Abstract
The state of the sensory cortical network can have a profound impact on neural responses and perception. In rodent auditory cortex, sensory responses are reported to occur in the context of network events, similar to brief UP states, that produce “packets” of spikes and are associated with synchronized synaptic input (Bathellier et al., 2012; Hromadka et al., 2013; Luczak et al., 2013). However, traditional models based on data from visual and somatosensory cortex predict that ascending sensory thalamocortical (TC) pathways sequentially activate cells in layers 4 (L4), L2/3, and L5. The relationship between these two spatio-temporal activity patterns is unclear. Here, we used calcium imaging and electrophysiological recordings in murine auditory TC brain slices to investigate the laminar response pattern to stimulation of TC afferents. We show that although monosynaptically driven spiking in response to TC afferents occurs, the vast majority of spikes fired following TC stimulation occurs during brief UP states and outside the context of the L4>L2/3>L5 activation sequence. Specifically, monosynaptic subthreshold TC responses with similar latencies were observed throughout layers 2–6, presumably via synapses onto dendritic processes located in L3 and L4. However, monosynaptic spiking was rare, and occurred primarily in L4 and L5 non-pyramidal cells. By contrast, during brief, TC-induced UP states, spiking was dense and occurred primarily in pyramidal cells. These network events always involved infragranular layers, whereas involvement of supragranular layers was variable. During UP states, spike latencies were comparable between infragranular and supragranular cells. These data are consistent with a model in which activation of auditory cortex, especially supragranular layers, depends on internally generated network events that represent a non-linear amplification process, are initiated by infragranular cells and tightly regulated by feed-forward inhibitory cells.
Keywords: auditory cortex, thalamo-cortical (TC), UP states, canonical microcircuit, calcium imaging, multiunit activity, patch clamp
Introduction
For audition, somatosensation and vision, sensory information from the outside world is routed almost entirely through the thalamus, and is projected into neocortex via thalamo-cortical (TC) afferents. Classical experimental approaches to understanding how neocortex uses this information to construct perception relied on single cell recordings under passive (often unconscious) stimulation conditions, emphasizing bottom-up information streams and portraying neocortex as a passive receiver of sensory information (Mountcastle et al., 1957; Hubel and Wiesel, 1963; Gerstein and Kiang, 1964).These approaches provided enormous advances in understanding the hierarchical, columnar and topographic organization of neocortex (Creutzfeldt, 1977; Mountcastle, 1997; Kaas and Collins, 2001). In developing our current understanding of how the cortico-thalamic network constructs awareness, new emphasis is being placed on the pre-eminence of intracortical network activity that is perturbed periodically by volleys of ascending information (Bastos et al., 2012; Singer, 2013).This view is more consistent with the observations that perception depends heavily on internally-generated processes such as expectation, attention and arousal (Warren, 1970; Davis and Johnsrude, 2007; Fritz et al., 2007), that ascending afferents account for a small fraction of synaptic connections in neocortical circuits (Benshalom and White, 1986; Peters and Payne, 1993; Budd, 1998; Schoonover et al., 2014), and that cortical responses to sensory stimuli exhibit a high degree of trial-by-trial variability, due largely to variable cortical activity levels at the time of stimulation (Kisley and Gerstein, 1999; Lakatos et al., 2005; Curto et al., 2009; Pasley et al., 2009; White et al., 2012; Goris et al., 2014).
Nearly ubiquitous in classical descriptions of neocortical function, the “canonical microcircuit model” sought to capture the sequential activation of cells in a neocortical column driven by bottom-up input following a sensory stimulus. As originally conceived, the model consisted of a supragranular network mediating horizontal and ascending cortical-cortical connections, and an infragranular network sending descending cortico-cortical and subcortical projections (Douglas et al., 1989; Douglas and Martin, 1991). In later refinements of the model, based primarily on anatomical data in visual cortex of cat (Gilbert, 1983; Gilbert and Wiesel, 1983; Binzegger et al., 2004) and supported by data from paired recordings in cortical brain slices of rat and cat (Thomson and Bannister, 1998, 2003; Feldmeyer et al., 2002; Thomson et al., 2002; Lübke et al., 2003; Kampa et al., 2006; Thomson and Lamy, 2007), the model predicts a stereotyped pattern of information flow through cortex upon sensory stimulation: spiny stellate cells, excitatory interneurons in layer 4, are activated first by thalamic input, in turn driving ascending projection cells in supragranular layers, which drive descending projection cells in infragranular layers (Douglas and Martin, 2004; Hirsch and Martinez, 2006; Lubke and Feldmeyer, 2007).
In spite of its widespread acceptance, there is evidence that cortical spiking following sensory stimulation is governed by processes in addition to those described in this model. Although electrophysiological mapping studies have shown stronger synaptic strength and higher connection probability from L2/3 to L5 than the reverse (Thomson and Bannister, 1998), such data do not take into account the relative likelihood that cells will spike in response to sensory stimulation, i.e., the observation that L2/3 cells fire far more sparsely than cells in L5 (Sakata and Harris, 2009; Barth and Poulet, 2012), or that infragranular cells often fire with latencies as short or shorter than granular and supragranular cells (Maunsell and Gibson, 1992; Sugimoto et al., 1997; Shen et al., 1999; De Kock et al., 2007; Sakata and Harris, 2009; Christianson et al., 2011; Constantinople and Bruno, 2013; Sun et al., 2013). Indeed, most cortical cells have dendrites that traverse the thalamo-recipient layers, and thus may be activated monosynaptically (Bullier and Henry, 1979; Mitani and Shimokouchi, 1985; Douglas and Martin, 1991; Gil and Amitai, 1996; Verbny et al., 2006; Sun et al., 2013).
Other evidence suggests that features of the model may not apply to auditory cortex in particular. The canonical microcircuit was developed based on studies of visual cortex, where thalamic input contacts spiny stellate cells in layer 4 (Gilbert and Wiesel, 1983). However, spiny stellate cells are rare in auditory cortex, their role likely being occupied by pyramidal cells (Smith and Populin, 2001; Barbour and Callaway, 2008; Sakata and Harris, 2009). Additionally, evidence for receptive field hierarchy from granular to supragranular to infragranular layers is weak compared to visual cortex and is not associated with longer latencies in the infragranular layers (Atencio et al., 2009).
In contrast to the orderly progression of mono-, di- and tri-synaptically-driven spiking activity in layers 4, 2/3, and 5, respectively, predicted by the canonical microcircuit model, spiking in neocortex during sensory stimulation also occurs in the context of synchronous network bursts (Petersen et al., 2003; Deweese and Zador, 2006; Sakata and Harris, 2009). It should be noted that in auditory cortex in vivo, there is some debate about these network events, with some authors referring to them as “bumps” (Hromadka et al., 2013) and others as “UP states” (Sakata and Harris, 2009); for simplicity, we will refer to these events as UP states, but we note that there is some controversy regarding this terminology. Although most commonly observed during slow-wave sleep and certain types of anesthesia (Steriade et al., 1993), UP states may share some characteristics with the aroused state, including higher levels of ongoing activity (both excitatory and inhibitory) and depolarized membrane potentials (Steriade et al., 2001; Destexhe et al., 2003, 2007). Indeed, evidence suggests that cortical processing of sensory information occurs during similar, albeit briefer, events in the awake brain (Bathellier et al., 2012; Hromadka et al., 2013; Luczak et al., 2013; Tan et al., 2014). In this model, sensory stimuli trigger these network events with probability that depends on stimulus intensity and identity, in contrast to traditional views of sensory coding in which individual cells' response magnitudes are smoothly varying functions of stimulus properties. Although these events are regulated by subcortical inputs (Metherate et al., 1992; Goard and Dan, 2009; Constantinople and Bruno, 2011), similar activity has also been observed in auditory, visual and somatosensory brain slice preparations (Metherate and Cruikshank, 1999; Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; Cruikshank et al., 2002; MacLean et al., 2005; Watson et al., 2008; Rigas and Castro-Alamancos, 2009), where it has been shown to be non-epileptiform in nature and represent an in vitro correlate of UP states that occur in vivo (Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; Shu et al., 2003; Cunningham et al., 2006; Rigas and Castro-Alamancos, 2007). UP states likely arise in layer 5 before spreading to other laminae (Chauvette et al., 2010; Wester and Contreras, 2012; Beltramo et al., 2013; Stroh et al., 2013), and may represent an intracortical filter that regulates incorporation of sensory signals into the cortical hierarchical processing stream (MacLean et al., 2005). Selective activation of infragranular layers by sensory input (Constantinople and Bruno, 2013) and failure of some UP states to propagate to supragranular layers (Sakata and Harris, 2009) suggests that full engagement of the cortical column may only occur in certain contexts. Here, we present data consistent with a model in which activation of sensory neocortex, especially cells in supragranular layers, depends on internally generated network events initiated by infragranular cells, a process likely tightly regulated by monosynaptic activation of feed-forward inhibitory cells.
Materials and methods
All experimental protocols conformed to American Physiological Society/National Institutes of Health guidelines and were approved by the University of Wisconsin Animal Care and Use Committee.
Slice preparation
Male B6CBAF1/J mice (first generation cross of C57BL/6J and CBA/J) were used in these studies, as they represent genetically identical animals that lack recessive mutations known to affect sensory systems (Dräger and Hubel, 1978; Johnson et al., 1997). Mice (3–10 weeks, median 31 days old) were decapitated under isoflurane anesthesia, and their brains were extracted and immersed in cutting artificial CSF [cACSF; composed of (in mM) 111 NaCl, 35 NaHCO3, 20 HEPES, 1.8 KCl, 1.05 CaCl2, 2.8 MgSO4, 1.2 KH2PO4, and 10 glucose] at 0–4°C. HEPES was included to improve slice health and prevent edema (MacGregor et al., 2001). Auditory TC brain slices (450 μm) were prepared from the right hemisphere as previously described (Cruikshank et al., 2002; Verbny et al., 2006). Slices were maintained in cACSF saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2 at 24°C for >1 h before transfer to the recording chamber, which was perfused at 3–6 ml/min with ACSF [composed of (in mM) 111 NaCl, 35 NaHCO3, 20 HEPES, 1.8 KCl, 2.1 CaCl2, 1.4 MgSO4, 1.2 KH2PO4, and 10 glucose] at 30–34°C. Modified ACSF with elevated concentrations of divalent cations used in some calcium imaging experiments as described below was composed of 105 NaCl, 35 NaHCO3, 20 HEPES, 3 KCl, 4 CaCl2, 4.2 MgCl2, and 10 glucose. Auditory cortex was identified based on its position relative to the hippocampus, strong granular layer responses to stimulation of thalamic afferents, and in preliminary experiments by the location of cells retrogradely labeled from the inferior colliculus, as in previous studies (Verbny et al., 2006; Banks et al., 2011). Cortical layers were identified by differences in cell density and based on distance from the pia in conjunction with previous studies (Banks et al., 2011). Afferents were activated using pairs of tungsten electrodes (0.1 MΩ, 75 μm diameter; FHC Inc., Bowdoin, ME). Stimuli (100 μs, 10–150 μA) were applied using constant current stimulus isolation units (A365, WPI Inc., Sarasota, FL; or STG4002, Multichannel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany) and consisted of either single pulses or brief trains (2–4 pulses, 40 Hz). Extracellular recordings in layer 4 taken at 200–300 μm intervals were used to locate the region of auditory cortex best activated by the stimulus and all further extra-/intra-cellular recording and calcium imaging was performed in this region. We followed the well-described procedure for preparing auditory TC slices, and based on the appearance of the slices and the published stereotaxic coordinates of auditory cortex we are confident that all recordings presented here are from auditory cortex. However, the slicing procedure was originally described for juvenile animals (Cruikshank et al., 2002) in which connections between MGv and core auditory cortex could be maintained. Developmental studies have shown that the circuit and membrane properties of neocortex are not fully developed in juvenile animals (Metherate and Aramakis, 1999; Frick et al., 2007; Oswald and Reyes, 2008, 2011; Romand et al., 2011), consistent with observations in vivo showing developmental changes in tuning properties of auditory cortical neurons between 3 and 5 weeks of age (Chang and Merzenich, 2003; Chang et al., 2005). Thus, we confined our study to animals >3 weeks old, and in most cases >4 weeks old, in which the connections from MGv to core auditory cortex were not always intact. To activate auditory TC fibers in slices from older animals, we stimulated outside of MGv, just rostral to the nucleus or in the fiber bundle (the superior thalamic radiation) that runs from auditory thalamus to auditory cortex. We verified that stimulation of these fibers elicited current sinks in layer 4 in all experiments. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that some of our recordings were from areas outside of A1 that receive driving core auditory thalamic inputs, e.g., AuV or AuD in the Paxinos (Paxinos and Franklin, 2003) terminology. Indeed, given that the slices are 450 μm thick, even if we were to place stimulating electrodes in MGv and record responses in auditory cortex, it cannot be guaranteed that all the cells recorded are located in core auditory cortex and that we are only stimulating fibers arising in MGv.
