Abstract
Memory for odour information may result from temporal coupling between the olfactory and hippocampal systems. Respiration defines the frequency of olfactory perception, but how the respiratory rate affects hippocampal oscillations remains poorly understood. The afferent connectivity of the medial septum/diagonal band of Broca complex (MS/DB) proposes this region as a crossroads between respiratory and limbic pathways. Here we investigate if the firing rates of septal neurons integrate respiratory rate signals. We demonstrate that approximately 50% of MS/DB neurons are temporally correlated with sniffing frequency. Moreover, a group of slow-spiking septal neurons are phase-locked to the sniffing cycle. We show that inter-burst intervals of MS/DB theta cells relate to the sniff rate. Intranasal odour infusion evokes sniff phase preference for the activity of fast-spiking MS/DB neurons. Concurrently, the infusion augments the correlation between sniffing and limbic theta oscillations. During periods of sniffing–theta correlation, CA1 place cells fired preferentially during the inhalation phase, suggesting the theta cycle as a coherent time frame for central olfactory processing. Furthermore, injection of the GABAergic agonist muscimol into medial septum induces a parallel decrease of sniffing and theta frequencies. Our findings provide experimental evidence that MS/DB does not merely generate theta rhythm, but actively integrates sensorimotor stimuli that reflect sniffing rate. Such integration may provide temporal oscillatory synchronisation of MS/DB-innervated limbic structures with the sniffing cycle.
Keywords: hippocampus, medial septum, respiration, theta rhythm
Introduction
One of the main sources of information for rodents about the surrounding environment is provided by the olfactory system. Olfactory perception has two components: (i) motor – the generation of fast respiratory rhythm (sniffing), and (ii) sensory – the decoding of odour stimuli. Activity in the olfactory bulb (OB) is periodically modulated by the animal∼s respiration; the phasic activity of bulbar neurons is dependent on nasal airflow (Ravel et al., 1987; Charpak et al., 2001; Spors & Grinvald, 2002; Buonviso et al., 2003; Scott, 2006). Thus, the rate of olfactory perception depends on the generation of respiratory rhythm.
Tagging odours to locations may be an important mechanism by which rodents navigate (Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Robitsek et al., 2008). The hippocampus encodes spatial representations of the environment, and changes in hippocampal physiology have also been correlated with respiration (Kay, 2005). The relationship between hippocampal theta rhythm (6–12 Hz) and automatic behaviours such as whisking and sniffing has been continuously debated (Kepecs et al., 2006). The current understanding is that theta and whisking/sniffing are independent oscillators, which may synchronise in the presence of a strong sensorimotor stimulus (Berg et al., 2006; Buonviso et al., 2006). Theta rhythm correlates with sniffing during odour contingency reversal learning (Macrides et al., 1982). Hippocampal theta also intermittently couples with OB oscillations during exploratory behaviour (Vanderwolf, 1992). The firing of single interneurons in the hippocampus is linked to performance in an olfactory identification task (Wiebe & Staubli, 2001). Thus, theta oscillation might provide the timing frequency of respiratory and limbic integration (Kepecs et al., 2006). On a systems level, however, there are different theories suggesting how these activities are synchronised and the network of centrifugal olfactory processing remains largely unexplored (Wachowiak, 2011).
Anatomical and physiological evidence suggest the medial septum/diagonal band of Broca complex (MS/DB, the target of our investigation here) as the key structure involved in the integration of respiration rate and limbic theta oscillations. First, the MS/DB receives ascending projections from cholinergic neurons in the laterodorsal and pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei (pontine respiratory group) (Fibiger, 1982; Satoh & Fibiger, 1986; Jones & Beaudet, 1987; Woolf & Butcher, 1989). The same pontine nuclei send descending inputs to the dorsal and ventral respiratory groups (Jones, 1990; Semba & Fibiger, 1992; Kubin & Fenik, 2004) that generate the respiratory rhythm (Smith et al., 1991; Onimaru & Homma, 2003). Second, GABAergic and cholinergic neurons in MS/DB play a crucial role in the regulation of hippocampal activity (Vertes & Kocsis, 1997; Buzsáki, 2002).
In turn, MS/DB efferents diverge across several limbic structures, where they regulate the amplitude and frequency of local field theta oscillations. Theta rhythm entrains hippocampal pyramidal neurons; the spiking rate of place cells relates to the phase of theta rhythm (O∼Keefe & Recce, 1993; Skaggs et al., 1996). In the present study, we performed single-unit extracellular recordings in MS/DB and hippocampal formation and evaluated neuronal activity in relation to the respiratory rate and phase in behaving rats. Here, we investigate if the septal region integrates olfactory sensorimotor processing and if inactivation of MS/DB reflects concurrent alterations in respiration and theta frequencies. We suggest that the coordination of hippocampal neurons with sensorimotor olfactory processing is mediated by MS/DB-generated theta rhythm.
Materials and methods
Surgical implantation of electrodes
The surgical implantation and the single-unit recordings were performed as previously described (Tsanov et al., 2011). Tetrodes were implanted in MS/DB (+0.5 AP, −1.1 ML, angle 10° medially and 5.5 mm dorsoventral to dura) and hippocampus (−3.8 AP, −2.3 ML and 1.8 mm dorsoventral to dura) of male Lister-Hooded rats (Harlan, UK). Experiments were conducted in accordance with European Community directive 86/609/EC and the Cruelty to Animals Act, 1876, and were approved by the Irish Medical Board (project authorisation number: AE19136/P003). The study followed Bioresources Ethics Committee, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland, and international guidelines of good practice.
Measurement of sniffing
We implanted a 22-gauge hollow guide cannula (C313G; Plastics One, Roanoke, VA, USA) for intranasal pressure measurements (Verhagen et al., 2007). During the surgery, the intranasal cannula was lowered into the dorsal recess (frontal–nasal fissure: 0 mm AP, ML: 0.9 mm, angle toward midline: 4°) and fixed to the rest of the headstage with dental cement. After recovery, sniffing was continuously recorded throughout all recording sessions by connecting the guide cannula implanted in the dorsal recess to a pressure sensor (0-1PSIG 24PCAFA6G; Farnell Ltd, Leeds, UK) via PE tubing (0.45 m × 0.1 mm ID × 0.15 mm OD). Sensor output was amplified 100×, low-pass filtered at 100 Hz, connected to the preamplifier and synchronised with tetrode recordings.
Recording techniques
After at least 1 week∼s recovery, subjects were connected, via a 32-channel headstage (Axona Ltd, St Albans, UK), to a recording system, which also allowed for animal position tracking. Signals were amplified (× 10000–30000) and band-pass filtered between 380 Hz and 6 kHz for single-unit detection. Candidate waveforms were discriminated off-line using graphical cluster-cutting software (Axona), which allows waveform separation based on multiple features including spike amplitude, spike duration and spike voltage. Autocorrelation histograms were calculated for each unit, and the unit was removed from further analysis if the histogram revealed the existence of correlations within the first 2 ms (refractory period), inconsistent with good unit isolation. Local field potential (LFP) recordings and analysis were performed as previously described (Tsanov et al., 2011). We measured LFP from the tetrode wires with the lowest impedance (50–250 KΩ) and for LFP analysis we used a band-pass filter (< 15 Hz).
Recording sessions
The recordings took place in a square arena (64 × 64 × 25 cm high) and/or rectangle-shaped linear track (64 × 64 × 9.5 cm and wide) situated in the centre of a room with multiple background cues available. Rats were placed in the open field and 20-mg food pellets (Formula 5TUL; TestDiet, St Louis, MO, USA) were thrown in every 20 s to random locations within the open field; in this way, animals locomoted continuously, allowing for complete sampling of the environment. The duration of each experimental session was 16 min.
