Abstract
Craniofacial sutures and synchondroses form the boundaries among bones in the human skull, providing functional, developmental and evolutionary information. Bone articulations in the skull arise due to interactions between genetic regulatory mechanisms and epigenetic factors such as functional matrices (soft tissues and cranial cavities), which mediate bone growth. These matrices are largely acknowledged for their influence on shaping the bones of the skull; however, it is not fully understood to what extent functional matrices mediate the formation of bone articulations. Aiming to identify whether or not functional matrices are key developmental factors guiding the formation of bone articulations, we have built a network null model of the skull that simulates unconstrained bone growth. This null model predicts bone articulations that arise due to a process of bone growth that is uniform in rate, direction and timing. By comparing predicted articulations with the actual bone articulations of the human skull, we have identified which boundaries specifically need the presence of functional matrices for their formation. We show that functional matrices are necessary to connect facial bones, whereas an unconstrained bone growth is sufficient to connect non-facial bones. This finding challenges the role of the brain in the formation of boundaries between bones in the braincase without neglecting its effect on skull shape. Ultimately, our null model suggests where to look for modified developmental mechanisms promoting changes in bone growth patterns that could affect the development and evolution of the head skeleton.
Keywords: anatomical networks, epigenetics, Gabriel rule, head development, morphology
Introduction
Craniofacial sutures and synchondroses articulate the bones of the human skull, forming boundaries that embody evolutionary, developmental and functional relations. Their presence or absence is used to identify homologous characters due to their deep evolutionary conservation (Hall, 1994; Depew & Simpson, 2006; Depew et al. 2008), their formation and obliteration timing are used to determine age in hominids (Cray et al. 2012; Falk et al. 2012), and their fibrous composition is important to distribute mechanical loads and stresses during the whole life of an individual (Herring, 2008). During ontogeny, the most important function of bone articulations in the skull is to act as primary bone growth sites (Opperman, 2000). For these reasons, suture formation is of particular medical relevance in humans, where disruption of normal suture patterns, for example due to premature fusion, often leads to malformations of the head and to brain injuries (Cohen & MacLean, 2000; Rice, 2008). In summary, insights on the development of suture patterns have broader applications in such disparate fields as taxonomy, functional morphology or pathological anatomy (Di Ieva et al. 2013).
Bone articulations in the human skull arise due to interactions between genetic and epigenetic factors that take place during skull late development (Moss, 2007; Sperber et al. 2010; Lieberman, 2011a). Epigenetic factors during suture formation are those originating not from the bone tissue itself, but from other tissues and cavities having a significant influence on the time, rate and direction of bone growth. The primacy of these epigenetic factors in skull development over autonomous genetic determination is the main claim of the functional matrix hypothesis (Moss & Young, 1960; Moss, 1968). Functional matrices include a variety of epigenetic factors, such as the presence of neighboring organs, surrounded cavities and the attachment of muscles, mediating in the morphogenesis of bones as well as their articulations to other bones in the skull (Lieberman, 2011a). According to this hypothesis, the position and shape of bones, as well as the formation of sutures, is fully determined by the functional needs of soft tissues and cavities that bones protect and support (Fig. 1). This hypothesis has been broadly used to explain many craniofacial disorders (Mulliken et al. 1989; Breitsprecher et al. 2002; Kikuchi, 2005) and some morphological features of the head (Festa et al. 2010; Richards & Jabbour, 2011), in particular the integration between brain growth and skull shape (Moss, 1975; Fields et al. 1978; Richtsmeier et al. 2006; Lieberman, 2011a; Richtsmeier & Flaherty, 2013). Other epigenetic factors that affect the formation of sutures include hormonal signals (Karsenty, 1999) and biomechanical mechanisms (Shwartz et al. 2012; Khonsari et al. 2013). In addition, external forces and movements related to functional matrices influence the internal complexity of sutures (Curtis et al. 2014).
Despite the usefulness of the functional matrix hypothesis to explain the development and evolution of the human skull, several issues remain controversial (reviewed in Lieberman, 2011b; chapter 2). For instance, bones seldom participate only in one functional matrix and are influenced by many overlapping factors (Lieberman, 2011a). Consequently, studies on morphological integration and modularity that test for developmental modules in the skull often indicate that co-variation between skull parts blurs the boundaries among functional matrices (Cheverud, 1982; Sardi et al. 2004; Bastir et al. 2008; Singh et al. 2012). In addition, some morphological features of the human skull are invariant to the modification of the functional matrices they supposedly belong to (see Lieberman, 2011b; p. 53). Bone growth and suture formation, however, can take place even in the absence of brain growth or muscular activity (Nonaka & Nakata, 1984; Hirabayashi et al. 1988; Sugawara et al. 1999), probably due to cell-autonomous mechanisms (e.g. the Wnt regulatory network) that promote bone formation (reviewed in Zhang et al. 2009). As a result, the formation of boundaries among skull bones is a dynamical compromise between intrinsic genetic regulation of bone growth and the above-mentioned epigenetic factors.