Electrophysiological recordings
Electrophysiological recordings were obtained using patch pipettes fabricated from borosilicate glass (KG-33; 1.7 mm outer diameter; 1.1 mm inner diameter; Garner Glass, Claremont, CA) with a Flaming-Brown two-stage puller (P-87; Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA). For whole-cell current clamp recordings, pipettes had open tip resistances of 3–5 MΩ when filled with (in mM): 140 K-gluconate, 10 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 0.1 EGTA and 2 MgATP, 0.3% biocytin, pH 7.2. Cells were visualized using a video camera (Dage MTI VE-1000) connected to an upright microscope (Olympus BX51-WI) with a long working-distance water-immersion objective (Olympus 40X, 0.9 N.A.) and differential interference contrast optics. For extracellular field potentials, the tips of the pipettes were broken under visual control to an outer tip diameter of 10–15 μm and had open-tip resistances of about 0.5 MΩ when filled with ACSF. Data were amplified (MultiClamp-700A; Molecular Devices, Union City, CA), low-pass filtered (4 kHz), digitized (40 kHz; DigiData 1322A; Molecular Devices), and recorded using pClamp version 9.2 (Molecular Devices). Current source density (CSD) and multiunit activity (MUA) was derived from extracellular recordings in which we recorded local field potentials simultaneously in all cortical layers using 16-channel silicon electrode arrays (A-series probes; 16 shanks spaced by 100 μm, one recording site per shank; 1 MΩ impedance; NeuroNexus, Ann Arbor, MI) connected to a unity-gain headstage (HS-16; Neuralynx, Bozeman, MT), amplified 2000× (Lynx-8, Neuralynx), digitized at 20 kHz (Digidata 1440A; Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, CA) and recorded using pClamp (Molecular Devices). The electrode array was inserted into the slice at an acute angle with the shanks oriented in parallel to the cortical laminae. Depending on the cortical thickness at the recording site, 11–13 of the electrode shanks were in cortex and used in calculating the CSD and MUA.
Calcium imaging
Calcium imaging using the acetoxymethyl ester form of Oregon Green BAPTA-1 (OGB-1 AM; Life Technologies, Grand Island, NY) was performed as described previously (Banks et al., 2011). Briefly, patch pipettes of ~2 μm outer tip diameter were filled with OGB-1 AM dissolved in DMSO containing 20% pluronic acid to a dye concentration of 5 mM and then gradually diluted using extracellular solution containing (in mM) 150 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 10 HEPES, pH 7.4 to a final dye concentration of 0.165 mM. The tissue was loaded using pressure ejection (Picospitzer II, General Valve, Fairfield, NJ; 5–10 PSI, 1–10 s pulses over 1–2 min) at 5–7 locations over a period of ~45 min. The total area labeled in this way measured ~200 × 800 μm. Data collection commenced ~60 min following the last injection. Data were collected using an upright microscope (BX51-WI, Olympus, Center Valley, PA), mercury arc lamp light source (X-Cite exacte; Lumen Dynamics, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada) with an excitation filter 475–505 nm and emission filter 520–570 nm (U-N41026, Chroma, Bellows Falls, VT), and a 10× water immersion objective (UMPlanFL N, N.A. = 0.3, Olympus). Images were captured at 30 frames/sec with a cooled CCD camera (500 × 500 pixels, corresponding to 800 × 800 μm, 16-bit; C9100-02, Hamamatsu Corp., Sewickley, PA) using SimplePCI software (v6.1, Hamamatsu). For calcium imaging experiments (except when multiple stimulus intensities are shown), the stimulus intensity was set high enough to trigger UP states under normal ACSF on consecutive trials using four pulses at 40 Hz and low enough that UP states were blocked using high divalent ACSF, typically 50–100 μA.
Data analysis
Electrophysiological recordings
EPSP latencies and amplitudes were obtained from averages of ten responses to single 100 μA pulses. EPSP latencies were measured as the time of rise to 10% of the peak. EPSP amplitude was measured as rest-to-peak and cells were omitted from amplitude measurement if they had a coincident IPSP revealed by depolarizing the cell. IPSPs were identified by stimulating while holding the cell at depolarized potentials using small current steps up to spike threshold. Step size varied with the input resistance, resting potential, and spike threshold of the cells as necessary to result in 5–10 steps between rest and threshold. Spike thresholds were measured as the membrane potential at the peak of the 2nd derivative of the voltage trace. Spike latencies were measured as time to peak. Monosynaptic spikes were those that occurred in response to a smooth, putative monosynaptic EPSP with short latency and low latency jitter (EPSP latency <5 ms, SD < 1 ms; spike latency <10 ms, SD < 1 ms) (Berry and Pentreath, 1976; Rose and Metherate, 2005). We note that the actual EPSP latency jitter preceding monosynaptic spikes was much more precise than this criterion (jitter mean ± SD: 0.23 ± 0.21 ms).
UP states were detected using the MUA signal obtained in extracellular recordings (using multichannel electrodes or glass field potential electrodes) from layer 5 using a method similar to that of Sakata and Harris (2009). We extracted the MUA signal by bandpass filtering the voltage signal between 0.5 and 3 kHz, taking the absolute value, and then smoothing with a lowpass filter (0.2 kHz cutoff) to yield the smoothed MUA signal, smMUA (t). A threshold was determined by computing the geometric mean x of smMUA (t) on data points greater than the mean of smMUA (t) during the pre-stimulus period. UP state onset was defined as the time at which smMUA (t) >x for 80% of the points in a 20 ms window, and offset was defined as the subsequent time when smMUA (t) <x for 80% of the points in a following 20 ms window. For multichannel recordings, CSD (Freeman and Nicholson, 1975; Mitzdorf, 1985) was estimated using the spline CSD method (Pettersen et al., 2006). MUA in the form of single action potentials was extracted from these same multichannel recordings by bandpass filtering the extracellular signal between 0.5 and 3 kHz and detecting negative-going level crossings using a threshold of 5 times the standard deviation of the baseline period.
We sought to compare directly the observed pattern of spiking responses observed via calcium imaging to the pattern we observed in on-cell recordings, in which we measured the number of spikes fired for each cell over a series of 10 trials. To compare these spiking probabilities to the calcium imaging data, we converted the spiking probability to the likelihood to detect a cell according to our calcium imaging calibration data illustrated in Figure 8F (using linear interpolation when necessary) and averaged across cells in each layer.
All statistical analyses of electrophysiological data used native MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA) functions. Non-parametric tests were used when data were distributed significantly different from the normal distribution (one-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, p < 0.05). When multiple pairwise comparisons were performed, significance thresholds were adjusted using the Holm–Bonferroni method (nominal α = 0.05); p-values were not adjusted.
Calcium imaging
Fluorescence traces (reported as ΔF/F, not %ΔF/F) were analyzed as described (Banks et al., 2011). Briefly, ΔF/F as a function of time was measured in single cells identified in averaged still images using custom software written in MATLAB. Traces were background-subtracted and peaks corresponding to action potential activity were detected. Cells were identified as “responsive” when the sustained peak increase in fluorescence (over a window of 4 data points or 133 ms) was greater than 3 times the standard deviation of a 1-s baseline before stimulation. In some experiments, we targeted responsive cells identified via calcium imaging for subsequent whole-cell patch clamp (see Results). Spikes were detected in all recordings of these cells, confirming that observed calcium transients were associated with action potentials. We computed the laminar spiking profile within a single experiment (e.g., Figure 7D) by dividing the cortical depth into 50 μm bins and counting the number of cells in each bin. When data were pooled across experiments (e.g., Figure 10), bin size was expanded to 100 μm to make plots easier to read. All analyses presented here were insensitive to this change in bin size. To compare relative dye (OGB-1) levels across analysis windows, the average baseline fluorescence was integrated across space and normalized to the bin with the largest integrated baseline fluorescence from that experiment. By using an integral rather than a spatial average, this measure incorporates variability in background (labeling intensity) due to heterogeneous loading as well as depth bins that fell outside the square imaging window (particularly layers 1 and deep layer 6).
Statistical comparisons between layers and between bathing media (normal ACSF or ACSF modified with high divalent cations or APV) were made by fitting a Poisson, log-linked Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) model to cell count measures normalized by layer thickness (Reed and Kaas, 2010). A standard ANOVA or general linear model (GLM) approach is less appropriate because the cell count data are not normally distributed, resulting in poor error estimates, especially when some counts are very low or zero. GEE models are suitable for repeated measures data (here, repeated across layers in each experiment and within layers for each ACSF condition) in which population effects are of interest but within-subject prediction is not necessary. Because of the logarithmic link function, a linear combination of parameters results in a multiplicative effect on the outcome data, in this case cell count. When comparing level parameters, we computed the Wald test on the parameter estimates themselves, but then present the exponent of the parameter, e.g., as “exp(βCondition),” and its confidence interval because this exponent is an easily understood multiplicative factor to the cell counts. Significance thresholds were adjusted using the Holm–Bonferroni method as above.
The GEE model was fit using IBM SPSS Statistics 22. Post-hoc tests and all other analyses used native and custom MATLAB functions.
Histology
Slices containing biocytin-filled cells were carefully removed from the recording chamber and immediately fixed in freshly prepared 4% paraformaldehyde solution. After at least 24 h in fixative, slices were sectioned and processed for biocytin staining using the cobalt/nickel intensification method (Adams, 1981). First, the slice was cryoprotected by passage through a series of glycerol–sucrose solutions. Labeled cells were often close to the slice surface so to assure that this tissue was not lost in the sectioning process the following method was used. The slice was placed on a glass slide and covered with a drop (s) of a 4% paraformaldehyde (1 ml)/egg albumin (10 ml) solution. A second slide was gently laid over the slice to flatten it. After the albumin had solidified the slides were removed and the flattened albumin-embedded slice was mounted on a freezing microtome and 60 or 90 μm sections were cut and collected in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (pH 7.4). Sections were then washed in 0.01 M phosphate buffered saline (PBS, pH 7.4), incubated in 0.5% H2O2 in PBS, and rinsed again in 0.01 M PBS. Sections were then incubated overnight in the secondary antibody to the avidin-biotin-HRP complex (ABC Kit, Vector Labs) in PBS with 0.3% Triton-X-100 and 2% BSA. On the second day, the sections were rinsed in 0.1 M PBS and the HRP was reacted using 0.04% diaminobenzidine with DMSO and nickel/cobalt intensification. We then added 0.01% H2O2 in 0.1 M PBS for 15 min followed by rinsing for 40 min in PBS. The sections were then mounted on slides, allowed to air dry and the mounted sections were then stained with cresyl violet and coverslipped. Photomicrographs of labeled cells were taken at 5, 25, and 100×. For the image in Figure 4E, the major processes of the labeled cell were found in two tissue sections. To overlay the images and produce a unified picture, the position of the superficial section relative to the deeper section was determined by aligning the 5X images with common structures. The Eyedropper tool in Adobe Photoshop was used to create a color mask to remove the background and Nissl-stained cells from the superficial section, leaving the darkly labeled processes, and this image was merged with the unaltered deep image section. The “auto-contrast” function in Photoshop was used on both photomicrographs in Figure 4 to improve the differentiation between the Nissl-stained cell bodies in the background and the biocytin-labeled somata and processes of interest. All image processing steps were performed across the entire image and did not alter the information contained.
