 15-minute parent-child interaction task completed by mothers and their child. Mothers and children completed this task in a laboratory when the child was in 1st, 3rd and 5th grades. This task was designed for the NICHD SECCYD and the observational rating scale on which mothers were rated was adapted from Egeland and Heister's Teaching Task Rating Scales (1993). In first grade the task was comprised of three semi-structured play activities, two of which were designed to be too difficult for the child to do without the mother's assistance. The first task was an Etch-A-Sketch drawing task, during which the mother and child had to operate the knobs together to draw a house and tree. The second task required children to fill in cutout frames with patterned tiles. The third activity was an interactive card game during which children and their mothers competed against each other to win cards. All observations were video taped and independently rated by trained coders blind to family financial means and child anxiety status on a 7-point global rating scale to reflect the degree to which the mothers respected the child's autonomy during the three semi-structured activities (NICHD ECRN, 2002). In 3rd and 5th grade, the mother-child interaction task was comprised of two activities; a planning task and a discussion task. On this scale, mothers were rated as exhibiting, very low, low, moderately low, moderate, moderately high, high, or very high respect for their child's autonomy. Lower scores indicated greater intrusiveness and higher scores indicated that the mother recognized and respected the child's individuality, motives, and perspectives. For the present analyses, responses on this scale were reverse scored, so that higher scores reflected greater intrusiveness and reduced parental respect for child autonomy. Acceptable inter-rater reliability was established for the respect for autonomy ratings in a sub-sample of 242 mothers of 54 month-old children (r = .64, and r > .77 when estimated using repeated measures ANOVA; NICHD, n.d.).
Data Analysis
Prospective longitudinal relationships between maternal intrusiveness and growth in child anxiety, and the potential moderating function of family financial means, were examined via latent growth models using Mplus 6.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2010). A maximum likelihood (ML) estimator was used to fit the unconditional LGM containing both linear and quadratic slope factors to the data to evaluate steady and accelerating growth in child anxiety. This LGM evaluated linear and quadric changes in child anxiety from 1st grade to age 15, incorporating child anxiety data from 6 developmental time points: 1st grade, 3rd grade, 4th grade, 5th grade, and 6th grade, and at age 15. A number of indices were used to assess model fit (χ2, SRMR; RMSEA; CFI; TLI). Modification indices were also examined to determine if freeing any model parameters would enhance model fit. Maternal intrusiveness, family financial means, and their interaction, were then entered into the model (see Figure 1) and the data were fit to the conditional LGM to evaluate if family financial means moderated the longitudinal relationship between maternal intrusiveness and changes in anxiety across childhood.
To examine whether family financial means moderated the contemporaneous relationships between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety at each major time point, cross-sectional analyses were conducted using multiple regression. These analyses were conducted in 3 steps: 1) maternal intrusiveness was regressed onto child anxiety, 2) financial means and maternal intrusiveness were regressed onto child anxiety, 3) the mean centered product term of maternal intrusiveness and financial means were added to the multiple regression as a third predictor of child anxiety.
Results
Longitudinal Analyses
To examine relationships between parental intrusiveness and the development of subsequent child anxiety, and whether such relationships are moderated by the family's financial means, an unconditional latent growth model was first fit to the child anxiety data from 1st grade through age 15 (See Table 1 for means of all observed variables). As the quadric growth factor provided good fit to the data, a piecewise linear model was evaluated to enhance interpretability of slope differences. The piecewise slopes included a pre-5th grade linear slope and post-5th grade nonlinear slope. A piecewise slope demonstrated good fit to the data (χ2=34.97, p<.01, SRMR=0.02, RMSEA=0.04 90% CI=.03-.06, CFI=0.99, TLI=0.99; see Table 2 for unstandardized parameter estimates). The fixed effects were not significant for the pre-5th grade slope (i.e., Slope 1), but were significant for the post-5th grade slope (i.e., Slope 2), meaning that on average, children did not experience significant change in anxiety from 1st to 5th grade, but did experience significant nonlinear decreases in anxiety from 5th grade to age 15. The random effects for pre-5th grade and post 5th grade slopes were statistically significant, meaning that children experienced significant individual differences in anxiety change from 1st to 5th grade and from 5th grade to age 15. The random effects for intercept were also significant (p<.01), indicating that there were significant individual differences in level of child anxiety at grade 1.
Table 1.