The overall pattern of bone articulations has been formalized in studies about the morphological organization of the skull using anatomical network models (Rasskin-Gutman, 2003; Esteve-Altava et al. 2011; Rasskin-Gutman & Esteve-Altava, 2014). These models represent the bones and articulations of the skull as the nodes and connections of a network: 1s for presence and 0s for absence (Fig. 2). The entire skull is modeled using both the external and internal sutures among bones; this gives a precise description of how left and right counterpart bones connect to each other and to unpaired bones. In an evolutionary context, we used network models to quantify morphological complexity in tetrapod skulls, demonstrating that the reduction in the number of bones and articulations during evolution is a trend toward increase of morphological complexity (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013b, 2014; Esteve-Altava & Rasskin-Gutman, 2014). Using this approach we showed that the human skull is divided into two connectivity modules: one facial and one cranial (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). The facial module has a hierarchical organization composed of smaller blocks (i.e. groups of bones tightly interconnected) held together by the ethmoid, which acts as the bearing wall of the face. In contrast, the cranial module has a regular organization of connections, like the panels of a soccer ball. An independent analysis using geometric morphometrics demonstrated that these morphological modules also behave as units of allometric growth, thus suggesting that each module arises by different growth relations among bones (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). Therefore, because craniofacial sutures and synchondroses are sites of bone growth (Opperman, 2000), anatomical network models are also implicit models of growth relations (or co-dependences) among skull bones. The implication is that, even though these models are purely structurally defined, any statement about modularity and integration in anatomical network models in the skull is necessarily rooted in the dynamics of bone growth (Esteve-Altava & Rasskin-Gutman, 2014).
Here, we extend the use of anatomical network models by building an explicit null model to simulate the outcome of uniform, unconstrained growth for the formation of bone articulations in the human skull. By comparing bone connections in the null model vs. actual bone articulations in the human skull, we aim to test whether a particular bone articulation is either: (i) sufficiently explained by unconstrained bone growth; or (ii) necessarily explained by a specific modification of bone growth, presumably due to the induction of functional matrices. The proposed null model is based on geometric proximity assumptions, modeling the human skull as if it were a Gabriel network (Gabriel & Sokal, 1969). The Gabriel rule of linkage is used to formalize an idealized bone growth mechanism, in which bones grow in all directions with uniform speed – as if each node of the network was an ossification center that grows without constraints until it meets another growing edge, forming a bone articulation (Esteve-Altava & Rasskin-Gutman, 2014). Thus, the null model only predicts bone articulations that meet this primary assumption: uniform (in rate, direction and time) and unconstrained bone growth. For this reason, the null model will not predict bone articulations that arise due to changes in bone growth leading, for example, to irregular shapes, allometric growth or changes in the timing of ossification; this ensures that the null model is appropriate as a contrast model for the actual skull network. In this manner, this network null model (NNM) produces testable hypotheses about the modification of growth mechanisms acting specifically at each craniofacial suture and synchondrosis. Elucidating the nature and modification of these developmental mechanisms will need complementary empirical studies.
Materials and methods
A NNM of bone articulations
Our NNM of the human skull is a theoretical network in which we use the same number of bones as in the actual human skull, but nodes are connected following the Gabriel rule of linkage (Gabriel & Sokal, 1969) rather than using the actual connectivity pattern (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). The algorithm connects each pair of bones if, and only if, the sphere whose diameter is the line between both bones does not have any other bone within its volume. The input used to initialize the algorithm requires a set of spatial positions (i.e. 3D coordinates) for each bone. However, using the coordinates of the centroid for each bone would provide a tautological answer to the question we are asking. Instead, we have used the overall connectivity of the skull to calculate a virtual position for each bone, which renders coordinates for each bone position as related to the growth dependences it establishes with all others. To do this, we have performed a non-metric multidimensional scaling of the topological overlap (TO) similarity matrix of the human skull network (see Yip & Horvath, 2007 as an example of this type of approach in genetic networks). The Kruskal normalized stress index (Kruskal & Wish, 1978) was used to find the optimal number of dimensions (optimal dim = 3, stress = 0.06) of the virtual space that includes the bones of the human skull. These dimensions resemble the three body axes: anteroposterior, left–right and dorsoventral, forming a virtual space in which the coordinates of each bone are positioned.
What this means in biological terms is that we translate the growth relations among bones of the human skull network (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a) into a set of relative positions of bones; as a consequence, distances among bones in the null model depend on sharing growth relations with the same neighboring bones. Because the main assumption of the model is that bone growth is uniform, these distances determine the connections among bones in the NNM. Therefore, we have used the TO (i.e. a measure of the number of common neighbors for each bone) as a proxy for the growth co-dependences among bones due to the spatial constraints imposed by each bone–bone articulation (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). The non-metric multidimensional scaling carries out isotonic regressions of the TO similarity matrix; this scaling technique is a non-parametric monotonic transformation that converts pairwise node (bone) similarity into Euclidean relative distances between two nodes within a virtual space (Borg & Groenen, 2005). Thereby, topological similarity among bones (size N × N) is scaled into virtual spatial coordinates of bones (size N × 3). The virtual position thus computed for each bone is used as the input of the Gabriel algorithm to build the NNM of the human skull. To further illustrate this method, we show a 2D toy example for only 10 bones, along with a simplified illustration of how an unconstrained model of growth determines bone connections in a NNM (Fig. 3).