Results
Overview
The data presented here were obtained to answer a simple question: what is the pattern of spiking elicited in auditory cortex by stimulation of core thalamic afferents? In classical models of the cortical microcircuit, TC afferents terminating in layer 4 activate excitatory interneurons (spiny stellate cells) resident in that layer, and spiking activity in these cells triggers sequential activation of pyramidal cells and interneurons in supragranular and then in infragranular layers. In primary auditory cortex, where spiny stellate cells are rare or absent (Smith and Populin, 2001; Barbour and Callaway, 2008), it is unclear whether this activation sequence is preserved. To address this issue, we recorded from 103 cells selected at random throughout layers 2–6 (L2/3 = 21, L4 = 19, L5 = 51, L6 = 12) using whole-cell patch clamp, and 104 cells selected at random throughout layers 2–6 (L2/3 = 27, L4 = 22, L5 = 47, L6 = 8) using on-cell recordings. We focus first on short latency, putatively monosynaptic responses, showing that spiking directly in response to these inputs is rare. We then show that spiking is common in the context of induced UP states and examine the laminar profile of this activity. We corroborate these findings from single cell recordings using two techniques that allow us to monitor spiking activity simultaneously across the cortical laminae: multichannel recordings of MUA (n = 16 slices) and calcium imaging using bulk loading of neurons with OGB-1AM (n = 1588 cells in 17 slices).
Laminar profile of current sinks activated by TC stimulation
We first verified that stimulation of TC afferents resulted in the expected laminar profile of synaptic current flow using multichannel recordings (Figure 1A). TC stimulation resulted in an early, large, fast sink (“early responses,” LFP Figure 1B; CSD Figure 1C; time to 10% of peak = 2.0 ± 1.1 ms, n = 16 slices) located in layer 4 (mean depth = 36 ± 5% of cortical depth) that sometimes extended into layer 3, as expected based on the laminar distribution of terminals in primary auditory cortex arising from MGv (Huang and Winer, 2000; Smith and Populin, 2001; Smith et al., 2012). There was usually an additional, weaker sink observed simultaneously in layer 6, which also receives direct MGv input. We typically observed modest depression of current sinks evoked by TC stimulation trains as seen previously with TC EPSPs (Rose and Metherate, 2005; Lee and Sherman, 2008).
Most of the data presented here were obtained in animals >4 weeks old, in which it is difficult or impossible to obtain slices with intact fiber bundles between MGv and auditory cortex. Thus, in most experiments stimulating electrodes were placed just rostral to the MGv, where the TC fibers coalesce, or in the superior thalamic radiation, just rostral and medial to hippocampus. In four experiments from 3 to 4 week old animals, we compared directly the current sinks obtained by stimulating in these locations with those obtained by stimulating in MGv. The results of one of these experiments are illustrated in Figure 1. In the same slice illustrated in Figures 1A–C, we moved the stimulating electrode to the fiber bundle just rostral to the hippocampus (Figure 1D) and obtained similar LFP/CSD patterns (Figures 1E,F) as those elicited by direct MGv stimulation (Figures 1B,C). Similar results were obtained in all four experiments in which we compared directly stimulation in MGv, just rostral to MGv, and in the fiber bundle rostral to hippocampus. Note that larger current sinks could be obtained by stimulating in the superior thalamic radiation compared to MGv itself. These results suggest that stimulation in the fiber bundle in older animals elicited responses that are equivalent to stimulating in MGv itself.
We summarize the results of 16 such recordings in older animals in Figure 1G, which shows the laminar profiles of current sinks evoked by TC stimulation. To compare across slices with slightly varying cortical depths, we normalized the single experiment profiles by dividing by the distance from the pia to white matter. These recordings clearly show that the vast majority of synaptic current flow in response to TC stimulation occurs in layers 3 and 4, with a modest response component in layer 6. In none of these experiments were early current sinks observed in layers 1, 2, or 5, indicating that activation of “matrix” (i.e., non-specific or extralemniscal) thalamic afferents in our stimulation protocol was minimal.
Brief UP states triggered by TC stimulation
In addition to early response components, TC stimulation typically triggered network events, seen in CSD profiles (Figures 1C,F) as intense patterns of sources and sinks subsequent to the initial TC response. During these events, cells throughout the cortical region depolarized nearly simultaneously under a barrage of synaptic inputs (Figure 2A), and occasionally fired action potentials. We also observed spontaneous UP states in 5/10 slices examined, but at low rates (0.2–2 per minute). Between these occasional events, the slice was in a persistent DOWN state, as is typical of cortical brain slices (Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000).
The UP state activity appeared in extracellular recordings in layer 5 as a low frequency negative or sometimes biphasic wave with associated high frequency (spiking) activity (Figure 2B, top) that often spread to involve all cortical layers, as previously described in vitro and in vivo (Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; Luczak et al., 2007; Sakata and Harris, 2009; Chauvette et al., 2010; Wester and Contreras, 2012; Beltramo et al., 2013). Following the method of Sakata and Harris (Sakata and Harris, 2009) for data recorded in vivo, we detected these events using the MUA signal derived from extracellular recordings in layer 5 (Figure 2B, bottom; see Materials and Methods). Below, we show that although synaptic responses to TC afferents are widespread in auditory cortex, it is rare for cells to spike in direct response to these EPSPs. Rather, the vast majority of spiking in response to TC stimulation occurs instead in the context of these UP states.
Short latency responses to TC stimulation
Using whole-cell patch clamp recordings, we examined the intracellular responses to stimulation of TC afferents in 103 cells (n = 21, 19, 51, and 12 cells randomly selected from layers 2/3, 4, 5, and 6, respectively; note that because layer 5 is roughly twice as thick as layers 2/3 and 4, this represents a roughly uniform sampling density for layers 2/3 through 5). Although the synaptic terminals of stimulated afferents target specific layers of cortex, we found that TC input triggered short latency EPSPs in cells of all layers (Figure 3A), most likely by synapsing on these cells' dendritic processes that span thalamo-recipient layers. Latencies were similar across the cortical laminae (Figure 3B; median latencies: L2/3, 3.0 ms; L4, 2.9 ms; L5, 3.0 ms; L6, 2.4 ms; no significant difference, Kruskal–Wallis H = 2.79, 3 d.f., p = 0.43). These short latencies are consistent with nearly all these responses being monosynaptic (Rose and Metherate, 2005; Verbny et al., 2006), except perhaps a few >5 ms in layers 5 and 6; likewise, the similarity of latencies across laminae is inconsistent with EPSPs in layers 2/3 and 5 being di- and tri-synaptic responses driven by intracortical spiking activity. The amplitude distribution of the initial EPSPs was more heterogeneous (Figure 3C; Kruskal–Wallis H = 14.79, 3 d.f., p = 0.002): the largest EPSPs were observed in layer 4, but we observed large EPSPs (>5 mV) in layers 3–6 (median EPSP amplitudes: L2/3:2.9 mV; L4:8.1 mV; L5: 5.2 mV; L6:4.7 mV; L4 significantly greater than each of the other layers by Tukey HSD).
Although many cells throughout the cortical laminae received monosynaptic TC inputs, spiking in response to these short latency EPSPs was rare. In these 103 cells, spikes in response to putatively monosynaptic thalamic EPSPs triggered by the first TC stimulus were observed in only four cells, two in layer 4 and two in layer 5 (Figure 3D). Both layer 4 cells (one example Figures 4A–D) and one of the layer 5 cells were fast-spiking interneurons. The fast-spiking cells were smooth, non-pyramidal cells (Figure 4A) and fit the classical characteristics of parvalbumin-positive interneurons (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997). The fourth monosynaptic-spiking cell was an intrinsically bursting pyramidal cell in layer 5 (Figures 4E–H). We defined these four cells as “monosynaptic spiking” cells based on the EPSPs leading to spikes being short latency (like the EPSPs of Figure 3A) and smooth, without evidence of additional di- or poly-synaptic activity prior to spike onset. We note that we are unable to ascertain without a doubt that these spikes were driven only by monosynaptic inputs, but given their latency and their occurrence prior to induced UP states, these are the only cells we observed that could potentially be driven monosynaptically. In both of the cells pictured, these monosynaptic spikes were followed by UP state activity such as in Figure 2 which was associated with additional spikes driven by polysynaptic activity. In the remaining 99 whole-cell recordings, all cells that spiked following TC stimulation fired late, UP state-associated spikes.
Spiking responses are dominated by activity during UP states
The observed sparseness of monosynaptic spiking responses to thalamic stimulation is surprising, given the pre-eminence of TC pathways for carrying sensory information to cortex. Because the intracellular milieu is disturbed during whole-cell recordings, it is possible that spike thresholds and spiking probability were altered in the recordings presented above. In 33 cells, we obtained on-cell recordings prior to the whole-cell recordings from Figures 3, 4, including two of the previously described cells that fired monosynaptic spikes according to our definition. In on-cell recordings, both of these cells also fired short-latency (<10 ms) spikes to TC stimulation with high temporal precision (standard deviation of spike times <1 ms) in the on-cell recordings. None of the other 31 cells fired spikes that met these criteria. We also recorded on-cell spiking responses to TC stimulation in 71 additional cells (for a total of 104 on-cell recordings). None of these cells fired spikes that met that <10 ms latency and <1 ms standard deviation criterion. We acknowledge that we would be unable to confidently identify monosynaptic-spiking cells using the on-cell data alone, but emphasize that no cells in the “on-cell only” dataset met even these less stringent criteria.
Despite the apparent paucity of monosynaptic spiking, TC stimulation consistently elicited robust spiking activity in the context of evoked UP states (Figure 2). To investigate the laminar profile of this spiking activity and its latency distribution, we sequentially and systematically measured spike times during on-cell recordings from randomly targeted cells at different cortical depths in the same slice (n = 4–9 cells in each slice) while simultaneously recording extracellularly from layer 5 to monitor the occurrence of evoked UP states. The data from eight cells recorded in one such experiment are illustrated in Figure 5. As was typical for these experiments, all spikes occurred during UP states (gray background) and at substantial delays relative to the first stimulus in the train. At low stimulus intensities (Figure 5A), spikes were observed only in layer 5 cells. Increasing the stimulus intensity (Figure 5B) decreased the latency of UP states and triggered spiking activity additionally in granular and supragranular cells, but the density of spiking was greater for cells in layer 5, and the spike latency in these cells was often shorter than that of cells in layers 2–4. Electrophysiological responses to polarizing current pulses are shown in Figure 5C; most of the cells in this experiment were regular spiking and likely pyramidal; the deepest layer 5 cell was intrinsically bursting.