Criteria for data inclusion
Cells were selected on the basis of their firing frequency, rhythmicity of their firing and spike isolation. We identified 334 well-isolated units within the MS/DB from seven rats and 157 well-isolated units within hippocampal area CA1 from five rats (250–350 g). Based on post-mortem histological criteria, we estimated that 150 of these units were from medial septum, and 184 were from diagonal band of Broca. Units from MS/DB with mean firing frequency < 10 Hz were classified as slow-spiking, and with mean firing frequency > 10 Hz as fast-spiking. Bursting units with an autocorrelogram theta index value of > 0.1 were classified as theta units (Tsanov et al., 2011). Bursts were defined as a sequence of three or more spikes with an inter-spike interval of 4 ms (Ramcharan et al., 2005). Spike trains were defined as a sequence of three or more spikes with an inter-spike interval of < 10 ms and a 15- to 250-ms pause, preceding the first spike of the train (Tsanov et al., 2011). Single hippocampal pyramidal cells and interneurons were identified using spike shape and firing frequency characteristics (Wilson & McNaughton, 1993; Csicsvari et al., 1999). Place fields were identified as described before (Wang et al., 2012). The firing map reflected the frequency of firing calculated from the top firing bins down to the bins with lowest firing rate by scaling them in decreasing intervals of 20% of the peak firing (Hollup et al., 2001; Brun et al., 2002).
Phase analyses
Each spike was assigned a position on the sniffing phase and/or theta phase (band-pass filtered LFP for CA1 recordings) between 0 and 360° (Mehta et al., 2002; Siapas et al., 2005). The peaks of the sniffing or theta signals were detected using the gradient for each cycle (defined as the signal between two successive peaks). A sine wave was fitted to the signal using the least square method. This fitted sine wave was then used to estimate the phase of sniffing or theta signal at each sample point, similar to previous procedures (O∼Keefe & Recce, 1993). Rayleigh∼s test of uniformity was used to test phase distributions for deviations from the circular uniform distribution (Fisher, 1993). The circular concentration coefficient, kappa k, was also employed to quantify the phase locking (Siapas et al., 2005). Higher values of k correspond to narrower distributions around a mean preferred phase (Jones & Wilson, 2005). The k value was calculated using Matlab circular statistics tool box (http://philippberens.wordpress.com/code/circstats/). Phase precession of place cells in CA1 on rectangle-shaped linear tracks was analysed over the length of a cell∼s place field. As such, the field size imposes natural bounds on the times and positions over which precession can occur. Linear regression was used to quantify the position vs. phase correlations that arise as a consequence of phase precession, calculated using Pearson∼s correlation coefficient (r2).
Correlation analysis
We used similarity measure analysis (Lyttle & Fellous, 2011) to examine the correlation between inter-spike intervals and sniff cycles. We performed a correlation analysis between inter-spike distance and the timing between two sniff cycles, using a modification of the similarity measuring method (Lyttle & Fellous, 2011). The sniffing signal was transformed in series of sine waves, and the entire signal was represented by their phase. This approach transforms the signal from a sine waveform to series of functions that linearly increase between sniffing cycles. Similarly, spike timing is transformed in a series of function that increase linearly with time (Lyttle & Fellous, 2011). Assuming a spike train with timings d = d1, d2, … dn, where n is the number of spikes detected for the cell during the recording, the transformation g(t) is, where t represents time:
We selected the maximum inter-spike interval that considers two spikes as one event, τ at 10 ms, to ascertain that spike trains are considered as one event. Finally, Pearson correlations were calculated between both signals at time lags to determine the inter-spike intervals and sniffing cycle relationship. A value of one at time zero would indicate a perfect relationship between the two signals.
The peak values of the cross-correlograms are positive when the spikes correlate with the peaks of the respiration cycle (0/360°), negative when the spikes correlate to the troughs of the respiration cycle (180°) and when the spikes that correlate with degrees from the descending (90°) or ascending (270°) slope of the cycle the peak values contain negative and positive components.
Coherence analyses
We calculated the coherence between sniff and the hippocampal LFP, using NeuroSpec (Halliday et al., 1995), version 2.0 for MATLAB. In the time domain, the cumulant density function was estimated from the cross-spectrum, via Fourier transform. The coherence uses the method of disjoint sections, where the recording (R) is divided into L non-overlapping epochs, each with time length T, where R = LT. The time length of T is approximately 2 s and T is equal to 512 samples, while R is the length of the analysed recoding, i.e. 16 min. For the segmented signal x, Discrete Fourier Transforms is used to estimate the auto-spectrums of the signals fxx(λ), where λ is a particular frequency. The linear relationship (coherence) between signals x and y is Rxy, which is estimated using auto and cross spectrums:
Shuffling spike analysis
To test the statistical significance of phase preference of neuronal spiking activity, we performed a spiking shuffle analysis (Louie & Wilson, 2001; Brandon et al., 2012). The spike times were divided into bins of 0.004 s, corresponding to the sampling frequency of LFP and sniffing signals. These spiking times were then randomly permutated to remove temporal alignment while maintaining the overall activity of the cell. The cell recording was then compared with the maximal random shuffled value.
We calculated the normal distribution of the firing frequency of the recorded neurons as a function the sniff phase (0–360°). For this the phase was binned into segments with bin size of 20°, resulting in 18 phase bins. One thousand permutations were used to estimate the firing frequency for all bins with mean value and standard deviation. The Z-score of the recorded spikes was calculated by subtracting the mean shuffled frequency from the recorded firing frequency and dividing over the shuffled standard deviation. The threshold z score of α level 1.96 corresponds to a probability of less than 5% and expresses the statistical significance for phase preference. Therefore, if Z ≥ 1.96 or ≤ −1.96, the probability that the observed firing frequency is consistently phase-locked is ≤ 0.05.
Odor delivery
Saturated odour vapour was produced by flowing clean (carbon-filtered) air through disposable syringe filters which were loaded with 30 μL liquid odorant (Verhagen et al., 2007). The flow rate of odorant streams (vanilla scent) was maintained at level of 50 mL/min, and a variable air flow dilution of 5% was produced.
Pharmacological manipulation
Muscimol, a GABA(A) agonist, was diluted in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) to 0.5 μg/mL (Shirvalkar et al., 2010; Brandon et al., 2011). Prior to an infusion, the dummy cannula was removed to allow access to the guide cannula. A unilateral injector cannula, cut to a length 1 mm longer than the guide cannula, was filled with the muscimol solution and lowered through the guide cannula and into the MS/DB. A microinfusion pump infused 0.5 μL of muscimol solution at 0.1 μL/min. The injector cannula remained connected for 1 min after the infusion to allow for the drug to perfuse through the neural tissue. The injector was removed and a dummy cannula was secured to the guide cannula. Recordings were continued 15 min after muscimol injection. The animals received a muscimol injection only once. At the end of the study brains were removed for histological verification.
Statistical analyses
All data were analysed using Prism software (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA, USA). Statistical significance was estimated by using a two-tailed t-test, non-parametric Watson–Williams test and two-way analysis of variance (anova) paired with post hoc Newman–Keuls test. The probability level interpreted as statistically significant was P < 0.05. All data points are given as mean ± SEM.
Results
To monitor sniffing activity, we implanted a pressure sensor in nasal dorsal medial meatus of 12 rats, allowing us to measure the pressure changes between inspired and expired air (Fig. 1A). In parallel, we implanted a microdrive with eight tetrodes that were lowered through the MS/DB (Fig. 1B). After 1 week of recovery, we recorded simultaneously respiration and the spiking activity of MS/DB in behaving animals (Fig. 1C and D). The recordings were performed in a square arena, which the animals explored during a pellet-chasing task (for 16 min). We identified 334 well-isolated units, histologically assigned to MS/DB (for regional distribution see Materials and methods), of which 175 (52%) fired in relation to the animal∼s sniffing frequency. To evaluate how spiking activity of the recorded units relates to sniffing, we performed two types of analyses: (i) distribution of the spikes throughout the sniffing phase (between 0 and 360°) and (ii) correlations between the inter-spike interval and the rate of sniffing cycles. We also analysed the polarity of spiking with respect to the main sniffing frequency bands (Fig. 1C). Based on their firing frequency and rhythmicity, we grouped the sniff-modulated units into slow-spiking, fast-spiking and theta groups (Fig. 1E).