Analysis of the NNM
The organization of the NNM has been analyzed in order to compare it with that reported for the human skull (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). This was done to verify that the properties of the NNM were indeed like those of the actual human network, further ensuring its usefulness as a baseline comparative model. Thus, we have compared to what extent both networks were similar in: (i) structural complexity; (ii) hierarchical organization; and (iii) modularity. To do that it is necessary to compute a series of parameters, which we explain in detail below.
First, to analyze the complexity of the NNM we have quantified the following three parameters: density of connections (D), which quantifies the number of existing bone connections with respect to the maximum possible; mean clustering coefficient (C), which quantifies the arithmetic mean of the number of existing connections among the neighbors of each bone with respect to the maximum possible; and mean shortest path length (L), which quantifies the arithmetic mean of the number of connections required to interconnect any two bones in the network (for algorithm descriptions see Data S1).
Second, to evaluate the hierarchy (i.e. nested aggrupation of bones) of the NNM we have quantified the following parameters: connectivity distribution, P(k); and clustering coefficient distribution, C(k). The P(k) is the frequency of nodes with k connections, and the C(k) is the mean clustering coefficient of nodes with k connections; for practical reasons, we will use the cumulative P(k) in order to avoid statistical fluctuations due to a small number of nodes (Dorogovtsev & Mendes, 2003). Theoretically, the P(k) and C(k) in hierarchical networks should fit a power-law distribution, an instance of the Pareto distribution (Newman, 2005); in random networks, the P(k) fits a Poisson distribution and the C(k) is independent of the number of connections; and, in scale-free networks, the P(k) fits a power law and the C(k) is also independent of the node connectivity (Ravasz & Barabási, 2003; Wuchty et al. 2006).
Third, to identify meaningful morphological modules in the NNM, we have verified the presence of a small-world organization (Esteve-Altava et al. 2011, 2013a). This is so because the organization of connections in a small-world network lies somewhere between regularity and randomness (Watts & Strogatz, 1998); the heterogeneous patterns of connections in small-world networks promote the formation of modules (i.e. some nodes have more connections among themselves than to other groups of nodes). We have previously determined that the human skull network is small world and that it has two meaningful modules (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a); in contrast, regular networks are not modular, whereas random networks can have modules by chance. To verify the presence of a small-world organization, we compared the clustering coefficient, C, and the shortest path length, L (described above), of the NNM with those of 10 000 random equivalent networks, which have the same number of nodes and connections but are rearranged at random. A network is small world if it fulfills the SW condition (Humphries & Gurney, 2008): (C/Crand)/(L/Lrand) ≥ 0.012n1.11, where Crand and Lrand are the computed parameters for the random networks. Therefore, the NNM (with n = 21) will be small world if SW is greater or equal to 0.35.
To further characterize the modular structure of the NNM, we have carried out a hierarchical cluster analysis of the TO similarity matrix (Ravasz et al. 2002; Li & Horvath, 2007; Yip & Horvath, 2007). The TO between two bones is a measure of the number of shared connections to the same other bones (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a): if TO = 1, two bones share all connections to the same other bones; if TO = 0, two bones lack common connected neighbors. Heuristically, we know that elements sharing connections to the same neighbors will tend to belong to the same module (Solé et al. 2006). This justifies the use of the TO similarity matrix as the input of a hierarchical cluster analysis; when performing this analysis, pairs of bones with higher TO will be grouped together in single branches until all bones form one single group and the outcome can be conveniently shown as a dendrogram. Each potential partition in the dendrogram is evaluated by quantifying an index of modularity (Q), which measures the strength of the modular organization in comparison to other possible random partitions (Newman & Girvan, 2004). The partition with the highest Q will identify the composition of each connectivity module in the NNM. The result of this analysis yields connectivity modules that are groups of bones with more connections among themselves than to other bones outside the group.
Further descriptions of network parameters and methods used in this study are available as Supplementary Information (see also Esteve-Altava et al. 2011, 2013a,b2013b; Esteve-Altava & Rasskin-Gutman, 2014). All algorithms have been scripted in Matlab.
Results
The NNM and the human skull network share the same basic organizational properties regarding their structural complexity, hierarchy and modularity. This similarity supports the use of the NNM as a baseline comparative model for the human skull; as expected by the assumptions, the null model and the human skull differ in the values for each network parameter.