We summarize data from all 104 cells in Figure 6A by showing the laminar profile of the spiking activity averaged across cells in each layer. Here we use a more inclusive definition of “early” spikes that included all spikes that came before the onset of UP states and no later than 20 ms after the final stimulus pulse. It is clear from these data that thalamic stimuli are effective at triggering spiking in auditory cortex, but almost all of these spikes occurred during UP states. Almost all early spikes occurred in layers 4 and 5. Spike rates were highest in layer 5, but spikes were detected in all laminae examined. In Figures 6B,C, we have plotted the cumulative distributions of latency relative to stimulus onset (Figure 6B) or relative to UP state onset (Figure 6C) for layers 2/3, 4, and 5. It is apparent that the earliest cells to spike are in layer 5 (Figure 6B, arrow), and that majority of cells in layers 2/3 and layer 5 are activated at about the same latency (no significant difference between distributions, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test), both relative to stimulus onset and UP state onset. The high firing rate of layer 5 cells and the fact that early spikes were nearly non-existent in layers 2/3 are more consistent with a model in which layer 5 is directly activated by TC input and subsequently by intralaminar activity, as opposed to being driven by layer 2/3 cells in a sequential layer 4 to layer 2/3 to layer 5 pattern as predicted by the canonical microcircuit.
The advantage of data obtained from patch clamp recordings such as those shown in Figures 5, 6 is that we can identify spikes associated with individual cells, and we can identify the laminar position of the cell body and thus the position of the cell in the cortical microcircuit. However, it was necessary to obtain these recordings sequentially, which could potentially distort the laminar spiking profile by adding variability in latencies that could obscure the progression of latencies in different cell types. To complement these single cell recordings, we also used multichannel MUA recordings to record from large numbers of cells simultaneously across the cortical laminae. To look at relative spiking latency between layers during UP states, we counted spikes in each layer in 5 ms bins after UP state onset, divided by the maximum spikes per bin for that layer for that experiment, and then averaged these normalized spike rates across experiments (Figure 6D). The time of spiking onset, defined as the earliest bin with at least 10% as many spikes as the peak bin, was significantly different between layers [Friedman test, χ2(3) = 28.84, p = 0.000002]; layer 5 was activated at the shortest latency (post-hoc Wilcoxon signed-rank test; z = −2.79, p = 0.0052; z = −2.36, p = 0.0181; z = −3.33, p = 0.0009 for comparison to layers 2/3, 4, and 6, respectively) and layer 2/3 at longest latency relative to the others (z = −4.14, p = 0.00003; z = −2.80, p = 0.0051 for comparison to layers 4 and 6, respectively); layers 4 and 6 were not significantly different from each other. These data are consistent with those obtained from single cell experiments.
In comparing the laminar EPSP amplitude profile (Figure 3C) with the laminar spiking profile observed in electrophysiological recordings (Figures 5, 6), it is clear that this profile is not simply a function of the monosynaptic EPSP amplitude distribution. However, it is consistent with the laminar profile of resting potentials recorded in our whole-cell current clamp recordings (mean ± SD: EL2/3 = −78.1 ± 5.5 mV; EL4 = −70.1 ± 7.9 mV; EL5 = −65.2 ± 5.4 mV; EL6 = −72.3 ± 4.3 mV), which varied significantly by layer [One-Way ANOVA, F(3, 93) = 24.96, p < 0.0000001]. Pairwise post-hoc tests (Tukey's HSD) showed that layer 5 cells had resting potentials that were significantly more depolarized than layer 2/3 cells (EL2/3-EL5 = −13.0 mV, 95% CI [−17.1 −8.9]), layer 4 cells (EL4-EL5 = −4.9 mV, 95% CI [−9.4 −0.44]), and layer 6 cells (EL6-EL5 = −7.1 mV, 95% CI [−12.1 −2.2]), similar to previous studies (Huggenberger et al., 2009; Constantinople and Bruno, 2013). In addition, spike thresholds did not vary by layer [One-Way ANOVA, F(3, 93) = 0.31, p = 0.82], such that depolarized cells were closer to threshold.
Together, the data from our intracellular, on-cell and multiunit recordings provide compelling evidence that most spiking in auditory cortex in response to thalamic stimulation occurs during cortical network activity. Furthermore, this spiking does not appear to follow the spatiotemporal pattern predicted by the canonical microcircuit model. However, each of these electrophysiological techniques has associated limitations, and we used calcium imaging to corroborate these findings and explore the properties of these network events and the cells participating in them further. In particular, the calcium imaging technique allowed us to explore the spatial pattern of spiking probability as a function of cortical layer by simultaneously measuring spiking responses from large numbers of identified cells in layers 2–6.
Calcium imaging technique
The calcium imaging technique employed here involves labeling a vertical strip of cortex with several injections of a membrane-permeable fluorescent calcium-sensing dye (OGB-1 AM; Figure 7A) and imaging at low enough power (10X) to capture most of the cortical depth (~80%) in a single field of view (Figures 7B–D). We did not image the most superficial 100 μm corresponding to layer 1, or the deepest 100–200 μm corresponding to deep layer 6. Consistent with these slices dwelling primarily in DOWN states, spontaneous spiking activity in cortical cells was rare (not shown). By contrast, afferent stimulation consistently elicited calcium transients in a subset of imaged cells (Figures 7B,C) presumably due to synaptically driven action potentials (Banks et al., 2011) (see below). The laminar profile of spiking activity was obtained by determining the position relative to the pia and white matter of each active cell (Figure 7D). Based on our electrophysiological recordings, we expected that most of these spikes occurred in the context of UP states, but we could not determine this directly because the time resolution of the calcium imaging (at least 30 ms due to the video frame rate and slower still due to the kinetics of OGB-1) is too slow to distinguish monosynaptically-driven spikes from those occurring during UP states. Instead, we recorded extracellularly in layer 5 to determine whether an UP state occurred on each trial, and used modified ACSF to reduce the likelihood of observing UP states on some trials (see below).
Cells in infragranular layers project to thalamus, and it is possible that electrical stimulation in these slices activated these cells antidromically and triggered the robust spiking activity observed in these layers. Antidromic activation is of particular concern because of previous reports in vivo that, at least under anesthesia, cortical network activity can be induced by brief activation of small numbers of layer 5 pyramidal cells (Stroh et al., 2013), or prolonged (3 min) activation of even single layer 2/3 pyramidal cells (Li et al., 2009). We investigated the incidence of antidromic activation in response to TC stimulation using the ionotropic glutamate receptor antagonists CNQX (10 μ M) and APV (40 μ M) to block excitatory synaptic transmission. Cells that continued spiking in the presence of CNQX/APV were assumed to be antidromically driven. In 8 experiments, only 3 out of 566 cells (0.5%) of cells that responded to TC stimulation spiked in the presence of CNQX/APV, and all were found in layer 6 (Figures 7E,F). We also did not encounter antidromically driven cells in any of the patch clamp experiments described above. The low percentage of cells insensitive to CNQX/APV suggests that antidromic activation, and network activity secondary to such activation, was minimal in our experiments.
Although ideally we sought to achieve uniform labeling across the ~800 μm of cortical depth that we targeted for imaging (i.e., layer 2—superficial layer 6) there was some variability in background labeling density, as measured by raw fluorescence as a function of cortical depth (Figure 7G). However, this variability in background did not have a strong effect on cell detection (Figure 7H). We compared background and cell count as a function of cortical depth by normalizing background and cell counts within experiments by their respective maxima and pooling these data across experiments. There was no significant correlation between background and cell count when layers 2/3, 4, and 5 were considered (Figure 7H; slope = 0.095, r2 = 0.04, p = 0.086). The correlation was significant if layer 6 was included, however the slope was small (slope = 0.22, r2 = 0.11, p = 0.0018) and therefore had minimal impact on cell counts; however we focused our analyses on layers 2/3 through 5.
Next, we verified that (a) we could measure the calcium transient due to single action potentials when averaging over a few trials, and (b) that the amplitude of the change in fluorescence is linear with the number of action potentials, by simultaneously recording optically and electrically from the same neurons. First, we delivered stimuli directly to cortex in the vicinity of an OGB-1 AM labeled area to evoke robust responses. Next, we obtained an on-cell recording from a randomly selected responding cell in layers 2–5 and simultaneously recorded fluorescence images at 10X and electrophysiological spiking responses to single pulses and brief trains of stimulation. Fluorescence traces on each trial were sorted according to the number of electrically identified spikes on that trial (Figure 8A). Plots of peak ΔF/F vs. number of spikes tended to be linear in individual cells but saturated in some cells at higher numbers of spikes (Figure 8B). After normalizing ΔF/F to the fewest spikes fired by a given cell and averaging these normalized values across cells, we find that the calcium-induced change in fluorescence in the population is linear with the number of action potentials fired up to about six spikes (Figure 8C). Cells that fired more than six spikes in response to a train of four stimuli varied in whether they continued to show near-linear increases in fluorescence with increases in spike count or whether they reached a plateau. Electrical stimuli delivered during the calibration experiments were strong, as they were delivered directly to cortex near the recording site, and we very rarely observed cells that fired more than six spikes in response to TC stimulation in patch clamp recordings.
Above, we presented evidence that cells in auditory cortex fire sparsely in response to stimulation of TC afferents, and thus most cells are unlikely to fire in response to every stimulus pulse in a train (Figures 5, 6A). To estimate our ability to detect cells that spike with a probability less than one, we simulated fluorescence data based on the calibration results (Figure 8D). Each simulation consisted of a mixture of n trials containing a simulated response (with a peak equal to the peak of the average one-spike calcium trace for that cell) embedded in Gaussian noise (generated with variance equal to the prestimulus variance for that cell), and 9 − n trials of Gaussian noise only, with n varied from 0 to 9 (Figure 8E). For each cell, this process was repeated 1000 times for each value of n. Using this procedure we detected a response in about 95% of the simulations that had a one-spike response on every trial of nine, and about 60–70% of simulations that had a one-spike response on four or five trials out of nine (Figure 8F). These experiments indicate that we can identify cells that spike consistently with a very high probability as well as a majority of those that fire with probability ~0.5 in response to single stimulation pulses. Note that in all of the calcium imaging experiments below, each trial consisted of trains of 4 pulses at 40 Hz. Thus, in these experiments, for a cell to fall below our detection limit 50% of the time, it would have to spike in response to less than 1 of 8 afferent stimulation pulses.
Spatial pattern of spiking activity in response to TC stimulation
We investigated the laminar spiking profile in response to activation of thalamic afferents using the calcium imaging technique. In a subset of experiments (n = 5 slices), we simultaneously monitored spiking activity by measuring MUA with multichannel electrodes (an example experiment shown in Figure 9). In this way, we could compare laminar spiking profiles obtained using the two techniques, both in the absence and presence of evoked UP states, and we could determine the contribution to the calcium responses of early spiking cells. At low stimulus intensities (Figure 9, right column), which either did not evoke UP states or evoked UP states with very long latency, spiking was limited to a small number of trials and a handful of cells located primarily in layers 4 and 5. Modest increases in stimulus intensity elicited UP states and dramatic changes in this spiking profile, with dense spiking observed throughout layers 2–6 (Figure 9, Left three columns). Consistent with the summary data shown in Figure 6D above, the MUA recordings in this example (Figure 9B) showed that the UP state spiking occurred earliest in layer 5 before spreading to other layers, with supragranular layers typically spiking at longest latency. Interestingly, in 4 of 5 slices, the threshold for early spiking appeared to be higher than the threshold for UP states, and short latency, well-timed spikes occurred only at the highest intensities and usually in layers 4 and 5 (Figure 9B). We also note that as stimulus intensity was increased, the spiking profile become relatively invariant, suggesting that above a certain threshold UP state activity saturated. In all cases, there was good correspondence between the laminar spiking profiles obtained using the imaging and electrophysiological techniques (Figure 9).