Phase-lock of slow-spiking MS/DB units to the sniff phase
The slow-spiking units (64/19%) are cells with a low average firing frequency (3.1 ± 0.3 Hz, Fig. 2A). They were significantly phase-locked to the sniffing cycle, where each cell expressed an individual phase preference (Fig. 2D). The statistical significance of phase-locking was evaluated using Rayleigh∼s statistic (Fisher, 1993). We compared circular concentration coefficient k values from recorded spikes (Jones & Wilson, 2005; Siapas et al., 2005) to k values from the spikes when shuffled (Brandon et al., 2012) within time bins of 0.004 s (Fig. 2B, left). As expected, the shuffled spikes expressed no phase-preference, with k values significantly lower compared with recorded spikes (P < 0.001, Watson–Williams test). To estimate the null distribution of septal firing frequency across the sniff phase we used 1000 shuffles. After we found the upper 95% value we compared the recorded data using Z-scores (see Methods). For Z scores calculation the mean shuffled frequency of 1000 permutations was subtracted from the neuronal firing frequency for all degrees (with phase bin of 20°) and divided over the shuffled standard deviation. The threshold α level of 1.96 (marked with red dashed lines in Fig. 2) corresponds to a probability of less 5% and expresses the statistical significance for phase preference. Slow-spiking units show a high number of significant Z-scores (Fig. 2C, left). Concurrently, septal units with no phase preference show Z-scores only within the non-significant range (Fig. 2C, right).
The transition from resting respiration (0–5 Hz) to active odour sampling (5–12 Hz, typically termed ‘sniffing’) can occur in a single respiration cycle. As respiration rates show a continuous distribution throughout the recording session (Verhagen et al., 2007), we use ‘sniffing’ to refer to all respiratory activity regardless of frequency (Kepecs et al., 2006). Similar to previous findings (Kepecs et al., 2007), the frequency histogram from our recordings revealed three major sniffing frequency bands. We then analysed the polar distribution of the spikes across the frequencies in the low (0–5 Hz), intermediate (5–9 Hz) and high frequency range (9–12 Hz) (Fig. 3; Fig. S1). We found that the polarity in the range 9–12 Hz was characterised by the highest k values (0.47 ± 0.03, Fig. 2B, right), which are significantly higher (P < 0.05, Watson–Williams test) compared with the non-filtered (0–12 Hz) k values of the recorded spikes (0.31 ± 0.02).
Temporal entrainment of fast-spiking and theta MS/DB activity to the sniffing cycle
We next analysed the group of fast-spiking units (45/13%), characterised by a high average firing frequency (26.3 ± 2.2 Hz), and the group of septal ‘theta’ cells (66/20%; average firing frequency 35.5 ± 2.8 Hz). Unlike for the slow-spiking units, the relation to the sniffing cycle of fast-spiking units is less obvious when visualised on raw data traces (Fig. 4A). These neurons tend to group their spikes in clusters and, in addition, they fire in bursts. We examined separately the bursts with an inter-spike interval ≤ 4 ms and the spike trains with an inter-spike interval of ≥ 4 ms and ≤ 10 ms (Fig. 4A, red-marked inset).
Theta cells are the most representative cell type of the medial septum (Ranck, 1973; Stewart & Fox, 1989b; King et al., 1998), related to its functional role as a theta rhythm generator (Bland & Bland, 1986; Stewart & Fox, 1989a). The main feature of theta cells in regard to sniffing is their ability to burst with a frequency similar to the sniffing rate for several seconds (Fig. 4B, left). During epochs with slow sniffing (0–5 Hz), characterised behaviourally by lower levels of arousal, theta units disengage from their rhythmic bursting, thereby reducing the degree of coupling with the sniff rate (Fig. 4B, right). We also analysed their activity for all spikes, spike trains and bursts (Fig. 4B, red-marked inset). Z-score analysis of fast-spiking (Fig. 4C) and theta units (Fig. 4D) shows that the firing frequency of both cell types expresses a sufficient number of Z-scores above the α level threshold of 1.96, corresponding to a probability of more than 95% for phase preference. Although the number of significant counts was less compared with the slow-spiking units the observed values reveal that a fraction of the fast spiking and theta firing frequency is consistently phase-locked to the sniff cycle.
The phase polarity of fast-spiking cells was less well expressed and k showed smaller values for all recorded spikes, compared with slow-spiking units (Fig. 5A and G, left). The distribution of the spike trains and bursts across the sniff cycle was phase-dependent (Fig. 5B and C), with higher k values (0.19 ± 0.02 for spike trains and 0.43 ± 0.01 for bursts, Fig. 5G middle and right, respectively) compared with shuffled spikes (P < 0.001, Watson–Williams test). Thus, spike trains/bursts define the relation of this group of cells to the sniffing cycle. Another difference between fast-spiking units and slow-spiking units is the lack of a phase-lock preference for the high theta sniffing range (9–12 Hz). A similar degree of phase polarity is expressed for the three sniff frequency ranges (0.11 ± 0.07 for 0–5 Hz; 0.10 ± 0.12 for 5–9 Hz; 0.14 ± 0.09 for 9–12 Hz; Fig. S2). Theta cells show a spike preference with the sniff phase, with k values for all recorded spikes significantly higher than the shuffled spikes (P < 0.001, Watson–Williams test, Fig. 5D and H, left). Although theta spike trains and bursts (Fig. 5E and F) revealed even higher k values (0.11 ± 0.01 and 0.16 ± 0.02, respectively; Fig. 5H middle and right), their phase polarity was less prominent compared with the slow- and fast-spiking units (Fig. S3).
Correlation analysis of septal spiking and sniffing rate
Another approach to detecting parallel patterns between neuronal spiking and an oscillation is to correlate the inter-spike intervals with the duration of oscillatory cycles. This correlation reveals if there is a consistent temporal match between these parameters and the time-shift between them (Fig. 6A and B). We correlated inter-spike intervals of septal neurons with sniffing, and compared the result with the correlation of the shuffled spikes and sniffing cycles. We found a peak in the cross-correlogram for the recorded spikes (Fig. 6C, red), but not for the shuffled spikes (Fig. 6C, blue). Interestingly, the slow-spiking units were characterised by a second peak at about −500 ms (with regard to the x-axis). Both correlation peaks were shifted by −30 ± 10 ms from time 0 and time lag −500, respectively. Thus, the inter-spike intervals are shifted from 20 to 40 ms, by consistently matching sniff cycles. Importantly, slow-spiking neurons repeat this correlated firing pattern after 500 ms. This characteristic feature suggests that the slow-spiking units correlate their rate with the sniffing every 4–5 cycles on average. The correlation peaks of different neurons revealed no preference for the sign of correlation, which can be negative or positive. The mean value of the correlation peaks for all cases (0.021 ± 0.003) was significantly higher (P < 0.01, t-test) than the mean correlation peak for shuffled spikes (0.010 ± 0.002). The interval of about 30 ms between lag 0 and the first correlation peak shows that the slow-spiking units are modulated by respiration with an oligosynaptic time delay.