Comparing their structural complexity (Fig. 4), the NNM has lower density of bone articulations (D = 0.25), lower clustering coefficient of bones (C = 0.51) and higher shortest path length among bones (L = 1.95) than the human skull (D = 0.3, C = 0.63, L = 1.74). Still, the values of C and L of the NNM are different from those expected in random equivalent networks (SW = 1.72), which indicates the presence of a small-world organization (SW ≥ 0.35). Regarding their hierarchical organization (Fig. 5), the NNM has an exponential P(k) (r = 0.95) and a power-law C(k) (r = 0.82), while both distributions fit a power law in the human skull (r = 0.98 and r = 0.89, respectively). These results indicate that both share similar hierarchical organizations.
Comparing their modularity (Fig. 6), the NNM shows a division in four modules (Qmax = 0.28): (i) left facial, composed of left palatine, lacrimal, nasal concha and maxillary bones; (ii) right facial, composed of right palatine, lacrimal, nasal concha and maxillary bone; (iii) central facial, composed of frontal, ethmoid and nasal bones; and (iv) cranial, composed of vomer, sphenoid, occipital, parietal, temporal and zygomatic bones. In contrast, the human skull is divided into two connectivity modules (Qmax = 0.27): (i) facial, composed of ethmoid, frontal, nasal conchas, lacrimals, maxillas, nasals, palatines and vomer; and (ii) cranial, composed of occipital, parietals, sphenoid, temporals and zygomatics. However, the four connectivity modules in the NNM still indicate a separation between facial and cranial regions; the formation of four modules instead of two is due to differences between predicted connections in the NNM and realized articulations in the human skull.
A one-to-one comparison between predicted and realized articulations shows that most of them (72%) can be predicted correctly (Fig. 7, solid gray lines); therefore, these articulations are sufficiently explained using unconstrained bone growth as in the null model. The role of the functional matrices in the formation of these bone–bone articulations would be, at most, concomitant to a uniform growth of bones. In general, the NNM predicts well those articulations among bones in the cranial connectivity module: among sphenoid, occipital, temporal, parietal and zygomatic bones; but not the zygomaticotemporal suture. In contrast, facial articulations are less predictable by the NNM: (i) some involve the zygomatic bones; (ii) some connect maxillary and palatine bones in the midline; and (iii) some connect unpaired and paired bones within the face (Fig. 7, red dotted lines). Finally, the NNM predicts some connections that are absent in the actual human skull (Fig. 7, green dashed lines) whose formation would necessarily imply modifying bone growth patterns.
Discussion
The overall pattern of bone articulation of the null model is organized as in the human skull. Similarly to the actual skull (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a), the NNM has a complex pattern of connections among bones, further characterized by a particularly interesting organization between randomness and regularity. Moreover, many bones have few articulations, while a few have many articulations; these highly connected bones participate in many more clusters (i.e. motifs of three interconnected bones) than the poorly connected ones. In addition, the NNM also has a modular organization of bones that discriminates between bones of the anterior facial region (facial connectivity modules) and the posterior cranial region (cranial connectivity module). Sharing such similarity of morphological organization supports the use of the NNM as a comparative baseline to explore the formation of craniofacial articulations in the human skull. In the following sections we compare both networks to analyze how functional matrices (i.e. cavities and soft tissues that mediate bone growth) differentially affect each bone–bone articulation.
Influence of functional matrices on the formation of facial sutures
The presence of functional matrices is especially important in the human face; these functional matrices include the oral cavity, the nasal cavity, the maxillary sinuses, the eyeballs and the masticatory muscles (Moss & Salentijn, 1969a,b1969b). At the same time, most differences in bone articulation between the null model and the human skull occur among facial bones, or bones of the cranial module that contact with facial ones such as the zygomatics. Indeed, the human skull deviates from the null model largely due to differences in the boundaries of the zygomatic bones.
On the one hand, zygomatic bones articulate to parietal and lacrimal bones in the null model, but these articulations do not occur during the development of the human skull. These articulations are wrongly predicted by the NNM precisely because the model does not take into account any constraints or local signals for non-uniform bone growth. Thus, our model suggests that, indeed, the growth of these bones must be constrained by external epigenetic factors. According to the functional matrix hypothesis (Moss, 2007), these factors could be the eyeball as well as the muscles that pass through the zygomatic arch (e.g. temporalis) and those attached to it (e.g. masseter). Together, the eyeball and the muscles of the zygomatic arch would modify greatly the morphology of the zygomatic bone preventing the articulation of the zygomatic to the lacrimal and the parietal bone. Furthermore, this key role of epigenetic factors mediating zygomatic morphology is further suggested by the observation that the absence of normal muscle contraction during late embryo development leads to different zygomatic deformations (Hall, 2010). The null model also predicts a connection between zygomatic and palatine bones that is absent in the human skull. This articulation is prevented by the presence of the maxillary bone, which fills the space between zygomatics and palatines rapidly due to its faster maturation rate (Buschang et al. 1983; Enlow & Hans, 1996; Bastir et al. 2006) and participates in the expansion of the sinus capsule (Moss & Greenberg, 1967; Smith et al. 2010). On the other hand, the null model does not predict the articulations of zygomatics to temporal and maxillary bones that naturally occur in the human skull. These bones would have to grow non-uniformly in order to establish these articulations. The same functional matrices that prevented the formation of articulations of the zygomatic to the lacrimal, parietal and palatine (i.e. eyeball, passing and attached muscles, and the expansion of the maxillary bone) might be responsible now for the particular irregular shape of the zygomatic that indeed allows its articulation with the temporal and maxillary bones.