We summarize the results of 17 calcium imaging experiments (including the 5 in which multichannel MUA was also recorded) in Figure 10. TC stimulation evoked spiking activity in layers 2 through 6, with peak density in layer 5 (Figure 10A, solid black line). We compared directly the observed pattern of spiking responses observed via calcium imaging to the pattern we observed in on-cell recordings, presented above in Figure 6 (see Materials and Methods). This spiking probability was strikingly similar to the cell count profile obtained using calcium imaging after normalizing to the cell count in layer 5 (Figure 10A, gray dotted lines). We performed a statistical analysis of cell counts from the calcium imaging data by fitting a generalized estimated equations (GEE) model using a Poisson distribution with logarithmic link function (see Materials and Methods). After normalizing for laminar thickness, layer 5 had a significantly greater density of responding cells than layers 2/3, 4, and 6 (exp(βL2/3-βL5) = 0.531, 95% CI [0.345 0.818], p = 0.004; exp(βL4-βL5) = 0.606, 95% CI [0.426 0.862], p = 0.005; exp(βL6-βL5) = 0.411, 95% CI [0.314 0.536], p < 0.000001). Other pairwise layer comparisons in control conditions were not significant.
We observed that robust activation of layer 5 was consistent across experiments, whereas activation in the granular and supragranular layers was more variable or perhaps bimodal (Figure 10A). In several experiments, activity was almost entirely absent in L2/3 during UP states recorded in layer 5. To ensure that layer 2/3 cells were healthy (able to spike in response to stimulation) in an experiment where spiking was almost entirely confined to layer 5 (Figure 10B), we also delivered electrical stimuli to cortex in layer 2/3 ~650 um from the recording site (Figure 10C). This stimulation triggered large UP states that included both layers 2/3 and 5, in contrast to thalamic stimulation. These data suggest that the involvement of supragranular layers in UP states was variable, but not because of the health of the tissue or inadequate labeling. We will return to this issue below.
In those experiments in which we wished to isolate early spike responses from spiking associated with UP states, we suppressed polysynaptic activity and blocked UP states using either high divalent cations ([Ca2+] = 4.0 mM, [Mg2+] = 4.2 mM; n = 13 experiments), which raise spike thresholds and preferentially block polysynaptic activity, or the NMDA receptor antagonist APV (40 μM) (Berry and Pentreath, 1976; Metherate and Cruikshank, 1999; Cruikshank et al., 2002; Rose and Metherate, 2005) (Figure 10D). These procedures left monosynaptic TC synaptic responses relatively unaffected (Figure 10D, inset). We note that a limitation of this method is that the modified ACSF will also raise spike thresholds in cortical cells and thus decrease even monosynaptic spike probability, and that this effect may be cell type-specific. If this is the case then some cell types recorded in modified ACSF may be overrepresented in our sample. However, 290/312 cells observed spiking in the absence of UP states in modified ACSF also spiked in control conditions, indicating that cells observed in modified ACSF were rarely made more likely to fire by the modified ACSF, and thus nearly all these cells participated in early spiking activity under control conditions as well. For our experiments it was important to use methods that selectively block evoked UP state activity rather than silencing the cortex, e.g., with muscimol, because we are looking for superthreshold responses that would be blocked by cortical silencing. Below, first we characterize how modified ACSF altered the laminar spiking profile, and then describe the properties of cells targeted for patch clamping under modified vs. control ACSF.
TC stimulation in ACSF modified by high divalent cations or APV resulted in less spiking activity than control conditions (exp(βhDiv/APV) = 0.102, 95% CI [0.056 0.185], p < 0.000001), as expected due to the suppression of UP states (Figure 10D, gray line). Although modified ACSF resulted in fewer responding cells in every layer, this effect was not uniform across layers, as is clear in an example experiment showing responding cells in control (Figure 10E) and modified (high divalent cations, Figure 10F) ACSF. The interaction between layer and modified ACSF was significant [Wald χ2(3) = 33.017, p = 0.00000032]. Pairwise interaction comparisons were significant between layers 2/3 and 4 (exp(βL2/3*ACSF−βL4*ACSF) = 0.082, 95% CI [0.026 0.256], p = 0.000018), layers 2/3 and 5 (exp(βL2/3*ACSF−βL5*ACSF) = 0.048, 95% CI [0.015 0.158], p = 0.0000005), and between layers 2/3 and 6 (exp(βL2/3*ACSF−βL6*ACSF) = 0.053, 95% CI [0.014 0.203], p = 0.000018). These interactions indicate that layer 2/3 is more affected by suppression of UP states than are the other layers. Other pairwise comparisons were not significant, but layer 5 was the least affected by suppression of UP states. If layer 5 is primarily driven by layer 2/3, as suggested by the canonical microcircuit, blocking polysynaptic activity would be expected to affect layer 5 more strongly than layer 2/3. These results instead suggest that layer 5 is strongly activated by TC input, independently of the activity of the supragranular and granular layers, as recently shown in vivo (Constantinople and Bruno, 2013).
To confirm that cells spiking in modified ACSF were in fact being driven by monosynaptic TC EPSPs, and to identify these early spiking cells' morphology and electrophysiology, we identified specific cells spiking in response to TC stimulation via their fluorescence signals and targeted them for patch clamp recording and subsequent classification. The protocol for these targeted patch experiments was to bathe the slice in modified ACSF, verify the absence of UP states, identify a cell still spiking in the absence of UP states, and obtain a whole-cell recording from that cell. All six cells in layer 5 that were identified via their fluorescence signals as spiking in modified ACSF in the absence of UP states had spikes that were monosynaptically driven by TC stimulation. Three of these cells spiked in response to a single TC stimulus, while the other three spiked in response to stimuli two through four of a 40 Hz train of TC stimuli. Interestingly, none of these targeted, early spiking cells were pyramidal cells. Five cells had morphology and spiking patterns consistent with Martinotti interneurons (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1996, 1997, 1998; Markram et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2004). The sixth cell had a non-pyramidal, non-spiny, bipolar morphology and a stuttering spiking pattern.
By contrast, we also targeted 7 cells identified via their fluorescence signals as spiking in normal ACSF under control conditions, i.e., in the presence of UP states. All 7 cells were identified as pyramidal cells based on their morphology and firing properties (either regular spiking or burst firing). Only one of these cells fired short latency, well-timed spikes prior to the onset of UP states; the other six cells fired spikes only during UP states. Including cells targeted in modified and normal ACSF and our random whole-cell recordings presented above (Figure 3D), 9 out of 11 cells that we observed spiking in direct response to monosynaptic EPSPs, outside the context of UP states, were putative interneurons, and 7 of these cells plus two pyramidal cells were located in layer 5. This result is even more striking given the low percentage of cells in neocortex that are GABAergic interneurons (~20%; Markram et al., 2004; Ascoli et al., 2008). The cells targeted in the absence of UP states were significantly more likely to be non-pyramidal cells than those targeted in the presence of UP states (Fisher's exact test, p = 0.0047), indicating that there was not simply a greater likelihood of choosing non-pyramidal cells via targeted patching.
Variable activation of layer 2/3 during UP states
The data of Figure 10 suggest that across experiments, the involvement of supragranular layers in UP states is more variable than that of infragranular layers: whereas layer 5 participated strongly in all experiments, layer 2/3 involvement was less consistent. This variability was also manifested on a trial-by-trial basis, even when the stimulus intensity was the same on each trial, both in extracellular electrophysiology (Figure 11) and calcium imaging (Figure 12) experiments. In extracellular recordings, we observed two types of UP states in CSDs that differed in their intensity, duration and in their involvement of supragranular layers (Figure 11A). In the example illustrated, TC responses in granular layers to a train of four stimuli on trials 1, 3, 4, 6, 7, and 9 first spread to layer 5 and then to supragranular layers as well as layer 6 over the next 200 ms (Figure 1A). By contrast, activity spread to layer 5 and triggered a modest current sink in layer 2 before dying out on trials 2, 5, 8, and 10. These visually distinct responses could also be separated on the basis of the total CSD power during the response window 0 < t < 300 ms (Figure 11B). Averaged CSDs of these two types of responses (Figures 11C,D, top) showed far larger sinks in supragranular layers for the larger UP states than for the smaller, and simultaneously measured MUA (Figures 11B,C, bottom) showed little involvement of layer 2/3 in superthreshold activity on trials with smaller UP states but robust activation on trials with larger UP states. In other CSD experiments (not shown), small increases in stimulus intensity biased the network toward the larger UP states, indicating that involvement of all layers could depend on stimulation being of sufficient strength.
Distinct UP states sizes and associated laminar response patterns were also observed in calcium imaging experiments (Figure 12). In the example illustrated, trials were sorted according to two clearly distinguishable response classes in the local field potential recorded in layer 5 (Figures 12A,B). Calcium traces in individual cells were larger and more cells were active, especially in the superficial layers, on the trials with a “larger” UP state (Figures 6C,D).
Discussion
Thalamically-evoked activity patterns in auditory cortex
We distinguished between two types of spiking responses to thalamic stimulation. In murine auditory cortex, most cells that spike do so during brief network bursts that occurred nearly simultaneously in cells throughout the cortical region; these cells depolarize in response to a barrage of polysynaptic activity and fire occasional spikes during these events. By contrast, only a small number of cells fire action potentials triggered at short latency by monosynaptic TC EPSPs (Figure 3D), even though short latency, monosynaptic, subthreshold synaptic responses are observed in cells throughout layers 2–6. We summarize our results in the circuit diagrams of Figure 13. In this model, monosynaptically driven spiking occurs predominantly in (putative) GABAergic interneurons, including Martinotti and fast spiking cells (Figure 4), as well as an occasional pyramidal cell. Stimuli of sufficient intensity elicit UP states involving robust spiking activity (Figures 13B,C). UP state activity occurs primarily in pyramidal cells, and appears with earliest latency in pyramidal cells in layer 5, consistent with previous reports (Chauvette et al., 2010; Wester and Contreras, 2012; Beltramo et al., 2013). In some cases, UP state activity is largely confined to infragranular layers (Figure 13B), where dense interconnectivity between pyramidal cells and depolarized resting potentials allow for sustained network events lasting ~100 ms. Functionally, ascending information during these smaller UP states flows back to subcortical structures and may engage cortico-TC loops, but does not engage direct cortico-cortical hierarchical processing. In other cases, likely only in response to stimuli of sufficient intensity or simultaneous ascending and descending input, UP state activity spreads throughout the column and involves supragranular layers as well (Figure 13C), potentially activating direct cortico-cortical pathways.
Dense spiking in infragranular layers
We observed that short latency and robust spiking in response to thalamic stimulation occurred in layer 5 cells during evoked UP states (Figures 6, 10D), and that even in the absence of UP states layer 5 cells could still dominate the spiking responses to TC stimulation. This stands in contrast to the predictions of the canonical microcircuit model, in which the shortest latencies to thalamic stimulation occur in layer 4 spiny stellate cells. Indeed, we would have expected that in the absence of polysynaptic activity (i.e., at low stimulus intensities or in modified ACSF), spiking would have been dominated by cells in layer 4, rather than cells in layer 5, as we observed (Figures 9, 10). However, spiny stellate cells are rare in auditory cortex (Smith and Populin, 2001; Barbour and Callaway, 2008; Sakata and Harris, 2009), and here and elsewhere in cortex pyramidal cells in layers 2–6 with dendrites in thalamo-recipient layers are likely excited directly (Figures 3A–C). These results suggest that layer 5 cells get more direct thalamic input than is appreciated based on afferent terminal density (Gil and Amitai, 1996; Bureau et al., 2006; De Kock et al., 2007). Additionally, cells in layer 5 have more depolarized resting potentials and lower spike thresholds than cells in other layers (see Results) similar to previous results in vitro (Huggenberger et al., 2009) and in vivo (Manns et al., 2004; Constantinople and Bruno, 2013). A recent study in somatosensory cortex demonstrated direct and early activation of layer 5 cells, independent of granular layer activation (Constantinople and Bruno, 2013). Consistent with these observations, thick-tufted pyramidal cells in vivo are the earliest and most likely to be activated by whisker deflection in barrel cortex (De Kock et al., 2007) and most likely to be activated by auditory tones or clicks in auditory cortex (Sakata and Harris, 2009). The purpose of a highly excitable recurrent layer 5 network may be to trigger arousal in response to strong stimuli (Harris and Thiele, 2011), or perhaps to evoke “packets” of information-carrying activity (Luczak et al., 2013), which have a similar duration to the evoked UP states we observed. If layer 5 cells are both highly responsive to input and directly activated by thalamus, connections from layer 5 to other layers (particularly supragranular layers) may contribute a greater fraction of synaptic current than the number of collateral fibers or response to focal stimulation would suggest. Thus, activation of layer 5 cells by TC input may provide a pathway through which information can ascend the cortical hierarchy, either through UP states and subsequent activation of supragranular cells, or through a cortico-TC circuit (Guillery and Sherman, 2002; Theyel et al., 2010) without active processing in supragranular layers (Constantinople and Bruno, 2013).