Variable | 1st grade | 3rd grade | 4th grade | 5th grade | 6th grade | Age 15 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Child Anxiety | 1.23 (1.44) | 1.24 (1.56) | 1.13 (1.53) | 1.23 (1.61) | 1.12 (1.58) | 0.81 (1.32) |
Maternal Intrusiveness | 2.74 (1.16) | 3.10 (1.03) | --- | 3.02 (.94) | --- | --- |
Financial Means | 3.95 (3.03) | 4.40 (3.78) | --- | 4.54 (4.06) | --- | --- |
Note: Standard deviations are presented in parentheses
Table 2.
Variable | Intercept | Intercept-Slope 1 | Intercept-Slope 2 | Slope 1 | Slope 2 | Slope 1-Slope2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | 1.22*** | --- | --- | −.00 | −.11*** | --- |
Variance | 1.21*** | --- | --- | .05*** | .08*** | --- |
Covariance | --- | −0.01 | −0.11*** | --- | --- | −0.02** |
Note:
p<.01
p<.001
As the unconditional model demonstrated good fit, the model was then fit as a conditional model, in which the growth factors were regressed onto maternal intrusiveness at grade 1 (MI1) and family financial means at grade 1 (FM1). The conditional model demonstrated good fit to the data (χ2=40.767, p<.01; SRMR=0.02; RMSEA=0.04 90% CI=.02-.05; CFI=0.99, TLI=0.99). The model accounted for 2.5% (p=.06) of the variance in grade 1 anxiety scores, 0.3% (p=.60) of the variance in anxiety slope from grade 1 to grade 5, and 0.6% (p=.37) of the variance in anxiety slope from grade 5 to age 15. The level of MI1 was found to account for significant unique variance in the level of child anxiety at grade 1 (MI1→Intercept standardized pathway=.09, p=.03), when controlling for FM1. The initial level of FM1 also accounted for significant unique variance in the level of child anxiety at grade 1 (FM1→Intercept standardized pathway=-.11, p=.01), when controlling for MI1. The initial level of MI1 did not account for significant unique variance in the slope of child anxiety from grade 1 to 5 (MI1→Slope 1 standardized pathway=-.06, p=.31), or from grade 5 to age 15 (MI1→Slope 2 standardized pathway=-.02, p=.55), when controlling for FM1. Likewise, the initial level of FM1 did not account for significant unique variance in the slope of child anxiety from grade 1 to 5 (FM1→Slope 1 standardized pathway=-.00, p=.94) or from grade 5 to age 15 (FM1→Slope 2 standardized pathway=.07, p=.12), when controlling for MI1.
The MI1×FM1 interaction term was then entered into the conditional LGM. The conditional model demonstrated good fit to the data (χ2=42.34, p<.01; SRMR=0.02; RMSEA=0.03 90%CI=.02-.05; CFI=0.99, TLI=0.99). See Figure 1 for standardized parameter estimates. The model accounted for 3.2% of the variability in grade 1 anxiety scores (p=.03), and 0.8% (p=.42) of the variability in anxiety slope from grade 1 to grade 5, and 0.8% (p=.24) of the variability in anxiety slope from grade 5 to age 15. The effect of FM1 on the intercept of child anxiety was significant (p<.01), while the effect of MI1 on the intercept of child anxiety was not significant (p=.13). However, the effect of MI1×FM1 on the intercept of child anxiety was significant (p=.04). Among families of lower FM1 (defined as >.5 SD below the mean), maternal intrusiveness was associated with greater anxiety, but among families of higher FM1 (defined as >.5 SD above the mean) it was not (see Figure 2). The effects of MI1, FM1, and MI1×FM1 on the slope of child anxiety from grade 1 to 5 and grade 5 to age 15 were not significant.
Cross-Sectional Analyses at Grades 3 and 5
Grade 3
Table 3 presents the results of linear multiple regression analyses examining the main and interactive effects of maternal intrusiveness and financial means on child anxiety at grade 3. Maternal intrusiveness observed at grade 3 (MI3) significantly predicted child anxiety when entered as the sole predictor. The addition of grade 3 financial means (FM3) to the model significantly added to the prediction of child anxiety. Although the addition of the interaction term FM3×MI3 added to the model's overall prediction of grade three child anxiety, the product term itself was not a significant predictor, indicating that the relationship between family FM3 and child anxiety in the third grade is uniform across levels of MI3.
Table 3.