The correlation between the inter-spike interval and the sniffing cycle revealed a characteristic profile of the fast-spiking units (Fig. 6D). It shows a single negative/positive peak, which was shifted to −100 ± 10 ms from time lag 0. Therefore, the inter-spike intervals (which are also set by the spike trains and bursts) of these neurons are preceded by 90–110 ms by the corresponding sniff rate. The peaks revealed high correlation values (0.034 ± 0.005), compared with the shuffled spikes correlation values (0.011 ± 0.003) (P < 0.001, t-test). The ability of theta cells to burst with a frequency similar to the sniffing rate is expressed through the higher values of the inter-spike-sniff correlation (0.038 ± 0.007). Furthermore, the correlation between inter-spike intervals and sniff cycles resulted in multiple positive and negative peaks (Fig. 6E). The distance between two positive (or two negative) peaks was in the range 80–130 ms, reflecting the bursting frequency of theta cells (8–12 Hz). The oscillation of correlation peaks ranged between +100 and −550 ms. This result suggests that theta bursting activity couples with the sniffing rate for about 0.5 s on average. The presence of several correlation peaks on both sides of lag 0 suggests that theta units integrate intrinsic burst oscillations with extrinsic oscillations of sniffing rate. The positioning of the majority of peaks within the negative part of the x-axis (left from time lag 0) reveals that the sniff itself initiates the coupling of both oscillations.
Olfaction-induced sniff-phase spiking preference of septal neurons parallels correlations between the sniff rate and the hippocampal theta rhythm
Septal sniff-modulated units express a higher degree of phase relation to the sniff cycle during higher sniff frequencies (see Figs 2B right and 3). Olfactory perception is intimately linked to the sniffing rate (Wesson et al., 2009) and one might hypothesise that the coupling between sniffing and spiking activity of MS/DB neurons is promoted by olfactory processing. We examined this hypothesis explicitly by infusing odorant through the nasal cannula and evaluating MS/DB neuronal firing. We compared spiking activity in the first 3 s after odour infusion to the preceding 3 s (Fig. 7A). Our choice of the post-infusion epoch duration was based on the finding that odour encoding of mitral cells in the OB occurs in dynamic timescales involving several sniffing cycles (Bathellier et al., 2008). The increase of sniffing frequency in the high theta range (9–12 Hz) for post-infusion epochs confirmed behaviourally that the odour was detected by the animals (Fig. 7E). To examine if the firing of fast-spiking neurons is linked to olfactory perception, we compared the polarity of all spikes in relation to the sniff phase before and after the odour infusion. Because different units show phase preference to different degrees (between 0 and 360°) we first normalised the polarity of all units. For this we aligned the peak firing frequency of each unit to 180° (see normalisation in Materials and methods). The averaged values of all normalised polarities prior to the odour infusion (Fig. 7B, blue trace) were compared with the averaged values of all normalised polarities after the odour infusion (Fig. 7B, red trace). The significant increase of phase polarity expressed by post hoc analysis of variance (P < 0.05; Newman–Keuls test) was confirmed with higher values of circular concentration coefficient k for post- (0.103 ± 0.011), compared with pre-infusion epochs (0.071 ± 0.008; P < 0.05, Watson–Williams test, Fig. 7B, inset). To determine the change of the fast-spiking patterns after odour infusion, we also compared the inter-spike interval with pre-infusion epochs. Increased numbers of inter-spike intervals were evident in the range 80–90 ms, of the high theta frequency of 10–12 Hz (Fig. 7D). These data show that the coupling of MS/DB neuronal activity to an animal∼s sniffing is augmented after olfactory stimulation.
Relevant olfactory input can reset the phase of limbic theta oscillation, and produce a short-lasting increase of coherence between theta rhythm and respiration. The septal region has the anatomical connectivity to integrate sniffing and theta because (i) it is connected with the pontine respiratory nuclei as well as with the olfactory cortex, and (ii) it innervates all limbic areas, and can induce simultaneous regulation of theta oscillations across multiple hippocampal and entorhinal areas. Thus, we aimed to determine if septal activity synchronises respiration and theta rhythm (although we are not aiming here to differentiate if odour-induced sniffing or fast sniffing frequency per se triggers this process). While the slow-spiking cells are constantly synchronised with the sniffing cycle, the fast-spiking and theta cells show temporal coupling. Such restricted coupling might be functionally linked to the coordination of two oscillators. Sniffing and limbic theta rhythm are oscillations with similar frequency in the range 5–12 Hz but with different peaks, suggesting that they are independent oscillators. The coupling between two independent oscillators could be coincidental or causal. To test the hypothesis that there is a causal relationship between theta and sniffing we examined if odour-potentiated phase-preference of fast-spiking parallels a change in the correlation between sniffing and hippocampal theta rhythm. We compared the changes in sniffing and LFP (band-pass filtered) for pre- and post-infusion epochs (Fig. 7C). Odour detection increased sniffing frequency (two-way anova, F1,12 = 9.12, P < 0.05, n = 8) in the range of 9–10 Hz for post-infusion epochs (Fig. 7E, Newman–Keuls test, P < 0.05). Importantly, the correlation between theta LFP and sniffing reached values of about 150%, compared with pre-infusion controls (P < 0.05, Mann–Whitney test, Fig. 7F).
Hippocampal local field and single-unit activity relates to the sniff phase
The medial septum sends dense projections to the hippocampal formation and if hippocampal neurons are entrained by the MS/DB-generated theta rhythm, then correlations between theta and sniffing should reflect hippocampal place cell activity. To examine this proposal, we first compared the hippocampal LFP, recorded from the CA1 region of five rats (Fig. 8A), with sniff dynamics during pellet-chasing in an open square arena, to which the animals were habituated. Food-foraging behaviour evoked the frequent occurrence of epochs with a high correlation between the LFP and sniffing (Fig. 8B) with a peak of coherence in the theta range, with maximum at 10 Hz (0.26 ± 0.02, Fig. 8C). We next examined if the spiking of hipppocampal neurons reflects the coupling of the LFP to sniffing during epochs of significant theta–sniff correlation.
We compared the phase relation of 52 place cells and 38 interneurons to the sniff-cycle between epochs with significant (P < 0.05) and epochs with non-significant (P > 0.05) theta–sniff correlation (see Materials and methods). The significant and non-significant correlation epochs were equally distributed in the maze without relation to an animal∼s trajectory. The significant correlation epochs were predominantly expressed during locomotion of 10–20 cm/s, which was the main running speed of the animals the during pellet chasing task. The identification of place cells was based on their spike shape, firing frequency and spatial properties (Fig. 8D). Because of their low firing frequency, the recording of one unit provides an insufficient number of spikes that can be compared with circular statistics to the shuffled data (Fig. S4). To increase the sampling number, we averaged the sniff polarity for all place cells for non-significant vs. significant correlation epochs. The non-correlation group showed no preference of the place cell spiking across the sniff cycle (Fig. 8E, left). The values of circular coefficient k for the non-correlation group did not show statistical significance compared with the k values of the shuffled place cells’ spikes (P = 0.28, Watson–Williams test). However, the correlation group revealed a preference of spiking for the range 180–240° (Fig. 8E, middle) and this observation was confirmed by a significantly higher circular coefficient k compared with non-correlation group (P < 0.05, Watson–Williams test, Fig. 8G, left). Importantly, Z-scores comparison of the place cell firing frequency for correlation (Fig. 8F, red bars) vs. non-correlation group (Fig. 8F, blue bars) showed a greater number of Z-scores above the significance threshold (marked with red dashed lines) for the sniff–theta correlation group. As the highest degree of sniff–theta coherence occurs at 10 Hz (Fig. 8C), and at this frequency place cells spike predominantly in bursts (complex-spike units entrained by theta), our next intention was to evaluate the sniff polarity for the bursts from all recorded epochs. The recorded bursts also showed a preference for sniff phase in the range 180–240° (Fig. 8E, right), with k significantly higher than the shuffled bursts (P < 0.01, Watson–Williams test, Fig. 8G, right). Therefore, place activity is tuned to the trough of the sniff cycle and this might be mediated through short-lasting synchronisation of local theta oscillators to the respiratory rate. Similarly, the sniff-phase polarity of theta-modulated hippocampal interneurons (Fig. 8H) was dependent on the degree of the sniff–LFP correlation. The significantly higher value of the circular coefficient k compared with the non-correlated epochs (P < 0.05, Watson–Williams test, Fig. 8I) suggests that rhythmic activity of these neurons is temporally reset by incoming inputs. Phase precession is an intrinsic feature of hippocampal neurons (O∼Keefe & Recce, 1993) and, despite the transient coupling between sniff and theta, the phase precession from place field recordings in linear tracks (Fig. 8J) correlated with theta (Fig. 8K, left, Pearson∼s r = −0.171, P < 0.01; n = 42) but not sniff oscillation (Fig. 8K, right; Pearson∼s r = 0.012, P = 0.16; n = 38). This result shows that the sniffing rate is synchronised for brief episodes with the hippocampal LFP, which are insufficient to form persistent coupling between sniff phase and place cell spiking.