Another group of articulations that the null model does not predict are midline connections between left and right maxillary and palatine bones, which make the hard palate of the skull. Thus, these articulations are not sufficiently explained by an unconstrained bone growth. Several forces would have to play a role for their formation; for example, by the nasal and oral cavities pushing these two bones to grow toward the midline in order to separate both functional spaces (Moss & Greenberg, 1967; Marazita & Mooney, 2004; Lieberman, 2011a).
Finally, the null model does not predict some articulations between unpaired and paired bones: frontal to lacrimals; vomer to maxillas; and ethmoid to inferior nasal conchas, lacrimals, nasals and palatines. All these bone articulations are structurally related to the organization of the facial skeleton surrounding the nasal cavity and the olfactory bulbs; thus, functional matrices are necessarily involved in the formation of these articulations as well (Bastir et al. 2011; Singleton, 2012).
Influence of bone growth relations on the formation of cranial sutures
In contrast to what happens in the facial skeleton, the null model successfully predicts most articulations among the bones of the braincase, which mainly occur within the cranial connectivity module (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). In accordance with the main premise of the null model, this result suggests that the sutural pattern in the braincase can emerge due to an unconstrained and uniform growth dynamics, that is, no additional processes need to be involved. In addition, this type of growth would also explain why the cranial connectivity module has a characteristic regular arrangement of bones (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). Furthermore, the only two bones showing exactly the same pattern of articulation in both networks, both in number and neighbors, are the sphenoid and the occipital. These two bones form the core of the cranial base, which has been reported as an evolutionary conserved developmental unit in tetrapods (McBratney-Owen et al. 2008). Moreover, this regular pattern of cranial sutures is also present in fishes such as the zebrafish (Quarto & Longaker, 2008). This result suggests that functional matrices nearby, such as the brain, do not constrain the establishment of their articulations, even though the shape and orientation of the cranial base in primates is highly integrated to facial size and cranial encephalization (Bastir et al. 2010; Bruner et al. 2014).
The brain and its dura mater are broadly recognized as epigenetic factors participating in the formation and maintenance of cranial sutures, as well as the shape of the cranial vault (Moss & Young, 1960; Smith & Tondury, 1978; Richtsmeier et al. 2006). Their prevalence, even over other epigenetic factors such as muscular movements and osteogenesis vs. apoptosis regulation (Pritchard et al. 1956; Ten Cate et al. 1977) is deeply rooted in mainstream explanations of the biology of craniofacial development and evolution (Lieberman, 2011b). However, brain size and cranial vault shape have co-evolved in hominins (Neubauer et al. 2009; Zollikofer & Ponce de Leon, 2010), suggesting that each influences the other's size and shape (Bruner, 2004; Bastir et al. 2011; Lieberman, 2011a). Indeed, evidence from studies on craniosynostosis suggests that the skull and the brain are so tightly integrated that there is no dominance of one over the other in shaping the final phenotype (Aldridge et al. 2005). Our conclusions regarding the lack of need to invoke extra forces to form the sutural pattern of the braincase might be seen as controversial. However, this conclusion by no means entails that other necessary forces imposed by the brain and related functional matrices have no effect in head development (Opperman et al. 1996; Morriss-Kay, 2001). Thus, processes of unconstrained bone growth would sufficiently explain articulations among neuro- and basicranial bones, whereas the growing brain would account for shape changes in cranial base and vault (Richtsmeier & Flaherty, 2013).
Conclusion
We have presented here a network growth model for the formation of bone–bone boundaries in the human skull, which has been built using information on growth co-dependences among bones captured in the anatomical network model of the human skull (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a). This null model is indeed a simplification of the actual skull development in mammals, where two or more ossification centers fuse into individual bones during growth (Sperber et al. 2010, chapter 6), the timing of ossification of the face and the braincase is decoupled (Koyabu et al. 2014), and the growth of some bones is biased by the active presence of functional matrices, as we have shown here. We have focused our anatomical network analysis on our simplified model, which is able to correctly show more than two-thirds of the sutures of the human skull; thus, the model suggests that those articulations can be sufficiently explained by a process of unconstrained bone growth. We will develop, in future work, a model of growth that will include the multiplicity of ossification centers, as well as their dissociated developmental timing, in order to extend the use of anatomical networks to fully model head development. For the same reason, we are presently working on extending skeletal networks to include soft tissues, such as muscles and tendons, in order to cover more complex problems in head development using the anatomical network analysis framework.