Variable Entered | B | SE (B) | β | t |
---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | ||||
MI3 | .13 | .05 | .08 | 2.50* |
Step 2 | ||||
MI3 | .07 | .05 | .05 | 1.40 |
FM3 | −.05 | .01 | −.13 | −3.67*** |
Step 3 | ||||
MI3 | .06 | .05 | .04 | 1.17 |
FM3 | −.06 | .02 | −.15 | −4.05*** |
MI3×FM3 | −.03 | .02 | −.06 | −1.73 |
Note: Step 1: R2=.01, F(l, 894)= 6.23, p=.01
Step 2: R2=.02, ΔR2=.02, F(2, 893)= 9.88, p<.01
Step 3: R2=.03, ΔR2=.00, F(3, 892)= 7.60, p<.01
p<.05
**= p<.01
p<.001
MI3 = Maternal Intrusiveness in 3rd Grade
FM3= Financial Means in 3rd Grade
Grade 5
Table 4 presents the results of linear multiple regression analyses examining the main and interactive effects of maternal intrusiveness and financial means on child anxiety at grade 5. As in third grade, maternal intrusiveness observed at grade 5 (MI5) significantly predicted child anxiety in grade 5, and the addition of family financial means at grade 5 (FM5) added to the prediction of child anxiety. The addition of the interaction term FM5×MI5 did not add to the prediction of child anxiety, indicating that the relationship between MI5 and child anxiety in the fifth grade is uniform across levels of family FM5.
Table 4.
Variable Entered | B | SE (B) | β | t |
---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | ||||
MI5 | .13 | .06 | .08 | 2.27* |
Step 2 | ||||
MI5 | .10 | .06 | .06 | 1.75 |
FM5 | −.03 | .01 | −.06 | −1.28 |
Step 3 | ||||
MI5 | .10 | .06 | .06 | 1.65 |
FM5 | −.03 | .01 | −.07 | −1.94 |
MI5× FM5 | −.01 | .01 | −.02 | −.68 |
Note: Step 1: R2=.01, F(1, 855)= 5.15, p=.02
Step 2: R2=.01, ΔR2=.01, F(2, 854)= 4.23, p=.02
Step 3: R2=.01, ΔR2=.00, F(3, 853)= 2.98, p=.03
p<.05
**= p<.01, ***= p<.001
MI5 = Maternal Intrusiveness in 5th Grade
FM5= Financial Means in 5th Grade
Discussion
The present findings provide a rare statistical portrait of the complex relationships between maternal intrusiveness, family financial means, and child anxiety from early childhood through early adolescence in a large multiphase sample of U.S. community youth. Longitudinal analyses using latent growth modeling revealed that individual trajectories of change in child anxiety were stable from 1st to 5th grade, and then decrease from 5th grade to age 15. Cross-sectional analyses at key developmental time points revealed significant links between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety that vary significantly as a function of family financial means, although early maternal intrusiveness did not prospectively impact the subsequent trajectory of child anxiety across childhood. Cross-sectional relationships found in this large national community sample between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety are consistent with previous work conducted with smaller, clinic-based, and/or geographically restricted samples (McLeod et al., 2007). However, the present analyses revealed that the link between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety varies as a function of time and of family financial means.
Across study time points, the link between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety was strongest in 1st grade, particularly for lower-income families, and was less pronounced in subsequent years. Specifically, among 1st graders, the relationship between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety was moderated by family financial means, but for 3rd and 5th graders family financial means did not moderate such a relationship and emerged as a stronger predictor of child anxiety than maternal intrusiveness in these later years. In 1st grade only, children with greater family financial means demonstrated lower levels of anxiety regardless of level of maternal intrusiveness, whereas among children with lower family financial means, children who experienced high levels of maternal intrusiveness displayed higher anxiety than those who experienced lower levels of intrusiveness. Findings suggest that at 1st grade, children from families with reduced financial means may be particularly susceptible to anxiety in general, as well as the specific association between maternal intrusiveness and child anxiety. Importantly, family financial means emerged as the only the significant predictor of child anxiety in 3rd grade, regardless of level of maternal intrusiveness, and was no longer a significant predictor of child anxiety in 5th grade. Taken together these findings help elucidate for whom and when maternal intrusiveness has the greatest link with child anxiety.