Muscimol abolishes in parallel fast sniffing and high theta but preserves their relationship
Odour-induced sniffing promotes the correlation between sniffing and hippocampal LFP (Fig. 7), but the evidence showing the role of MS/DB in both sniffing rate and theta rhythm is insufficient. The coherence between sniffing and theta is shown to follow a linear pattern in relation to frequency, where the sniffs are timed to maintain a preferred latency relationship with theta rhythm during different theta–sniff frequencies (Macrides et al., 1982). To demonstrate the functional role of MS/DB in the frequency control of the sniffing–theta coherence, we performed unilateral muscimol microinfusions to attenuate septal activity pharmacologically (Shirvalkar et al., 2010; Brandon et al., 2011). The inactivation of medial septum in rats causes disruption of theta oscillations in the hippocampus (Rawlins et al., 1979; Mizumori et al., 1990) and impairs hippocampal-dependent memory (Winson, 1978; Givens & Olton, 1994). Fifteen minutes after muscimol injection, we monitored sniffing and concurrent local field activity in CA1 stratum radiatum from dorsal hippocampus. Interestingly, the coherence between sniffing and LFP was preserved, although it was shifted to a lower frequency range with peak of 6 Hz, compared with a pre-infusion peak of 10 Hz (Fig. 9A). After injection of muscimol into the MS/DB, the fast component of the sniffing frequency (9–12 Hz) was strongly attenuated, compared with baseline controls (Fig. 9B, anova, F1,8 = 13.75, P < 0.01, n = 4). In parallel, the hippocampal recordings showed a clear decrease of the local spectral power amplitude in the range of theta frequency (Fig. 9C and D). The peak of post-injection theta frequency was 7.5 Hz, compared with 8.5 Hz of pre-injection baseline (Fig. 9D, anova, F1,8 = 16.14, P < 0.01, n = 4). The coherent episodes between sniffing and theta from baseline recordings (Fig. 9E) were preserved after muscimol injection, although the amplitude and frequency of theta as well as the sniffing frequency were reduced (Fig. 9F). Muscimol injection also abolished the odour-triggered augmentation of the correlation between sniffing and LFP with no statistical difference between the pre- and post-infusion epochs correlation (P = 0.37, t-test, n = 4). These data show that muscimol-induced inhibition of MS/DB activity triggers parallel shifts of sniffing rate and hippocampal theta.
Discussion
We provide novel evidence here that a substantial population of neurons in the MS/DB discharge in synchrony with an animal∼s sniffing. Single-unit recordings in chronically implanted rats revealed that slow-spiking units are phase-locked to the sniffing cycle, while the spike trains and bursts of fast spiking and theta units expressed a preference to the sniff phase. Correlation analyses of septal inter-spike intervals and sniffing oscillations showed significant peaks of correlation for all cell types. We also demonstrate that inhibition of septal theta neurons reduces the frequency of sniffing-theta coherence, while odour infusion augments the correlation between sniffing and hippocampal theta rhythm.
Synchronous oscillatory activity functionally links remote neuronal populations or brain areas, providing a temporal window for transient communication (Fries, 2005). Olfactory neuronal and local field activity is locked to respiration (Cang & Isaacson, 2003; Fantana et al., 2008), and respiration can synchronise with hippocampal theta rhythm (Macrides et al., 1982). As a result, hippocampal theta and OB oscillations are correlated during performance of olfactory discrimination tasks (Kay, 2005; Martin et al., 2007). The activity in limbic structures has been linked to respiration (Harper et al., 1998), suggesting that phase synchronisation between respiration and limbic rhythms may have a crucial role in the formation of hippocampal-dependent memory for odours. Thus, the sniff cycle may act as a coherent ‘timing unit’ for olfactory and hippocampal systems (Buonviso et al., 2006).
Functional relation of MS/DB to respiratory circuitry and olfactory processing
The respiratory rhythm is generated within the ventrolateral side of the brainstem and is distributed between the pontine, ventral and dorsal respiratory groups (Onimaru & Homma, 2003; Smith et al., 2007). The pontine respiratory group (including the pedunculopontine tegmental and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei) sends ascending afferents to MS/DB complex (Cornwall et al., 1990; Woolf et al., 1990), the region of our interest. Concurrently, MS/DB receives indirect olfactory inputs via the piriform cortex and hypothalamus (Price & Powell, 1970; Carnes et al., 1990), allowing the septal region to integrate the motor and the sensory components of olfactory perception. Importantly, neuronal activity in anterior piriform cortex is phase-locked to the sniff cycle (Rennaker et al., 2007). Here, we show that 52.4% of recorded units in MS/DB (175 of 334) relate their spiking to respiration. Slow-spiking units demonstrate a strict phase-locking to the sniff cycle, whereas the fast-spiking and theta units entrain their burst with respect to sniff phase. The spiking of all three groups is correlated with the sniffing rate. The peaks of the correlation are either positive or negative, revealing that each neuron shows an individual preference for the sniff phase. Interestingly, slow-spiking units are characterised by two peaks of correlation where the second peak is about −530 ms from time 0 (Fig. 6C). Thus, sniff and theta couple their activity in windows of four sniff cycles on average. Over a similar time frame (−550 ms from time 0), the negative–positive correlation peak sequences of theta units are distributed (Fig. 6E), representing the frequency of theta-burst and subsequent inter-burst pauses. These data show that slow-spiking and theta units are functionally related. Both groups fire regularly (in spikes or bursts, respectively) with correlation peaks of < −30 ms to time 0. Both neuronal types fire with a higher correlation during fast sniffing (9–12 Hz), and this frequency precisely defines the range of coherence between the hippocampal LFP and sniffing (Fig. 8C). Fast-spiking units fire with irregular bursts and show a high degree of correlation to the sniff rate with a temporal delay of about −100 ms. This timing is sufficient for polysynaptic signal processing, following the anatomical and physiological connectivity between olfactory structures and MS/DB (Barkai & Hasselmo, 1997; Linster & Hasselmo, 2000). The correlations between septal inter-spike intervals and sniffing revealed significant but relatively low values of about ± 0.03. The weak correlation shows that MS/DB neuronal activity is not solely involved in olfactory processing but rather integrates processed respiratory signals. Additionally, the timing of neuronal activity in the OB relative to the onsets of airflow into the nasal cavity can vary by hundreds of milliseconds depending on the odorants in the inhaled air (Macrides & Chorover, 1972; Cury & Uchida, 2010; Carey & Wachowiak, 2011), which can also lead to noise in the sniff-spiking correlations. Early work has proposed the influence of respiration on theta frequency oscillations in the OB and piriform cortex (Eeckman & Freeman, 1990; Barrie et al., 1996). The olfactory processing involves sensory (olfactory) and motor (respiratory) signal integrity, and thus MS/DB might mediate processed olfactory sensorimotor inputs. Our data support this hypothesis, demonstrating that olfactory perception induced by odorant stimulation augments the degree of neuronal polarity to the sniff phase (Fig. 7B). We also aimed to find if the olfactory-induced sniff phase preference of septal neurons reflects the functional activity of MS/DB output regions.