In summary, our results suggest that the formation of bone articulations in the braincase does not need the concurrence of any other developmental process modifying bone growth, such as functional matrices; however, the influence of functional matrices during head development is necessary to form the articulations among facial bones surrounding functional spaces or acting as muscular attachments. In an Evo-Devo context, our results agree with the presence of two skull developmental modules (facial vs. basicranial), which show semi-independent allometric growth patterns (Esteve-Altava et al. 2013a), and decoupled patterns of heterochrony in bone ossification in mammals (Koyabu et al. 2014). We are aware that our conclusions underplay the role of functional matrices as sole actors to fully explain craniofacial skeleton development. We hope that these findings will motivate new experimental and theoretical studies on the role of genetic and epigenetic factors in the evolution and development of the head skeleton.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Markus Bastir for bibliographic support, and two anonymous reviewers for many insightful comments. This research project was initially supported by grant (BFU2008-00643) from the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación. The authors also thank the Cavanilles Institute for Biodiversity and Evolutionary Biology for funding support.
Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article:
References
- Aldridge K, Kane AA, Marsh JL, et al. Brain morphology in nonsyndromic unicoronal craniosynostosis. Anat Rec. 2005;285:690–698. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastir M, Rosas A, O'Higgins P. Craniofacial levels and the morphological maturation of the human skull. J Anat. 2006;209:637–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2006.00644.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastir M, Rosas A, Lieberman DE, et al. Middle cranial fossa anatomy and the origin of modern humans. Anat Rec. 2008;291:130–140. doi: 10.1002/ar.20636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastir M, Rosas A, Stringer C, et al. Effects of brain and facial size on basicranial form in human and primate evolution. J Hum Evol. 2010;58:424–431. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastir M, Rosas A, Gunz P, et al. Evolution of the base of the brain in highly encephalized human species. Nat Commun. 2011;2:588. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borg I, Groenen PJF. Modern Multidimensional Scaling: Theory and Applications. New York: Springer; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Breitsprecher L, Fanghanel L, Noe A, et al. The functional anatomy of the muscles of facial expression in humans with and without cleft lip and palate. A contribution to refine muscle reconstruction in primary cheilo- and rhinoplasties in patients with uni- and bilateral complete CLP. Ann Anat. 2002;184:27–34. doi: 10.1016/s0940-9602(02)80030-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruner E. Geometric morphometrics and paleoneurology: brain shape evolution in the genus Homo. J Hum Evol. 2004;47:279–303. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruner E, De la Cuétara JM, Masters M, et al. Functional craniology and brain evolution: from paleontology to biomedicine. Front Neuroanat. 2014;8:00019. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2014.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschang PH, Baume RM, Nass GG. A craniofacial growth maturity gradient for males and females between 4 and 16 years of age. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1983;61:373–381. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330610312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheverud JM. Phenotypic, genetic, and enviromental morphological integration in the cranium. Evolution. 1982;36:499–516. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1982.tb05070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MM, Jr, MacLean RE. Craniosynostosis: Diagnosis, Evaluation, and Management. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Cray J, Jr, Cooper GM, Mooney MP, et al. Ectocranial suture fusion in primates: as related to cranial volume and dental eruption. J Med Primatol. 2012;41:356–363. doi: 10.1111/jmp.12018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis N, Witzel U, Fagan MJ. Development and three-dimensional morphology of the zygomaticotemporal suture in primate skulls. Folia Primatol. 2014;85:77–87. doi: 10.1159/000357526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Depew MJ, Simpson CA. 21st century neontology and the comparative development of the vertebrate skull. Dev Dyn. 2006;235:1256–1291. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.20796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Depew MJ, Compagnucci C, Griffin J. Suture neontology and paleontology: The bases for where, when and how boundaries between bones have been established and have evolved. In: Rice DP, editor. Craniofacial Sutures, Development, Disease and Treatment. London: Karger; 2008. pp. 57–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Ieva A, Bruner E, Davidson J, et al. Cranial sutures: a multidisciplinary review. Childs Nerv Syst. 2013;29:893–905. doi: 10.1007/s00381-013-2061-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dorogovtsev R, Mendes JFF. Evolution of Networks: from Biological Networks to the Internet and WWW. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Enlow DH, Hans MG. Essentials of Facial Growth. Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Esteve-Altava B, Rasskin-Gutman D. Theoretical morphology of tetrapod skull networks. CR Palevol. 2014;13:41–50. [Google Scholar]
- Esteve-Altava B, Marugán-Lobón J, Botella H, et al. Network models in anatomical systems. J Anthropol Sci. 2011;89:175–184. doi: 10.4436/jass.89016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esteve-Altava B, Marugán-Lobón J, Botella H, et al. Grist for Riedl's mill: a network model perspective on the integration and modularity of the human skull. J Exp Zool B Mol Dev Evol. 2013a;320:489–500. doi: 10.1002/jez.b.22524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esteve-Altava B, Marugán-Lobón J, Botella H, et al. Structural constraints in the evolution of the tetrapod skull complexity: Williston's law revisited using network models. Evol Biol. 2013b;40:209–219. [Google Scholar]