The present findings highlight the shifting importance of various contextual influences on the maintenance of child anxiety across time. The present findings highlight the importance of parenting behavior and the financial environment in the maintenance of early childhood anxiety, but suggest other environmental influences may play a more important role as children develop. Such findings are consistent with Bronfenbrenner's ecological model of child development (1986), which characterizes shifting external forces that are linked with children's functioning and provide the contexts within which child development unfolds. Bronfenbrenner's model emphasizes how contextual influences vary in degree of proximity to the child, can be described as nested within one another, and influence one another over time to shape child development. The present findings suggest that early in childhood intrusive parenting environments in the child's microsystem interact with environments external to the child's world (e.g., exosystems such as parental income), but as the child ages this interaction is less salient and other aspects of the child's environment (e.g., peer relationships, neighborhood safety, or other parenting behaviors) may play a more central role in child anxiety. Indeed, research shows that the development of adolescent emotional stability is more closely linked to relationships with peers rather than parents (Hay & Ashman, 2003), and relationships with anxious or deviant peers have been shown to be associated with anxiety among adolescents, particularly those living in unsafe neighborhoods (Nebbitt & Lambert, 2009; Van Zalk, Van Zalk & Kerr, 2011). Further, as the current model accounted for only up to 3% of the variance in child anxiety, other parenting behaviors associated with child anxiety, such as parental modeling, warmth, and hostile expressed emotion (Burstein & Ginsburg, 2010; Hale, Engels, & Meeus, 2006; van der Bruggen, Stams, Bögels, & Paulussen-Hoogeboom, 2010), may account for some of the unexplained variance in child anxiety. However, it is likely that these other parenting behaviors will explain relatively less variance in child anxiety, given that among parenting behaviors associated with child anxiety, autonomy granting consistently demonstrates the largest effects (McLeod et al., 2007).
Consequently, our findings highlight the importance of addressing maternal intrusiveness in the treatment of early child anxiety, particularly among low-income families. Indeed, evidence-based approaches to the treatment of early childhood anxiety have found success in efforts to reduce parental intrusiveness (e.g., Comer et al., 2012; Hirshfeld-Becker et al., 2010; Pincus et al., 2008; Rapee et al., 2010), whereas parent-focused efforts in the treatment of older youth with anxiety has not necessarily yielded improved outcomes relative to individual child treatment (e.g., Kendall et al., 2008). The present findings underscore the urgency of focused efforts to disseminate parent-focused child anxiety interventions for younger children from families of lower financial means.
Given that findings did not provide support for the roles of maternal intrusiveness or financial means in 1st grade predicting subsequent changes in anxiety, maternal intrusiveness appears to be more linked to the maintenance, rather than development, of child anxiety. Recent work evaluating the directionality of this relationship through the evaluation of genetic and environmental factors similarly suggest that high levels of intrusiveness are likely elicited by high child anxiety (Aschenbrand & Kendall, 2012; Hudson et al., 2008; Eley, Napolitano, Lau, & Gregory, 2010), which in turn may serve to maintain child anxiety. Other factors that may better account for changes in anxiety over the course of childhood may include early child temperament, parental anxiety, and stressful life events (Muris, van Brakel, Arntz, & Schouten, 2011).
Several limitations of the present work merit comment. First, measures of child anxiety were solely obtained through maternal reports of child anxiety. Although research supports the validity of parent reports of child anxiety (Brown-Jacobsen, Wallace & Whiteside, 2011; Manassis, Tannock, & Monga, 2009), it is possible that parents’ awareness of unobservable symptoms of child anxiety may be limited (Comer & Kendall, 2004). Future work in this area would do well to incorporate multimodal assessments of child anxiety. Second, as child anxiety was assessed as a unitary construct in these analyses, it remains possible that maternal intrusiveness and financial means may differentially impact disorder-specific anxiety symptom clusters across childhood. Future work should investigate such relationships to optimally inform the tailored treatment of specific anxiety disorders in childhood. Third, though from a large and geographically diverse sample, children in the NICHD SECCYD typically had mothers who displayed low levels of intrusiveness. Thus, the ability to detect differences in the development of child anxiety related to maternal intrusiveness in the present community sample may have been compromised by under representation of the more highly intrusive mothers who may typically present in treatment clinics. Fourth, the predominantly Caucasian make-up of the sample may limit the generalizability of findings to other racial groups. Finally, the generalizability of findings to all parents is limited, as the present analyses included only mothers. Consistent with theoretical work positing fathers play a unique role in the etiology of child anxiety (Bögels & Phares, 2008), a separate analysis of fathers’ autonomy restricting parenting behavior in the NICHD SECCYD sample evidenced a divergent pattern with respect child anxiety (Cooper-Vince, Chan, Pincus, & Comer, 2013). Further work is needed to expand on these parenting differences and better understand specifically how different parenting roles (i.e., single parents, parents in same-sex relationships) may impact the relationship between parenting behavior and child anxiety.