Monosynaptic TC activation of non-pyramidal cells
We observed that cells that spiked at short latency to TC stimulation, in the absence of UP states, were most often non-pyramidal, and had firing patterns and morphology consistent with either fast spiking parvalbumin-positive cells (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1993) (in layers 4 and 5) or low-threshold/burst-spiking somatostatin-positive Martinotti cells (in layer 5) (Kawaguchi, 1993; Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Wang et al., 2004). The spikes from these latter cells sometimes occurred with a considerable delay after the stimulus (up to 15 ms) and appeared to result from a slow, intrinsic conductance triggered by the initial depolarization similar to that seen with just-threshold current injection.
Preferential activation of fast spiking interneurons by TC afferents has been described previously in somatosensory (Beierlein et al., 2003; Gabernet et al., 2005; Cruikshank et al., 2007) and auditory cortex (Schiff and Reyes, 2012), where these cells are postulated to mediate feedforward inhibition that restricts integration windows and sharpens spike timing. Evidence for short-latency activation of infragranular Martinotti cells is mixed. Several studies have found that these cells, whose axons synapse on the dendrites and apical tufts of pyramidal cells in layers 1, 4, and 5 (Markram et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2004), are only weakly excited by TC afferents compared to fast-spiking interneurons (Gibson et al., 1999; Verbny et al., 2006; Cruikshank et al., 2010). In contrast, others have shown that somatostatin-positive Martinotti cells in layers 2/3 are closer to threshold than regular- and fast-spiking cells at rest (Fanselow et al., 2008) and that layer 5 somatostatin-positive interneurons can mediate feed-forward inhibition (Tan et al., 2008); however, the population sampled in the latter work likely did not include the Martinotti cells specifically (see Ma et al., 2006). Our results support the notion that both fast spiking and Martinotti cells are directly involved in, and may even dominate, the monosynaptic cortical response to thalamic input. Because many of the early-activated interneurons are located in layer 5, where evoked UP states are likely to originate (Sanchez-Vives and McCormick, 2000; McCormick et al., 2003; Sakata and Harris, 2009; Chauvette et al., 2010; Wester and Contreras, 2012; Beltramo et al., 2013), these cells may be involved in regulating the onset of evoked UP states.
Although we did not systematically test for the presence of disynaptic (i.e., feedforward) inhibition in every pyramidal cell recorded, of the 33 cells in which we did investigate this we observed short latency inhibitory responses in only 8 cells. This observation is consistent with previous reports in auditory cortex (Hefti and Smith, 2003; Rose and Metherate, 2005; Verbny et al., 2006). A heavy contribution by Martinotti cells to feedforward inhibition in auditory cortex could explain why disynaptic inhibition is not observed more commonly. Because these cells are dendrite-targeting interneurons (Kawaguchi and Kubota, 1997; Markram et al., 2004), these electrotonically-remote inhibitory conductances are unlikely to be well-observed at the soma. This may explain the low percentage of cells with observed disynaptic inhibition relative to the likelihood of putative interneurons to spike in our recordings.
Variable activation of supragranular layers during evoked UP states
Whereas cells in layer 5 were consistently activated during UP states, activation of cells in layer 2/3 was more variable (Figures 10–12). The paucity of spiking observed in supragranular layers is consistent with previous reports of sparse spiking in layer 2/3 (Barth and Poulet, 2012), and may be a feature of how these cells encode sensory information. It is also possible that once the supragranular network is activated, spiking in layer 2/3 cells is as dense as in layer 5. UP states that involved layer 2/3 were more intense than those that were confined to infragranular layers, and spiking during these larger UP states in supragranular layers was as dense as in infragranular layers (Figures 11, 12). In some experiments, we observed that the size of the UP state and the involvement of layer 2/3 depended on stimulus intensity (not shown), suggesting a model in which the cortical microcircuit gates ascending sensory input: only selected inputs activate layer 2/3 and incorporate into direct cortico-cortical hierarchical processing, whereas “packets” (Luczak et al., 2013) of spikes occur readily in infragranular layers to provide rapid motor output, and possibly activate cortico-TC loops. This differential involvement of supragranular layers has been observed before in auditory cortex in vivo, where spontaneous UP states were shown to only on occasion spread from infra- to supragranular layers (Sakata and Harris, 2009). Probabilistic involvement of layer 2/3 in these network events is consistent with recent observations that layer 2/3 activity occurs as population bursts whose probability depends on stimulus parameters in a non-linear fashion (Bathellier et al., 2012).
The observation of differential involvement of layers 5 and layer 2/3 in UP states adds to a growing body of evidence that activity in infra- and supragranular layers can be uncoupled and may be linked to different cortical processes. For example, activity in infra- and supragranular layers exhibit different spectral characteristics (Maier et al., 2010), and activity in supragranular layers has been more closely linked to attention and perceptual awareness (He and Raichle, 2009; Buffalo et al., 2011). “Avalanches” in cortical networks have also been observed exclusively in supragranular and not infragranular networks (Beggs and Plenz, 2003; Gireesh and Plenz, 2008; Petermann et al., 2009). These observations are also clearly incompatible with the prediction of the canonical microcircuit that activity in infragranular layers is driven by activity in supragranular layers, and support instead a model of two parallel, interacting networks, as proposed in the original formulation of the model (Douglas et al., 1989; Douglas and Martin, 1991).
Concurrence of electrophysiological and calcium imaging data
Although our calcium imaging technique is less sensitive than those employing two-photon microscopy, our simultaneous on-cell/calcium imaging calibration experiments (Figure 8) demonstrate that we are able to detect single spikes in cells from all layers. Our main findings using calcium imaging, i.e., that spiking is densest in layer 5 following TC stimulation, that early spiking cells tend to cluster in layers 4 and 5 and are mostly non-pyramidal, and that the participation of layer 2/3 in UP states is variable, were all corroborated with parallel electrophysiological recordings. In addition, we reported calcium responses as the number of responding cells rather than the amplitude of the response in part to avoid bias toward cell populations that are larger, fire bursts of spikes or have lower concentrations of endogenous calcium binding proteins. Thus, the laminar profile of spiking probability following TC stimulation obtained with calcium imaging was nearly identical to that obtained with random on-cell patch experiments (Figure 10A). In addition, the cells recorded using targeted patch experiments in modified ACSF were mostly interneurons, similar to the cells recorded using random whole-cell patch recordings (Figure 4). We note that interneurons such as those targeted are smaller than pyramidal cells and likely have high concentrations of calcium buffers (Lee et al., 2000; Aponte et al., 2008), which if anything should make them harder to detect in calcium imaging experiments than pyramidal cells. Although these cells do fire bursts of action potentials, which could make them easier to detect despite their small size, the cells targeted in modified ACSF were not exclusively burst-firing cells, and our calibration experiments showed that cells that fired one spike per trial were easily detected (Figure 8).We also noted (Figure 8C) that there is a reduced probability of detecting cells that spike less than once per trial (i.e., <1 spike per four afferent stimulus pulses). Because layer 2/3 cells are reported to fire more sparsely in vivo than layer 5 cells (Sakata and Harris, 2009, 2012), we were concerned that these detection limits in our calcium imaging technique may underestimate the participation of L2/3 cells in activity patterns generated by TC stimulation. However, spiking probability in L2/3 estimated via on-cell recordings was consistent with that observed during calcium imaging, though cells in supragranular layers were still observed to fire more sparsely, and at longer latency, than infragranular or granular cells. The variable involvement of these layer 2/3 cells in evoked UP states observed in calcium imaging experiments was also observed in multichannel electrode recordings (Figures 11C,D).
Functional implications
The data and model presented here present have far-reaching implications for understanding the state-dependence of information processing in auditory cortex. In particular, the observation that supragranular layers may only participate in UP state activity under certain conditions may be relevant to top-down, state-dependent modulation of cortical sensory responses. The low efficacy with which thalamic afferents drive monosynaptic spiking activity in cortex has been noted previously (MacLean et al., 2005), and recent reports emphasize the importance of cortical network activity in responses to sensory stimuli in vivo (Bathellier et al., 2012; Hromadka et al., 2013; Luczak et al., 2013). It is clear that extrapolating from the observations presented here to functional interpretations in vivo is challenging due to the limitations of the brain slice preparation. In our experiments, the absence of intact cortical networks and the resulting relative quiescence network state at rest, the limited influence of subcortical neuromodulators, and the substitution of synchronous fiber bundle stimulation for sensory input all constrain the range of possible cellular and network responses to a subset of those available in vivo. In spite of these limitations, however, there are striking similarities between the responses observed here and those observed in vivo. For example, UP states in our slices had durations that ranged from ~50 to 200 ms, similar to those reported in vivo for UP states (Sakata and Harris, 2009) and “bumps” (Hromadka et al., 2013) after accounting for temperature differences between preparations. The variable involvement of supragranular layers in UP states that we observed is similar to that reported for spontaneous UP states in vivo (Sakata and Harris, 2009), as is the observation of early activation of layer 5 cells (Constantinople and Bruno, 2013). Thus, we suggest that the network activity elicited by repetitive stimulation of TC afferents under control conditions in our slices is similar to the UP state-associated activity elicited by sensory stimulation in vivo (Sakata and Harris, 2009; Luczak et al., 2013). However, in the intact cortico-thalamic network, other afferent inputs and neuromodulators likely bias the network state toward or away from UP state activity and may particularly influence the involvement of the superficial layers.
The stochastic nature of UP states evoked by TC stimulation, for example the variability of their onset latency and duration on successive trials (Figure 5), as well as their characterization as intracortically-mediated events, raise the question as to whether spikes occurring during UP states are time-locked to the TC stimuli and can carry information about specific temporal features of the TC input train. The observation that most spiking occurs during these intracortically-mediated events is particularly surprising in auditory cortex, given the importance of spike timing in the ascending auditory pathway for sensory coding. Although we observed some evidence for consistent spike timing across trials in the context of UP states [e.g., in the raster plot for the deepest layer 5 cell (Figure 5B), in which spikes occurring between t = 25 and 50 ms on each trial tend to line up vertically, indicating consistent timing on successive trials], in other cells this was not the case. Further experiments aimed specifically at information coding will be better able to quantify spike jitter during UP states and the implications for encoding and decoding.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Supported by National Institutes of Health (R01 DC006013 to Matthew I. Banks and T32 GM007507 to Bryan M. Krause), the Department of Anesthesiology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, and the University of Wisconsin - Madison Office of the Vice Chancellor for Research and Graduate Education, with funding from the Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation. The authors thank Sean Grady (Department of Anesthesiology) and Anna Kowalkowski (Department of Neuroscience) for technical support on this project.