Integration of sniff rate and theta frequency by septo-hippocampal circuitry
A major MS/DB output targets the hippocampal system (Amaral & Kurz, 1985; Wainer et al., 1985). Lesions of the septal inputs impair hippocampal theta rhythm by reducing its rate and decreasing its power (Rawlins et al., 1979; Buzsáki et al., 1983; Lee et al., 1994; Bassant et al., 1995). Septal inactivation decreases the firing rate of hippocampal neurons and reduces their ability to spike in trains (Leutgeb & Mizumori, 1999; Koenig et al., 2011). Current theories on the generation of theta oscillations in the hippocampal formation attribute a central role to interconnected inhibitory GABA neurons, either as oscillators or as resonators to theta activity (Buzsáki, 2002). In agreement with earlier studies (Macrides et al., 1982; Kay, 2005) we show here that hippocampal theta rhythm increases its correlation with respiratory oscillations during odour-induced epochs of faster sniffing in the range 9–12 Hz (Fig. 7C). The spiking of place cells depends on a number of variables, including the position of the animal, and the theta phase (O∼Keefe & Recce, 1993; Harris et al., 2002; Mehta et al., 2002). Hippocampal theta amplitude and frequency depend on locomotor behaviour (Vanderwolf, 1969; Geisler et al., 2007), which entrains the firing rate of hippocampal place cells as a function of an animal∼s speed (McNaughton et al., 1983; Wiener et al., 1989; Czurkó et al., 1999). Rodent locomotion strongly interferes with sniffing behaviour and anatomically this is related to the activity of pontine cholinergic structures (the pedunculopontine tegmental and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei), which converge inputs from basal ganglia and respiratory circuitry (Kubin & Fenik, 2004; Mena-Segovia et al., 2004). Thus, we explored if hippocampal neurons could reflect the sniffing phase during an animal∼s locomotion in a pallet-chasing task. Indeed, we found that place cells burst with a preference for the sniff phase in the range 180–240° (Fig. 8E). This phase-preference precisely matches the ability of CA1 place cells to fire with highest frequency on the trough of theta in the range 180–270° (O∼Keefe & Recce, 1993; Mehta et al., 2002). The coupling of place cell spiking to the sniff phase in our experiments was expressed during epochs of significant sniff–LFP correlations. Such a relationship has been proposed by observations of hippocampal neurons, recorded from partial complex seizure patients, which showed an overall rate correlation with the respiratory cycle, with cells discharging on a breath-by-breath relationship (Frysinger & Harper, 1989).
The phase link between sniffing and hippocampal theta rhythm has been shown to vary linearly with frequency (Macrides et al., 1982). If both oscillations are functionally related to MS/DB, then inactivation of the septal region should evoke concurrent changes in both frequencies. To test if MS/DB reflects the frequency of both respiratory and hippocampal local field oscillations, we inhibited septal activity by injection of a GABAergic agonist (muscimol). We found that both sniffing and theta rhythm underwent a low-frequency shift: both oscillators lost their upper theta range (9–12 Hz). Importantly, the coherence between them was preserved in the range of 6 Hz (Fig. 9). The preserved information processing between both oscillators is supported by the finding that inhalation-coupled transient activity in OB is invariant to respiration frequency (Cury & Uchida, 2010). The muscimol experiment demonstrated that MS/DB plays a crucial role in resetting both theta and respiration oscillations and particularly their fast frequency (9–12 Hz) bands, which are characteristic features of the odour-induced sniff–theta correlation. The fact that the sniffing frequency was concurrently decreased proposes bidirectional connectivity between septal and ponto-medullar neuronal groups (both of which are cholinergic). Respiration drives directly or indirectly (through OB and piriform corthex) septal neurons, while medial septum regulates the frequency of sniffing. The partial inhibition of MS/DB led to a synchronised decrease of theta and sniffing frequencies. There is also the possibility that MS/DB is just one of the structures that mediate the sniff–theta coupling, particularly in the slow frequencies. Other candidates involved in this network might be nucleus basalis magnocellularis (also a cholinergic structure) as well as the hippocampal formation itself. Nucleus basalis of the basal forebrain forms extensive cholinergic projections to the cortex (Woolf et al., 1983; Casamenti et al., 1986) and it plays a role in arousal, sensorimotor integration and locomotion (Wenk, 1997; Wrenn & Wiley, 1998). Inactivation of nucleus basalis affects the cortical oscillatory synchrony (Buzsáki et al., 1988; Detari et al., 1999; Sarter & Bruno, 2000). Nucleus basalis also receives inputs from pediculopontine tegmental nucleus (Semba et al., 1988), which could be a potential bypass for sniff–theta coupling, for the low (0–5 Hz) frequencies. Overall, the cholinergic circuitry of ponto-medullar nuclei and basal forebrain controls both limbic oscillations and exploratory behaviour, thus enabling a functional synchronisation of different brain oscillators.
In summary, our data suggest that the septo-hippocampal axis integrates respiratory rate and limbic theta rhythm. This temporal coupling incorporates the integration of intrinsic theta and extrinsic sensorimotor signals on each theta cycle. The functional significance of our data addresses the assembly organisation of hippocampal neurons, which is crucially dependent on the relationship between global theta frequency, the oscillation frequency of single neurons and the sensorimotor theta timescale (Buzsáki, 2010).
Acknowledgments
We thank Mani Ramaswami and James Knierim for helpful comments on early versions of the manuscript. This work was supported by Science Foundation Ireland, the Health Research Board and the Wellcome Trust under the Biomedical Research Partnership, grant number 099926/Z/12/Z to M.T. The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Glossary
Abbreviations
- LFP
local field potential
- MS/DB
medial septum/diagonal band of Broca
- OB
olfactory bulb
Supporting Information
Fig. S1. Slow-spiking activity for the main sniffing frequencies.
Fig. S2. Fast-spiking activity for the main sniffing frequencies.
Fig. S3. Theta activity for the main sniffing frequencies.
Fig. S4. Place cells∼ relation to sniffing parameters.