Despite these limitations, the current findings provide a large, longitudinal portrait of the relationships between parental intrusiveness, financial means, and child anxiety across development in the community, and highlight the need to disseminate parent-focused treatments for early child anxiety, particularly to families of lower financial means. Such treatments coach parents to assume a less overprotective parenting posture by granting children more autonomy in age-appropriate situations and affording children gradual exposure to anxiety-provoking situations and their resolution. Future work is needed to investigate how other factors, such as peer relationships, neighborhood safety, and other parenting behavior may contribute to the development and maintenance of anxiety across childhood.
Acknowledgments
This project was supported by K23 MH090247. The authors wish to thank Dr. Timothy Brown for his helpful comments on earlier versions of this work.
References
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; Burlington, VT: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Allhusen V, Appelbaum M, Belsky J, Booth CL, Bradley R, Brownell C, Weinraub M. Nonmaternal care and family factors in early development: An overview of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2001;22:457–492. doi: 10.1016/s0193-3973(01)00092-2. [Google Scholar]
- Aschenbrand SG, Kendall PC. The effect of perceived child anxiety status on parental latency to intervene with anxious and nonanxious youth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80:232–238. doi: 10.1037/a0027230. doi: 10.1037/a0027230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bacchini D, Miranda MC, Affuso G. Effects of parental monitoring and exposure to community violence on antisocial behavior and anxiety/depression among adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2011;26:269–292. doi: 10.1177/0886260510362879. doi: 10.1177/0886260510362879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barber B. Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development. 1996;67:3296–3319. doi: 10.2307/1131780. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrett PM, Dadds MR, Rapee RM. Family treatment of childhood anxiety: A controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:333–342. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.2.333. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.2.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayer JK, Sanson AV, Hemphill SA. Parent influences on early childhood internalizing difficulties. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2006;27:542–559. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.002. [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Bell B, Bradley RH, Stallard N, Stewart-Brown SL. Socioeconomic risk, parenting during the preschool years and child health age 6 years. European Journal of Public Health. 2006;17:508–513. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckl261. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckl261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bogels S, Phares V. Fathers’ role in the etiology, prevention and treatment of child anxiety: a review and new model. Clinical Psychology Review. 2008;28:539–558. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.011. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bronfenbrenner U. Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology. 1986;22:20. doi: 0012-1649/86/S00.75. [Google Scholar]
- Brown-Jacobsen AM, Wallace DP, Whiteside SPH. Multimethod, multi-informant agreement, and positive predictive value in the identification of child anxiety disorders using the SCAS and ADIS-C. Assessment. 2011;18:382–392. doi: 10.1177/1073191110375792. doi: 10.1177/1073191110375792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner JC, Mezzacappa E, Beardslee WR. Characteristics of resilient youths living in poverty: the role of self-regulatory processes. Developmental Psychopatholgy. 2003;15:139–62. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burstein M, Ginsburg GS. The effect of parental modeling of anxious behaviors and cognitions in school-aged children: An experimental pilot study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2010;48:506–515. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.02.006. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chorpita BF, Barlow DH. The development of anxiety: The role of control in the early environment. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;124:3–21. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.3. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comer JS, Blanco C, Grant B, Hasin D, Liu SM, Turner JB, Olfson M. Health-related quality of life across the anxiety disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2011;72:43–50. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05094blu. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05094blu. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comer JS, Kendall PC. A symptom-level examination of parent-child agreement in the diagnosis of anxious youths. Journal of The American Academy Of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;43:878–886. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000125092.35109.c5. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000125092.35109.c5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comer JS, Puliafico AC, Aschenbrand SG, McKnight K, Robin JA, Goldfine ME, Albano A. A pilot feasibility evaluation of the CALM program for anxiety disorders in early childhood. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2012;26:40–49. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.011. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper-Vince C, Chan PT, Pincus D, Comer J. Paternal autonomy restriction, neighborhood safety, and child anxiety across time in a large multiphase sample of community youth. 2013. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Costello E, Mustillo S, Erkanli A, Keeler G, Angold A. Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:837–844. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dadds M, Barrett P. Psychological management of anxiety disorders in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:999–1011. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00798. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duckworth AL, Quinn PD, Tsukayama E. What No Child Left Behind leaves behind: The roles of IQ and self-control in predicting standardized achievement test scores and report card grades. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012;104:439–451. doi: 10.1037/a0026280. doi:10.1037/a0026280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumas J, LaFreniere P, Serketich W. “Balance of Power”: A transactional analysis of control in mother-child dyads involving socially competent, aggressive, and anxious children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:104–113. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.1.104. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebesutani C, Bernstein A, Nakamura BJ, Chorpita BF, Higa-McMillan CK, Weisz JR. Concurrent validity of the Child Behavior Checklist DSM-Oriented Scales: Correspondence with DSM diagnoses and comparison to syndrome scales. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2010;32:373–384. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9174-9. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9174-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edison SC, Evans MA, McHolm AE, Cunningham CE, Nowakowski ME, Boyle M, Schmidt LA. An investigation of control among parents of selectively mute, anxious, and non-anxious children. Child Psychiatry Human Development. 