References
- Adams J. C. (1981). Heavy metal intensification of DAB-based HRP reaction product. J. Histochem. Cytochem. 29, 775 10.1177/29.6.7252134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aponte Y., Bischofberger J., Jonas P. (2008). Efficient Ca2+ buffering in fast-spiking basket cells of rat hippocampus. J. Physiol. 86, 2061–2075 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.147298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ascoli G. A., Alonso-Nanclares L., Anderson S. A., Barrionuevo G., Benavides-Piccione R., Burkhalter A., et al. (2008). Petilla terminology: nomenclature of features of GABAergic interneurons of the cerebral cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 557–568 10.1038/nrn2402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atencio C. A., Sharpee T. O., Schreiner C. E. (2009). Hierarchical computation in the canonical auditory cortical circuit. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 21894–21899 10.1073/pnas.0908383106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banks M. I., Uhlrich D. J., Smith P. H., Krause B. M., Manning K. A. (2011). Descending projections from extrastriate visual cortex modulate responses of cells in primary auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2620–2638 10.1093/cercor/bhr048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbour D. L., Callaway E. M. (2008). Excitatory local connections of superficial neurons in rat auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 11174–11185 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2093-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barth A. L., Poulet J. F. (2012). Experimental evidence for sparse firing in the neocortex. Trends Neurosci. 35, 345–355 10.1016/j.tins.2012.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastos A. M., Usrey W. M., Adams R. A., Mangun G. R., Fries P., Friston K. J. (2012). Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron 76, 695–711 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bathellier B., Ushakova L., Rumpel S. (2012). Discrete neocortical dynamics predict behavioral categorization of sounds. Neuron 76, 435–449 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beggs J. M., Plenz D. (2003). Neuronal avalanches in neocortical circuits. J. Neurosci. 23, 11167–11177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beierlein M., Gibson J. R., Connors B. W. (2003). Two dynamically distinct inhibitory networks in layer 4 of the neocortex. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 2987–3000 10.1152/jn.00283.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beltramo R., D'urso G., Dal Maschio M., Farisello P., Bovetti S., Clovis Y., et al. (2013). Layer-specific excitatory circuits differentially control recurrent network dynamics in the neocortex. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 227–234 10.1038/nn.3306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benshalom G., White E. L. (1986). Quantification of thalamocortical synapses with spiny stellate neurons in layer IV of mouse somatosensory cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 253, 303–314 10.1002/cne.902530303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry M. S., Pentreath V. W. (1976). Criteria for distinguishing between monosynaptic and polysynaptic transmission. Brain Res. 105, 1–20 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90919-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binzegger T., Douglas R. J., Martin K. A. (2004). A quantitative map of the circuit of cat primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 8441–8453 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1400-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Budd J. M. (1998). Extrastriate feedback to primary visual cortex in primates: a quantitative analysis of connectivity. Proc. Biol. Sci. 265, 1037–1044 10.1098/rspb.1998.0396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffalo E. A., Fries P., Landman R., Buschman T. J., Desimone R. (2011). Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 11262–11267 10.1073/pnas.1011284108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullier J., Henry G. H. (1979). Laminar distribution of first-order neurons and afferent terminals in cat striate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 42, 1271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bureau I., Saint Paul F., Svoboda K. (2006). Interdigitated paralemniscal and lemniscal pathways in the mouse barrel cortex. PLoS Biol. 4:e382 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040382 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang E. F., Bao S., Imaizumi K., Schreiner C. E., Merzenich M. M. (2005). Development of spectral and temporal response selectivity in the auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 16460–16465 10.1073/pnas.0508239102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang E. F., Merzenich M. M. (2003). Environmental noise retards auditory cortical development. Science 300, 498–502 10.1126/science.1082163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chauvette S., Volgushev M., Timofeev I. (2010). Origin of active states in local neocortical networks during slow sleep oscillation. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2660–2674 10.1093/cercor/bhq009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christianson G. B., Sahani M., Linden J. F. (2011). Depth-dependent temporal response properties in core auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 12837–12848 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2863-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantinople C. M., Bruno R. M. (2011). Effects and mechanisms of wakefulness on local cortical networks. Neuron 69, 1061–1068 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantinople C. M., Bruno R. M. (2013). Deep cortical layers are activated directly by thalamus. Science 340, 1591–1594 10.1126/science.1236425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Creutzfeldt O. D. (1977). Generality of the functional structure of the neocortex. Naturwissenschaften 64, 507–517 10.1007/BF00483547 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruikshank S. J., Lewis T. J., Connors B. W. (2007). Synaptic basis for intense thalamocortical activation of feedforward inhibitory cells in neocortex. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 462–468 10.1038/nn1861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruikshank S. J., Rose H. J., Metherate R. (2002). Auditory thalamocortical synaptic transmission in vitro. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 361–384 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruikshank S. J., Urabe H., Nurmikko A. V., Connors B. W. (2010). Pathway-specific feedforward circuits between thalamus and neocortex revealed by selective optical stimulation of axons. Neuron 65, 230–245 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham M. O., Pervouchine D. D., Racca C., Kopell N. J., Davies C. H., Jones R. S., et al. (2006). Neuronal metabolism governs cortical network response state. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 5597–5601 10.1073/pnas.0600604103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curto C., Sakata S., Marguet S., Itskov V., Harris K. D. (2009). A simple model of cortical dynamics explains variability and state dependence of sensory responses in urethane-anesthetized auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 29, 10600–10612 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2053-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. H., Johnsrude I. S. (2007). Hearing speech sounds: top-down influences on the interface between audition and speech perception. Hear. Res. 229, 132–147 10.1016/j.heares.2007.01.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Kock C. P., Bruno R. M., Spors H., Sakmann B. (2007). Layer- and cell-type-specific suprathreshold stimulus representation in rat primary somatosensory cortex. J. Physiol. 581, 139–154 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.124321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Destexhe A., Hughes S. W., Rudolph M., Crunelli V. (2007). Are corticothalamic “up” states fragments of wakefulness? Trends Neurosci. 30, 334–342 10.1016/j.tins.2007.04.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Destexhe A., Rudolph M., Pare D. (2003). The high-conductance state of neocortical neurons in vivo. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 739–751 10.1038/nrn1198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deweese M. R., Zador A. M. (2006). Non-gaussian membrane potential dynamics imply sparse, synchronous activity in auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 26, 12206–12218 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2813-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas R. J., Martin K. A. (1991). A functional microcircuit for cat visual cortex. J. Physiol. 440, 735–769 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas R. J., Martin K. A. (2004). Neuronal circuits of the neocortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 419–451 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas R. J., Martin K. A. C., Whitteridge D. (1989). A canonical microcircuit for neocortex. Neural Comput. 1, 480–488 10.1162/neco.1989.1.4.480 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dräger U. C., Hubel D. H. (1978). Studies of visual function and its decay in mice with hereditary retinal degeneration. J. Comp. Neurol. 180, 85–114 10.1002/cne.901800107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow E. E., Richardson K. A., Connors B. W. (2008). Selective, state-dependent activation of somatostatin-expressing inhibitory interneurons in mouse neocortex. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 2640–2652 10.1152/jn.90691.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldmeyer D., Lübke J., Silver R. A., Sakmann B. (2002). Synaptic connections between layer 4 spiny neurone-layer 2/3 pyramidal cell pairs in juvenile rat barrel cortex: physiology and anatomy of interlaminar signalling within a cortical column. J. Physiol. 538, 803–822 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.012959 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman J., Nicholson C. (1975). Experimental optimization of current source-density technique for anuran cerebellum. J. Neurophysiol. 38, 369–382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick A., Feldmeyer D., Sakmann B. (2007). Postnatal development of synaptic transmission in local networks of L5A pyramidal neurons in rat somatosensory cortex. J.Physiol 585, 103–116 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.141788 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz J. B., Elhilali M., Shamma S. A. (2007). Adaptive changes in cortical receptive fields induced by attention to complex sounds. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 2337–2346 10.1152/jn.00552.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabernet L., Jadhav S. P., Feldman D. E., Carandini M., Scanziani M. (2005). Somatosensory integration controlled by dynamic thalamocortical feed-forward inhibition. Neuron 48, 315–327 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerstein G. L., Kiang N. Y. S. (1964). Responses of single units in the auditory cortex. Exp. Neurol. 10, 1–18 10.1016/0014-4886(64)90083-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson J. R., Beierlein M., Connors B. W. (1999). Two networks of electrically coupled inhibitory neurons in neocortex. Nature 402, 75–79 10.1038/47035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gil Z., Amitai Y. (1996). Properties of convergent thalamocortical and intracortical synaptic potentials in single neurons of neocortex. J. Neurosci. 16, 6567–6578 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert C. D. (1983). Microcircuitry of the visual cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 217–247 10.1146/annurev.ne.06.030183.001245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert C. D., Wiesel T. N. (1983). Functional organization of the visual cortex. Prog. Brain Res. 58, 209–218 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60022-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gireesh E. D., Plenz D. (2008). Neuronal avalanches organize as nested theta- and beta/gamma-oscillations during development of cortical layer 2/3. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 7576–7581 10.1073/pnas.0800537105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goard M., Dan Y. (2009). Basal forebrain activation enhances cortical coding of natural scenes. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1444–1449 10.1038/nn.2402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goris R. L., Movshon J. A., Simoncelli E. P. (2014). Partitioning neuronal variability. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 858–865 10.1038/nn.3711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillery R. W., Sherman S. M. (2002). Thalamic relay functions and their role in corticocortical communication: generalizations from the visual system. Neuron 33, 163–175 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00582-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris K. D., Thiele A. (2011). Cortical state and attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 509–523 10.1038/nrn3084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He B. J., Raichle M. E. (2009). The fMRI signal, slow cortical potential and consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 302–309 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hefti B. J., Smith P. H. (2003). Distribution and kinetic properties of GABAergic inputs to layer V pyramidal cells in rat auditory cortex. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 4, 106–121 10.1007/s10162-002-3012-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch J. A., Martinez L. M. (2006). Laminar processing in the visual cortical column. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 377–384 10.1016/j.conb.2006.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hromadka T., Zador A. M., Deweese M. R. (2013). Up states are rare in awake auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 1989–1995 10.1152/jn.00600.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang C. L., Winer J. A. (2000). Auditory thalamocortical projections in the cat: laminar and areal patterns of input. J. Comp. Neurol. 427, 302–331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel D. H., Wiesel T. N. (1963). Shape and arrangement of columns in cat's striate cortex. J. Physiol. 165, 559–568 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huggenberger S., Vater M., Deisz R. A. (2009). Interlaminar differences of intrinsic properties of pyramidal neurons in the auditory cortex of mice. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1008–1018 10.1093/cercor/bhn143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson K. R., Erway L. C., Cook S. A., Willott J. F., Zheng Q. Y. (1997). A major gene affecting age-related hearing loss in C57BL/6J mice. Hear. Res. 114, 83–92 10.1016/S0378-5955(97)00155-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas J. H., Collins C. E. (2001). The organization of sensory cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 498–504 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00240-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kampa B. M., Letzkus J. J., Stuart G. J. (2006). Cortical feed-forward networks for binding different streams of sensory information. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1472–1473 10.1038/nn1798 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi Y. (1993). Groupings of nonpyramidal and pyramidal cells with specific physiological and morphological characteristics in rat frontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 416–431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi Y., Kubota Y. (1993). Correlation of physiological subgroupings of nonpyramidal cells with parvalbumin and calbinding immunoreactive neurons in layer V of rat frontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 387–396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi Y., Kubota Y. (1996). Physiological and morphological identification of somatostatin- or vasoactive intestinal polypeptide-containing cells among GABAergic cell subtypes in rat frontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 16, 2701–2715 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi Y., Kubota Y. (1997). GABAergic cell subtypes and their synaptic connections in rat frontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 7, 476–486 10.1093/cercor/7.6.476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawaguchi Y., Kubota Y. (1998). Neurochemical features and synaptic connections of large physiologically-identified GABAergic cells in the rat frontal cortex. Neuroscience 85, 677–701 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00685-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kisley M. A., Gerstein G. L. (1999). Trial-to-trial variability and state-dependent modulation of auditory- evoked responses in cortex. J. Neurosci. 