- Esteve-Altava B, Marugán-Lobón J, Botella H, et al. Random loss and selective fusion of bones originate morphological complexity trends in tetrapod skull networks. Evol Biol. 2014;41:52–61. [Google Scholar]
- Falk D, Zollikofer CP, Morimoto N, et al. Metopic suture of Taung (Australopithecus africanus) and its implications for hominin brain evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:8467–8470. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1119752109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Festa F, Capasso L, D'Anastasio R, et al. Maxillary and mandibular base size in ancient skulls and of modern humans from Opi, Abruzzi, Italy: a cross-sectional study. World J Orthod. 2010;11:e1–e4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields HWJ, Metzner L, Garol JD, et al. The craniofacial skeleton in anencephalic human fetuses. I. Cranial floor. Teratology. 1978;17:57–65. doi: 10.1002/tera.1420170113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabriel KR, Sokal RR. A new statistical approach to geographic variation analysis. Syst Zool. 1969;18:259. [Google Scholar]
- Hall BK. Homology: The Hierarchical Basis of Comparative Biology. San Diego: Academic Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Hall JG. Importance of muscle movement for normal craniofacial development. J Craniofac Surg. 2010;21:1336–1338. doi: 10.1097/SCS.0b013e3181ebcd4f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herring SW. Mechanical influences on suture development and patency. In: Rice DP, editor. Craniofacial Sutures, Development, Disease and Treatment. London: Karger; 2008. pp. 41–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirabayashi S, Harii K, Sakurai A, et al. An experimental study of craniofacial growth in a heterotopic rat head transplant. Plast Reconstr Surg. 1988;82:236–243. doi: 10.1097/00006534-198808000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphries MD, Gurney K. Network ‘small-world-ness’: a quantitative method for determining canonical network equivalence. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e0002051. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karsenty G. The genetic transformation of bone biology. Genes Dev. 1999;13:3037–3051. doi: 10.1101/gad.13.23.3037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khonsari RH, Olivier J, Vigneaux P, et al. A mathematical model for mechanotransduction at the early steps of suture formation. Proc Biol Sci. 2013;280:20122670. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kikuchi M. Orthodontic treatment in children to prevent sleep-disordered breathing in adulthood. Sleep Breath. 2005;9:146–158. doi: 10.1007/s11325-005-0028-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koyabu D, Werneburg I, Morimoto N, et al. Mammalian skull heterochrony reveals modular evolution and a link between cranial development and brain size. Nat Commun. 2014;5:3625. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruskal JB, Wish M. Multidimensional Scaling. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Li A, Horvath S. Network neighborhood analysis with the multi-node topological overlap measure. Bioinformatics. 2007;23:222–231. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btl581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman DE. Epigenetic integration, complexity, and evolvability of the head: rethinking the functional matrix hypothesis. In: Hallgrimsson B, Hall BK, editors. Epigenetics: Linking Genotype and Phenotype in Development and Evolution. Los Angeles: California University Press; 2011a. pp. 271–289. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman DE. The Evolution of the Human Head. London: Belknap Press; 2011b. [Google Scholar]
- Marazita ML, Mooney MP. Current concepts in the embryology and genetics of cleft lip and cleft palate. Clin Plast Surg. 2004;31:125–140. doi: 10.1016/S0094-1298(03)00138-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McBratney-Owen B, Iseki S, Bamforth SD, et al. Development and tissue origins of the mammalian cranial base. Dev Biol. 2008;322:121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2008.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morriss-Kay GM. Derivation of the mammalian skull vault. J Anat. 2001;199:143–151. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-7580.2001.19910143.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss ML. A theoretical analysis of the functional matrix. Acta Biotheor. 1968;18:195–202. doi: 10.1007/BF01556727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss ML. New studies of cranial growth. Birth Defects Orig Artic Ser. 1975;11:283–295. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss ML. The differential roles of periosteal and capsular functional matrices in orofacial growth. Euro J Orthod. 2007;29:i96–i101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss ML, Greenberg SN. Functional cranial analysis of the human maxillary bone: I, Basal bone. Angle Orthod. 1967;37:151–164. doi: 10.1043/0003-3219(1967)037<0151:FCAOTH>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss ML, Salentijn L. The capsular matrix. Am J Orthod. 1969a;56:474–490. doi: 10.1016/0002-9416(69)90209-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss ML, Salentijn L. The primary role of functional matrices in facial growth. Am J Orthod. 1969b;55:566–577. doi: 10.1016/0002-9416(69)90034-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss ML, Young RW. A functional approach to craniology. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1960;18:281–292. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330180406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulliken JB, Ferraro NF, Vento AR. A retrospective analysis of growth of the constructed condyle-ramus in children with hemifacial microsomia. Cleft Palate J. 1989;26:312–317. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neubauer S, Gunz P, Hublin JJ. The pattern of endocranial ontogenetic shape changes in humans. J Anat. 2009;215:240–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2009.01106.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman MEJ. Power laws, Pareto distributions and Zipf's law. Contemp Phys. 2005;46:323–351. [Google Scholar]