2011;42:270–290. doi: 10.1007/s10578-010-0214-1. doi: 10.1007/s10578-010-0214-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egeland B, Heister M. Teaching task rating scales. Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Eley TC, Napolitano M, Lau JY, Gregory AM. Does childhood anxiety evoke maternal control? A genetically informed study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:772–779. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02227.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02227.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Essau C, Conradt J, Petermann F. Frequency, comorbidity, and psychosocial impairment of anxiety disorders in German adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2000;14:263–79. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(99)00039-0. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(99)00039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Essex MJ, Kraemer HC, Armstrong JM, Boyce WT, Goldsmith HH, Klein MH, Kupfer DJ. Exploring risk factors for the emergence of children's mental health problems. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:246–56. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1246. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ezpeleta L, Keeler G, Erkanli A, Costello E, Angold A. Epidemiology of psychiatric disability in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:901–914. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00786. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng X, Shaw DS, Moilanen KL. Parental negative control moderates the shyness–emotion regulation pathway to school-age internalizing symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:425–436. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9469-z. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9469-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant KE, Compas BE, Stuhlmacher AF, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Halpert JA. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:447–66. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray CMK, Carter R, Silverman WK. Anxiety symptoms in African American Children: Relations with ethnic pride, anxiety sensitivity, and parenting. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2010;20:205–213. doi: 10.1007/s10826-010-9422-3. [Google Scholar]
- Hale WW, III, Engels R, Meeus W. Adolescent's perceptions of parenting behaviours and its relationship to adolescent generalized anxiety disorder symptoms. Journal of Adolescence. 2006;29:407–417. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.002. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hay I, Ashman AF. The development of adolescents’ emotional stability and general self-concept: The interplay of parents, peers, and gender. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education. 2003;50:77–91. doi: 10.1080/1034912032000053359. [Google Scholar]
- Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Masek B, Henin A, Blakely LR, Pollock-Wurman RA, McQuade J, Biederman J. Cognitive behavioral therapy for 4- to 7-year-old children with anxiety disorders: a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78:498–510. doi: 10.1037/a0019055. doi: 10.1037/a0019055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudson J, Comer J, Kendall P. Parental responses to positive and negative emotions in anxious and nonanxious children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2008;37:303–313. doi: 10.1080/15374410801955839. doi: 10.1080/15374410801955839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudson J, Rapee R. Parent-child interactions in clinically anxious children and their siblings. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2002;31:548–555. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3104_13. doi: 10.1207/153744202320802214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson JG, Cohen P, Dohrenwend BP, Link BG, Brook JS. A longitudinal investigation of social causation and social selection processes involved in the association between socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:490–9. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.3.490. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.108.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendall PC, Hudson J, Gosch E, Flannery-Schroeder E, Suveg C. Cognitive–behavioral therapy for anxiety disordered youth: A randomized clinical trial evaluating child and family modalities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76:282–297. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.282. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington M, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Prior juvenile diagnosis in adults with mental disorder: Developmental follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:709–717. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.709. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovacs M, Gatsonis C, Paulauskas S, Richards C. Depressive disorders in childhood. IV. A longitudinal study of comorbidity with and risk for anxiety disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1989;46:776–782. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810090018003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kushner M, Sher K, Beitman B. The relation between alcohol problems and anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1990;147:685–695. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.6.685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manassis K, Tannock R, Monga S. Anxious by maternal- versus self-report: Are they the same children? Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;18:103–109. doi: 10.1037/t03542-000. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLeod BD, Wood JJ, Weisz JR. Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:155–172. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist. 1998;53:185–204. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.185. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merikangas KR, Ames M, Cui L, Stang PE, Ustun TB, Von Korff M, Kessler RC. The impact of comorbidity of mental and physical conditions on role disability in the U.S. adult household population. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1180–1188. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1180. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merikangas K, He J, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, Swendsen J. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication-Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of The American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2010;49:980–989. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merikangas K, Nakamura E, Kessler R. Epidemiology of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 2009;11:7–20. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2009.11.1/krmerikangas. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muris P, van Brakel AM, Arntz A, Schouten E. Behavioral inhibition as a risk factor for the development of childhood anxiety disorders: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2011;20:157–170. doi: 10.1007/s10826-010-9365-8. doi: 10.1007/s10826-010-9365-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus 6.0 [Computer software] Author; Los Angeles, CA: 1998–2010. [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura BJ, Ebesutani C, Bernstein A, Chorpita BF. A psychometric analysis of the Child Behavior Checklist DSM-Oriented Scales. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2009;31:178–189. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9174-9. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9119-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [NICHD] NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development: Phase II Instrument Document. (n.d.)