References
- Amaral DG, Kurz J. An analysis of the origins of the cholinergic and noncholinergic septal projections to the hippocampal formation of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1985;240:37–59. doi: 10.1002/cne.902400104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkai E, Hasselmo MH. Acetylcholine and associative memory in the piriform cortex. Mol. Neurobiol. 1997;15:17–29. doi: 10.1007/BF02740613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrie JM, Freeman WJ, Lenhart MD. Spatiotemporal analysis of prepyriform, visual, auditory, and somesthetic surface EEGs in trained rabbits. J. Neurophysiol. 1996;76:520–539. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassant MH, Apartis E, Jazat-Poindessous FR, Wiley RG, Lamour YA. Selective immunolesion of the basal forebrain cholinergic neurons: effects on hippocampal activity during sleep and wakefulness in the rat. Neurodegeneration. 1995;4:61–70. doi: 10.1006/neur.1995.0007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bathellier B, Buhl DL, Accolla R, Carleton A. Dynamic ensemble odor coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb: sensory information at different timescales. Neuron. 2008;57:586–598. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berg RW, Whitmer D, Kleinfeld D. Exploratory whisking by rat is not phase locked to the hippocampal theta rhythm. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:6518–6522. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0190-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bland SK, Bland BH. Medial septal modulation of hippocampal theta cell discharges. Brain Res. 1986;375:102–116. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(86)90963-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandon MP, Bogaard AR, Libby CP, Connerney MA, Gupta K, Hasselmo ME. Reduction of theta rhythm dissociates grid cell spatial periodicity from directional tuning. Science. 2011;332:595–599. doi: 10.1126/science.1201652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandon MP, Bogaard AR, Andrews CM, Hasselmo ME. Head direction cells in the postsubiculum do not show replay of prior waking sequences during sleep. Hippocampus. 2012;22:604–618. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brun VH, Otn___ MK, Molden S, Steffenach HA, Witter MP, Moser MB, Moser EI. Place cells and place recognition maintained by direct entorhinal-hippocampal circuitry. Science. 2002;296:2243–2246. doi: 10.1126/science.1071089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buonviso N, Amat C, Litaudon P, Roux S, Royet JP, Farget V, Sicard G. Rhythm sequence through the olfactory bulb layers during the time window of a respiratory cycle. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2003;17:1811–1819. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02619.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buonviso N, Amat C, Litaudon P. Respiratory modulation of olfactory neurons in the rodent brain. Chem. Senses. 2006;31:145–154. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2002;33:325–340. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00586-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G. Neural syntax: cell assemblies, synapsembles, and readers. Neuron. 2010;68:362–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Leung LW, Vanderwolf CH. Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Res. 1983;287:139–171. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(83)90037-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Bickford RG, Ponomareff G, Thal LJ, Mandel R, Gage FH. Nucleus basalis and thalamic control of neocortical activity in the freely moving rat. J. Neurosci. 1988;8:4007–4026. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-11-04007.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cang J, Isaacson JS. In vivo whole-cell recording of odor-evoked synaptic transmission in the rat olfactory bulb. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:4108–4116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04108.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey RM, Wachowiak M. Effect of sniffing on the temporal structure of mitral/tufted cell output from the olfactory bulb. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:10615–10626. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1805-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carnes KM, Fuller TA, Price JL. Sources of presumptive glutamatergic/aspartatergic afferents to the magnocellular basal forebrain in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1990;302:824–852. doi: 10.1002/cne.903020413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casamenti F, Deffenu G, Abbamondi AL, Pepeu G. Changes in cortical acetylcholine output induced by modulation of the nucleus basalis. Brain Res. Bull. 1986;16:689–695. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(86)90140-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charpak S, Mertz J, Beaurepaire E, Moreaux L, Delaney K. Odor-evoked calcium signals in dendrites of rat mitral cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:1230–1234. doi: 10.1073/pnas.021422798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornwall J, Cooper JD, Phillipson OT. Afferent and efferent connections of the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus in the rat. Brain Res. Bull. 1990;25:271–284. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(90)90072-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Czurkó A, Mamiya A, Buzsáki G. Oscillatory coupling of hippocampal pyramidal cells and interneurons in the behaving Rat. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:274–287. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00274.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cury KM, Uchida N. Robust odor coding via inhalation-coupled transient activity in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2010;68:570–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Czurkó A, Hirase H, Csicsvari J, Buzsáki G. Sustained activation of hippocampal pyramidal cells by ‘space clamping’ in a running wheel. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1999;11:344–352. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00446.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Détári L, Rasmusson DD, Semba K. The role of basal forebrain neurons in tonic and phasic activation of the cerebral cortex. Prog. Neurobiol. 1999;58:249–277. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00084-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eeckman FH, Freeman WJ. Correlations between unit firing and EEG in the rat olfactory system. Brain Res. 1990;528:238–244. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91663-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;30:123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fantana AL, Soucy ER, Meister M. Rat olfactory bulb mitral cells receive sparse glomerular inputs. Neuron. 2008;59:802–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fibiger HC. The organization and some projections of cholinergic neurons of the mammalian forebrain. Brain Res. 1982;257:327–388. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(82)90011-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher NI. Statistical Analysis of Circular Data. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frysinger RC, Harper RM. Cardiac and respiratory correlations with unit discharge in human amygdala and hippocampus. Electroen. Clin. Neuro. 1989;72:463–470. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90222-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geisler C, Robbe D, Zugaro M, Sirota A, Buzsáki G. Hippocampal place cell assemblies are speed-controlled oscillators. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:8149–8154. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610121104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Givens B, Olton DS. Local modulation of basal forebrain: effects on working and reference memory. J. Neurosci. 1994;14:3578–3587. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-06-03578.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halliday DM, Rosenberg JR, Amjad AM, Breeze P, Conway BA, Farmer SF. A framework for the analysis of mixed time series/point process data–theory and application to the study of physiological tremor, single motor unit discharges and electromyograms. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Bio. 1995;64:237–278. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6107(96)00009-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harper RM, Poe GR, Rector DM, Kristensen MP. Relationships between hippocampal activity and breathing patterns. Neurosci. Biobehav. R. 1998;22:233–236. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(97)00010-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Henze DA, Hirase H, Leinekugel X, Dragoi G, Czurkó A, Buzsáki G. Spike train dynamics predicts theta related phase precession in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Nature. 2002;417:738–741. doi: 10.1038/nature00808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollup SA, Molden S, Donnett JG, Moser MB, Moser EI. Place fields of rat hippocampal pyramidal cells and spatial learning in the watermaze. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2001;13:1197. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01487.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones BE. Immunohistochemical study of choline acetyltransferase-immunoreactive processes and cells innervating the pontomedullary reticular formation in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1990;295:485–514. doi: 10.1002/cne.902950311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones BE, Beaudet A. Retrograde labeling of neurones in the brain stem following injections of [3H]choline into the forebrain of the rat. Exp. Brain Res. 1987;65:437–448. doi: 10.1007/BF00236317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones MW, Wilson MA. Phase precession of medial prefrontal cortical activity relative to the hippocampal theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 2005;15:867–873. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay LM. Theta oscillations and sensorimotor performance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:3863–3868. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407920102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kepecs A, Uchida N, Mainen ZF. The sniff as a unit of olfactory processing. Chem. Senses. 2006;31:167–179. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kepecs A, Uchida N, Mainen ZF. Rapid and precise control of sniffing during olfactory discrimination in rats. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;98:205–213. doi: 10.1152/jn.00071.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King C, Recce M, O∼Keefe J. The rhythmicity of cells of the medial septum/diagonal band of Broca in the awake freely moving rat: relationships with behaviour and hippocampal theta. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1998;10:464–477. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00026.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenig J, Linder AN, Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S. The spatial periodicity of grid cells is not sustained during reduced theta oscillations. Science. 2011;332:592–595. doi: 10.1126/science.1201685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kubin L, Fenik V. Pontine cholinergic mechanisms and their impact on respiratory regulation. Resp. Physiol. Neurobi. 2004;143:235–249. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2004.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee MG, Chrobak JJ, Sik A, Wiley RG, Buzsáki G. Hippocampal theta activity following selective lesion of the septal cholinergic system. Neuroscience. 1994;62:1033–1047. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90341-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leutgeb S, Mizumori SJ. Excitotoxic septal lesions result in spatial memory deficits and altered flexibility of hippocampal single-unit representations. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:6661–6672. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-15-06661.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linster C, Hasselmo ME. Neural activity in the horizontal limb of the diagonal band of broca can be modulated by electrical stimulation of the olfactory bulb and cortex in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 2000;282:157–160. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)00885-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louie K, Wilson MA. Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron. 2001;29:145–156. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00186-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyttle D, Fellous JM. A new similarity measure for spike trains: sensitivity to bursts and periods of inhibition. J. Neurosci. Meth. 2011;199:296–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macrides F, Chorover SL. Olfactory bulb units: activity correlated with inhalation cycles and odor quality. Science. 1972;175:84–87. doi: 10.1126/science.175.4017.84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macrides F, Eichenbaum HB, Forbes WB. Temporal relationship between sniffing and the limbic theta rhythm during odor discrimination reversal learning. J. Neurosci. 1982;2:1705–1717. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-12-01705.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin C, Beshel J, Kay LM. An olfacto-hippocampal network is dynamically involved in odor-discrimination learning. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;98:2196–2205. doi: 10.1152/jn.00524.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, O∼Keefe J. The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Exp. Brain Res. 1983;52:41–49. doi: 10.1007/BF00237147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta MR, Lee AK, Wilson MA. Role of experience and oscillations in transforming a rate code into a temporal code. Nature. 2002;417:741–746. doi: 10.1038/nature00807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mena-Segovia J, Bolam JP, Magill PJ. Pedunculopontine nucleus and basal ganglia: distant relatives or part of the same family? Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:585–588. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mizumori SJ, Perez GM, Alvarado MC, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Reversible inactivation of the medial septum differentially affects two forms of learning in rats. Brain Res. 1990;528:12–20. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)90188-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O∼Keefe J, Recce ML. Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus. 1993;3:317–330. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onimaru H, Homma I. A novel functional neuron group for respiratory rhythm generation in the ventral medulla. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:1478–1486. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-04-01478.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price JL, Powell TP. The afferent connexions of the nucleus of the horizontal limb of the diagonal band. J. Anat. 1970;107:239–256. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramcharan EJ, Gnadt JW, Sherman SM. Higher-order thalamic relays burst more than first-order relays. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:12236–12241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502843102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranck JB., Jr Studies on single neurons in dorsal hippocampal formation and septum in unrestrained rats. I. Behavioral correlates and firing repertoires. Exp. Neurol. 1973;41:461–531. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(73)90290-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravel N, Caille D, Pager J. A centrifugal respiratory modulation of olfactory bulb unit activity: a study on acute rat preparation. Exp. Brain Res. 1987;65:623–628. doi: 10.1007/BF00235985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rawlins JN, Feldon J, Gray JA. Septo-hippocampal connections and the hippocampal theta rhythm. Exp. Brain Res. 1979;37:49–63. doi: 10.1007/BF01474253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rennaker RL, Chen CF, Ruyle AM, Sloan AM, Wilson DA. Spatial and temporal distribution of odorant-evoked activity in the piriform cortex. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:1534–1542. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4072-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robitsek RJ, Fortin NJ, Koh MT, Gallagher M, Eichenbaum H. Cognitive aging: a common decline of episodic recollection and spatial memory in rats. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:8945–8954. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1893-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarter M, Bruno JP. Cortical cholinergic inputs mediating arousal, attentional processing and dreaming: differential afferent regulation of the basal forebrain by telencephalic and brainstem afferents. Neuroscience. 2000;95:933–952. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00487-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satoh K, Fibiger HC. Cholinergic neurons of the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus: efferent and afferent connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 1986;253:277–302. doi: 10.1002/cne.902530302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott JW. Sniffing and spatiotemporal coding in olfaction. Chem. Senses. 2006;31:119–130. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semba K, Fibiger HC. Afferent connections of the laterodorsal and the pedunculopontine tegmental nuclei in the rat: a retro- and antero-grade transport and immunohistochemical study. J. Comp. Neurol. 1992;323:387–410. doi: 10.1002/cne.903230307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semba K, Reiner PB, McGeer EG, Fibiger HC. Brainstem afferents to the magnocellular basal forebrain studied by axonal transport, immunohistochemistry, and electrophysiology in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1988;267:433–453. doi: 10.1002/cne.902670311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shirvalkar PR, Rapp PR, Shapiro ML. Bidirectional changes to hippocampal theta-gamma comodulation predict memory for recent spatial episodes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:7054–7059. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911184107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siapas AG, Lubenov EV, Wilson MA. Prefrontal phase locking to hippocampal theta oscillations. Neuron. 2005;46:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Wilson MA, Barnes CA. Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus. 1996;6:149–172. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:2<149::AID-HIPO6>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith JC, Ellenberger HH, Ballanyi K, Richter DW, Feldman JL. Pre-Bötzinger complex: a brainstem region that may generate respiratory rhythm in mammals. Science. 1991;254:726–729. doi: 10.1126/science.1683005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith JC, Abdala AP, Koizumi H, Rybak IA, Paton JF. Spatial and functional architecture of the mammalian brain stem respiratory network: a hierarchy of three oscillatory mechanisms. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3370–3387. doi: 10.1152/jn.00985.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spors H, Grinvald A. Spatio-temporal dynamics of odor representations in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2002;34:301–315. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00644-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart M, Fox SE. Firing relations of medial septal neurons to the hippocampal theta rhythm in urethane anesthetized rats. Exp. Brain Res. 1989a;77:507–516. doi: 10.1007/BF00249604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart M, Fox SE. Two populations of rhythmically bursting neurons in rat medial septum are revealed by atropine. J. Neurophysiol. 1989b;61:982–993. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.5.982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsanov M, Chah E, Vann SD, Reilly RB, Erichsen JT, Aggleton JP, O∼Mara SM. Theta-modulated head direction cells in the rat anterior thalamus. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:9489–9502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0353-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderwolf CH. Hippocampal electrical activity and voluntary movement in the rat. Electroen. Clin. Neuro. 1969;26:407–418. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(69)90092-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderwolf CH. Hippocampal activity, olfaction, and sniffing: an olfactory input to the dentate gyrus. Brain Res. 1992;593:197–208. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91308-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verhagen JV, Wesson DW, Netoff TI, White JA, Wachowiak M. Sniffing controls an adaptive filter of sensory input to the olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:631–639. doi: 10.1038/nn1892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP, Kocsis B. Brainstem-diencephalo-septohippocampal systems controlling the theta rhythm of the hippocampus. Neuroscience. 1997;81:893–926. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00239-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wachowiak M. All in a sniff: olfaction as a model for active sensing. Neuron. 2011;71:962–973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wainer BH, Levey AI, Rye DB, Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. Cholinergic and non-cholinergic septohippocampal pathways. Neurosci. Lett. 1985;54:45–52. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(85)80116-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang BW, Hok V, Della-Chiesa A, Callaghan C, Barlow S, Tsanov M, Bechara R, Irving E, Virley DJ, Upton N, O∼Mara SM. Rosiglitazone enhances learning, place cell activity, and synaptic plasticity in middle-aged rats. Neurobiol. Aging. 2012;33:835.e13–835.e30. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wenk GL. The nucleus basalis magnocellularis cholinergic system: one hundred years of progress. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1997;67:85–95. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.3757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wesson DW, Verhagen JV, Wachowiak M. Why sniff fast? The relationship between sniff frequency, odor discrimination, and receptor neuron activation in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;101:1089–1102. doi: 10.1152/jn.90981.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiebe SP, Staubli UV. Recognition memory correlates of hippocampal theta cells. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:3955–3967. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-11-03955.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiener SI, Paul CA, Eichenbaum H. Spatial and behavioral correlates of hippocampal neuronal activity. J. Neurosci. 1989;9:2737–2763. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.09-08-02737.1989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science. 1993;261:1055–1058. doi: 10.1126/science.8351520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winson J. Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat. Science. 1978;201:160–163. doi: 10.1126/science.663646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolf NJ, Butcher LL. Cholinergic systems in the rat brain: IV. Descending projections of the pontomesencephalic tegmentum. Brain Res. Bull. 1989;23:519–540. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(89)90197-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolf NJ, Eckenstein F, Butcher LL. Cholinergic projections from the basal forebrain to the frontal cortex: a combined fluorescent tracer and immunohistochemical analysis in the rat. Neurosci. Lett. 1983;40:93–98. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(83)90285-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolf NJ, Harrison JB, Buchwald JS. Cholinergic neurons of the feline pontomesencephalon. II. Ascending anatomical projections. Brain Res. 1990;520:55–72. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91691-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wrenn CC, Wiley RG. The behavioral functions of the cholinergic basal forebrain: lessons from 192 IgG-saporin. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 1998;16:595–602. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(98)00071-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Fig. S1. Slow-spiking activity for the main sniffing frequencies.
Fig. S2. Fast-spiking activity for the main sniffing frequencies.
Fig. S3. Theta activity for the main sniffing frequencies.
Fig. S4. Place cells∼ relation to sniffing parameters.