- Newman ME, Girvan M. Finding and evaluating community structure in networks. Phys Rev E. 2004;69:026113. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.69.026113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nonaka K, Nakata M. Genetic variation and craniofacial growth in inbred rats. J Craniofac Genet Dev Biol. 1984;4:271–302. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opperman LA. Cranial sutures as intramembranous bone growth sites. Dev Dyn. 2000;219:472–485. doi: 10.1002/1097-0177(2000)9999:9999<::AID-DVDY1073>3.0.CO;2-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opperman LA, Passarelli RW, Nolen AA, et al. Dura mater secretes soluble heparin-binding factors required for cranial suture morphogenesis. In Vitro Cell Dev Biol. 1996;32:627–632. [Google Scholar]
- Pritchard JJ, Scott JH, Girgis FG. The structure and development of cranial and facial sutures. J Anat. 1956;90:73–86. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quarto N, Longaker MT. The zebrafish (Danio rerio): a model system for cranial suture patterning. Cells Tissues Organs. 2008;181:109–118. doi: 10.1159/000091100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasskin-Gutman D. Boundary constraints for the emergence of form. In: Müller G, Newman S, editors. Origination of Organismal Form. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2003. pp. 305–322. [Google Scholar]
- Rasskin-Gutman D, Esteve-Altava B. Connecting the dots: anatomical network analysis in morphological EvoDevo. Biol Theory. 2014;9:178–193. [Google Scholar]
- Ravasz E, Barabási A-L. Hierarchical organization in complex networks. Phys Rev E. 2003;67:026112. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.67.026112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravasz E, Somera AL, Mongru DA, et al. Hierarchical organization of modularity in metabolic networks. Science. 2002;297:1551–1555. doi: 10.1126/science.1073374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice DP. Craniofacial Sutures: Development, Disease, and Treatment. Basel: Karger; 2008. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards GD, Jabbour RS. Foramen magnum ontogeny in Homo sapiens: a functional matrix perspective. Anat Rec. 2011;294:199–216. doi: 10.1002/ar.21319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richtsmeier JT, Flaherty K. Hand in glove: brain and skull in development and dysmorphogenesis. Acta Neuropathol. 2013;125:469–489. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1104-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richtsmeier JT, Aldridge K, DeLeon VB, et al. Phenotypic integration of neurocranium and brain. J Exp Zool B Mol Dev Evol. 2006;306:360–378. doi: 10.1002/jez.b.21092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sardi ML, Ramirez Rozzi F, Pucciarelli HM. The neolithic transition in Europe and North Africa. The functional craneology contribution. Anthropol Anz. 2004;62:129–145. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shwartz Y, Farkas Z, Stern T, et al. Muscle contraction controls skeletal morphogenesis through regulation of chondrocyte convergent extension. Dev Biol. 2012;370:154–163. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2012.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh N, Harvati K, Hublin JJ, et al. Morphological evolution through integration: a quantitative study of cranial integration in Homo, Pan, Gorilla and Pongo. J Hum Evol. 2012;62:155–164. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singleton M. Postnatal cranial development in papionin primates: an alternative model for hominin evolutionary development. Evol Biol. 2012;39:499–520. [Google Scholar]
- Smith DW, Tondury G. Origin of the calvaria and its sutures. Am J Dis Child. 1978;132:662–666. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1978.02120320022004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TD, Rossie JB, Cooper GM, et al. The maxillary sinus in three genera of new world monkeys: factors that constrain secondary pneumatization. Anat Rec. 2010;293:91–107. doi: 10.1002/ar.21017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solé RV, Valverde S, Rodríguez-Caso C. Modularity in biological networks. In: Képès F, editor. Biological Networks. Singapore: World Scientific; 2006. pp. 21–40. [Google Scholar]
- Sperber GH, Sperber MS, Guttmann GD. Craniofacial Embriogenetics and Development. Shelton: People's Medical Publishing House-USA; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Sugawara Y, Hirabayashi S, Harii K. Craniofacial growth in a whole rat head transplant: how does a non-functional head grow? J Craniofac Genet Dev Biol. 1999;19:102–108. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ten Cate AR, Freeman E, Dickinson JB. Sutural development: structure and its response to rapid expansion. Am J Orthod. 1977;71:622–636. doi: 10.1016/0002-9416(77)90279-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watts DJ, Strogatz SH. Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks. Nature. 1998;393:440–442. doi: 10.1038/30918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wuchty S, Ravasz E, Barabási A-L. The architecture of biological networks. In: Deisboeck TS, Kresh JT, editors. Complex Systems Science in Biomedicine. New York, NY: Springer; 2006. pp. 165–181. [Google Scholar]
- Yip AM, Horvath S. Gene network interconnectedness and the generalized topological overlap measure. BMC Bioinformatics. 2007;8:22. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-8-22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang G, Eames BF, Cohn MJ. Evolution of vertebrate cartilage development. Curr Top Dev Biol. 2009;86:15–42. doi: 10.1016/S0070-2153(09)01002-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zollikofer CP, Ponce de Leon MS. The evolution of hominin ontogenies. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2010;21:441–452. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2009.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.