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [NICHD] NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development: Orientation to the study. ICPSR Summer Course. 2009 Retrieved from http://lecb.physics.lsa.umich.edu/wl/carma/2009/20090708-ICPSR/20090708-umwlcd0011-090818/real/f001.htm.
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network [NICHD ECCRN] Early child care and children's development prior to school entry: Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. American Educational Research Journal. 2002;39:133–164. doi:10.3102/00028312039001133. [Google Scholar]
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network [NICHD ECCRN] Duration and developmental timing of poverty and children's cognitive and social development from birth through third grade. Child Development. 2005;76:795–810. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00878.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00878.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nebbitt VE, Lambert SF. Correlates of anxiety sensitivity among African American adolescents living in urban public housing. Journal of Community Psychology. 2009;37:268–280. doi: 10.1002/jcop.20292. [Google Scholar]
- Pincus DB, Ehrenreich JT, Santucci L, Eyberg S. The implementation of modified Parent-Child Interaction Therapy for youth with separation anxiety disorder. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 2008;15:118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2007.08.002. [Google Scholar]
- Rapee RM, Kennedy SJ, Ingram M, Edwards SL, Sweeney L. Altering the trajectory of anxiety in at-risk young children. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;167:1518–1525. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09111619. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09111619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin K, Cheah C, Fox N. Emotion regulation, parenting and display of social reticence in preschoolers. Early Education & Development. 2001;12:97–115. doi: 10.1207/s15566935eed1201_6. [Google Scholar]
- Siqueland L, Kendall P, Steinberg L. Anxiety in children: Perceived family environments and observed family interaction. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1996;25:225–237. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2502_12. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Elmer JD, Mounts NS. Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Development. 1989;60:1424–1436. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb04014.x. doi: 10.2307/1130932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss C, Frame C, Forehand R. Psychosocial impairment associated with anxiety in children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1987;16:235–239. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp1603_8. [Google Scholar]
- van der Bruggen CO, Stams GM, Bögels SM, Paulussen-Hoogeboom MC. Parenting behaviour as a mediator between young children's negative emotionality and their anxiety/depression. Infant And Child Development. 2010;19:354–365. doi: 10.1037/t05150-000. [Google Scholar]
- Van Zalk N, Van Zalk MHW, Kerr M. Socialization of social anxiety in adolescent crowds. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:1239–1249. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9533-3. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9533-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittaker JEV, Harden BJ, See HM, Meisch AD, Westbrook TR. Family risks and protective factors: Pathways to Early Head Start toddlers’ social–emotional functioning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2011;26:74–86. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.04.007. [Google Scholar]
- Wood J. Parental intrusiveness and children's separation anxiety in a clinical sample. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2006;37:73–87. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0021-x. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0021-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood JJ, McLeod BD, Sigman M, Hwang WC, Chu BC. Parenting and childhood anxiety: Theory, empirical findings, and future directions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:134–151. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00106. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood JJ, Piacentini JC, Southam-Gerow M, Chu BC, Sigman M. Family cognitive behavioral therapy for child anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:314–321. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000196425.88341.b0. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000196425.88341.b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]