19, 10451–10460 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakatos P., Shah A. S., Knuth K. H., Ulbert I., Karmos G., Schroeder C. E. (2005). An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1904–1911 10.1152/jn.00263.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee C. C., Sherman S. M. (2008). synaptic properties of thalamic and intracortical inputs to Layer 4 of the First- and Higher-order cortical areas in the auditory and somatosensory systems. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 317–326 10.1152/jn.90391.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S. H., Rosenmund C., Schwaller B., Neher E. (2000). Differences in Ca2+ buffering properties between excitatory and inhibitory hippocampal neurons from the rat. J. Physiol. 525, 405–418 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-3-00405.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li C. Y., Poo M. M., Dan Y. (2009). Burst spiking of a single cortical neuron modifies global brain state. Science 324, 643–646 10.1126/science.1169957 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubke J., Feldmeyer D. (2007). Excitatory signal flow and connectivity in a cortical column: focus on barrel cortex. Brain Struct. Funct. 212, 3–17 10.1007/s00429-007-0144-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lübke J., Roth A., Feldmeyer D., Sakmann B. (2003). Morphometric analysis of the columnar innervation domain of neurons connecting layer 4 and layer 2/3 of juvenile rat barrel cortex. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1051–1063 10.1093/cercor/13.10.1051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luczak A., Bartho P., Harris K. D. (2013). Gating of sensory input by spontaneous cortical activity. J. Neurosci. 33, 1684–1695 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2928-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luczak A., Bartho P., Marguet S. L., Buzsaki G., Harris K. D. (2007). Sequential structure of neocortical spontaneous activity in vivo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 347–352 10.1073/pnas.0605643104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma Y., Hu H., Berrebi A. S., Mathers P. H., Agmon A. (2006). Distinct subtypes of somatostatin-containing neocortical interneurons revealed in transgenic mice. J. Neurosci. 26, 5069–5082 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0661-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacGregor D. G., Chesler M., Rice M. E. (2001). HEPES prevents edema in rat brain slices. Neurosci. Lett. 303, 141–144 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01690-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean J. N., Watson B. O., Aaron G. B., Yuste R. (2005). Internal dynamics determine the cortical response to thalamic stimulation. Neuron 48, 811–823 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier A., Adams G. K., Aura C., Leopold D. A. (2010). Distinct superficial and deep laminar domains of activity in the visual cortex during rest and stimulation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:31 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manns I. D., Sakmann B., Brecht M. (2004). Sub- and suprathreshold receptive field properties of pyramidal neurones in layers 5A and 5B of rat somatosensory barrel cortex. J. Physiol. 556, 601–622 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.053132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markram H., Toledo-Rodriguez M., Wang Y., Gupta A., Silberberg G., Wu C. Z. (2004). Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 793–807 10.1038/nrn1519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell J. H., Gibson J. R. (1992). Visual response latencies in striate cortex of the macaque monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 1332–1344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCormick D. A., Shu Y., Hasenstaub A., Sanchez-Vives M., Badoual M., Bal T. (2003). Persistent cortical activity: mechanisms of generation and effects on neuronal excitability. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1219–1231 10.1093/cercor/bhg104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metherate R., Aramakis V. B. (1999). Intrinsic electrophysiology of neurons in thalamorecipient layers of developing rat auditory cortex. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 115, 131–144 10.1016/S0165-3806(99)00058-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metherate R., Cox C. L., Ashe J. H. (1992). Cellular bases of neocortical activation: modulation of neural oscillations by the nucleus basalis and endogenous acetylcholine. J. Neurosci. 12, 4701–4711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metherate R., Cruikshank S. J. (1999). Thalamocortical inputs trigger a propagating envelope of gamma-band activity in auditory cortex in vitro. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 160–174 10.1007/s002210050726 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitani A., Shimokouchi M. (1985). Neuronal connections in the primary auditory cortex: an electrophysiological study in the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 235, 417–429 10.1002/cne.902350402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitzdorf U. (1985). Current source-density method and application in cat cerebral cortex: investigation of evoked potentials and EEG phenomena. Physiol. Rev. 65, 37–100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mountcastle V. B. (1997). The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain 120(pt 4), 701–722 10.1093/brain/120.4.701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mountcastle V. B., Davies P. W., Berman A. L. (1957). Response properties of neurons of cat's somatic sensory cortex to peripheral stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 20, 374–407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswald A. M., Reyes A. D. (2008). Maturation of intrinsic and synaptic properties of layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons in mouse auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 2998–3008 10.1152/jn.01160.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswald A. M., Reyes A. D. (2011). Development of inhibitory timescales in auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1351–1361 10.1093/cercor/bhq214 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pasley B. N., Allen E. A., Freeman R. D. (2009). State-dependent variability of neuronal responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex. Neuron 62, 291–303 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G., Franklin K. B. J. (2003). The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
- Petermann T., Thiagarajan T. C., Lebedev M. A., Nicolelis M. A., Chialvo D. R., Plenz D. (2009). Spontaneous cortical activity in awake monkeys composed of neuronal avalanches. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 15921–15926 10.1073/pnas.0904089106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters A., Payne B. R. (1993). Numerical relationships between geniculocortical afferents and pyramidal cell modules in cat primary visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex 3, 69–78 10.1093/cercor/3.1.69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen C. C. H., Hahn T. T. G., Mehta M., Grinvald A., Sakmann B. (2003). Interaction of sensory responses with spontaneous depolarization in layer 2/3 barrel cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 13638–13643 10.1073/pnas.2235811100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettersen K. H., Devor A., Ulbert I., Dale A. M., Einevoll G. T. (2006). Current-source density estimation based on inversion of electrostatic forward solution: effects of finite extent of neuronal activity and conductivity discontinuities. J. Neurosci. Methods 154, 116–133 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed J. L., Kaas J. H. (2010). Statistical analysis of large-scale neuronal recording data. Neural Netw. 23, 673–684 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.04.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rigas P., Castro-Alamancos M. A. (2007). Thalamocortical Up states: differential effects of intrinsic and extrinsic cortical inputs on persistent activity. J. Neurosci. 27, 4261–4272 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0003-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rigas P., Castro-Alamancos M. A. (2009). Impact of persistent cortical activity (up states) on intracortical and thalamocortical synaptic inputs. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 119–131 10.1152/jn.00126.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romand S., Wang Y., Toledo-Rodriguez M., Markram H. (2011). Morphological development of thick-tufted layer v pyramidal cells in the rat somatosensory cortex. Front. Neuroanat. 5:5 10.3389/fnana.2011.00005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose H. J., Metherate R. (2005). Auditory thalamocortical transmission is reliable and temporally precise. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 2019–2030 10.1152/jn.00860.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata S., Harris K. D. (2009). Laminar structure of spontaneous and sensory-evoked population activity in auditory cortex. Neuron 64, 404–418 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakata S., Harris K. D. (2012). Laminar-dependent effects of cortical state on auditory cortical spontaneous activity. Front. Neural Circuits 6:109 10.3389/fncir.2012.00109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Vives M. V., McCormick D. A. (2000). Cellular and network mechanisms of rhythmic recurrent activity in neocortex. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1027–1034 10.1038/79848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiff M. L., Reyes A. D. (2012). Characterization of thalamocortical responses of regular-spiking and fast-spiking neurons of the mouse auditory cortex in vitro and in silico. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1476–1488 10.1152/jn.00208.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoonover C. E., Tapia J.-C., Schilling V. C., Wimmer V., Blazeski R., Zhang W., et al. (2014). Comparative strength and dendritic organization of thalamocortical and corticocortical synapses onto excitatory Layer 4 neurons. J. Neurosci. 34, 6746–6758 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0305-14.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen J. X., Xu Z. M., Yao Y. D. (1999). Evidence for columnar organization in the auditory cortex of the mouse. Hear. Res. 137, 174–177 10.1016/S0378-5955(99)00149-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shu Y. S., Hasenstaub A., Badoual M., Bal T., McCormick D. A. (2003). Barrages of synaptic activity control the gain and sensitivity of cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 23, 10388–10401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer W. (2013). Cortical dynamics revisited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 616–626 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith P. H., Populin L. C. (2001). Fundamental differences between the thalamocortical recipient layers of the cat auditory and visual cortices. J. Comp. Neurol. 436, 508–519 10.1002/cne.1084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith P. H., Uhlrich D. J., Manning K. A., Banks M. I. (2012). Thalamocortical projections to rat auditory cortex from the ventral and dorsal divisions of the medial geniculate nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 34–51 10.1002/cne.22682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M., McCormick D. A., Sejnowski T. J. (1993). Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science 262, 679–685 10.1126/science.8235588 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M., Timofeev I., Grenier F. (2001). Natural waking and sleep states: a view from inside neocortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1969–1985 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroh A., Adelsberger H., Groh A., Ruhlmann C., Fischer S., Schierloh A., et al. (2013). Making waves: initiation and propagation of corticothalamic Ca2+ waves in vivo. Neuron 77, 1136–1150 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugimoto S., Sakurada M., Horikawa J., Taniguchi I. (1997). The columnar and layer-specific response properties of neurons in the primary auditory cortex of Mongolian gerbils. Hear. Res. 112, 175–185 10.1016/S0378-5955(97)00119-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun Y. J., Kim Y. J., Ibrahim L. A., Tao H. W., Zhang L. I. (2013). Synaptic mechanisms underlying functional dichotomy between intrinsic-bursting and regular-spiking neurons in auditory cortical layer 5. J. Neurosci. 33, 5326–5339 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4810-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan A. Y., Chen Y., Scholl B., Seidemann E., Priebe N. J. (2014). Sensory stimulation shifts visual cortex from synchronous to asynchronous states. Nature 509, 226–229 10.1038/nature13159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan Z., Hu H., Huang Z. J., Agmon A. (2008). Robust but delayed thalamocortical activation of dendritic-targeting inhibitory interneurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 2187–2192 10.1073/pnas.0710628105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Theyel B. B., Llano D. A., Sherman S. M. (2010). The corticothalamocortical circuit drives higher-order cortex in the mouse. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 84–88 10.1038/nn.2449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomson A., Bannister A. (1998). Postsynaptic pyramidal target selection by descending layer III pyramidal axons: dual intracellular recordings and biocytin filling in slices of rat neocortex. Neuroscience 84, 669–683 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00557-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomson A. M., Bannister A. P. (2003). Interlaminar connections in the neocortex. Cereb. Cortex 13, 5–14 10.1093/cercor/13.1.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomson A. M., Lamy C. (2007). Functional maps of neocortical local circuitry. Front. Neurosci. 1, 19–42 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.002.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomson A. M., West D. C., Wang Y., Bannister A. P. (2002). Synaptic connections and small circuits involving excitatory and inhibitory neurons in Layers 2–5 of adult rat and cat neocortex: triple intracellular recordings and biocytin labelling in vitro. Cereb. Cortex 12, 936–953 10.1093/cercor/12.9.936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbny Y. I., Erdelyi F., Szabo G., Banks M. I. (2006). Properties of a population of GABAergic cells in murine auditory cortex weakly excited by thalamic stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 3194–3208 10.1152/jn.00484.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y., Toledo-Rodriguez M., Gupta A., Wu C., Silberberg G., Luo J., et al. (2004). Anatomical, physiological and molecular properties of Martinotti cells in the somatosensory cortex of the juvenile rat. J. Physiol. 561, 65–90 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.073353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warren R. M. (1970). Perceptual restoration of missing speech sounds. Science 167, 392–393 10.1126/science.167.3917.392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson B. O., Maclean J. N., Yuste R. (2008). UP states protect ongoing cortical activity from thalamic inputs. PLoS ONE 3:e3971 10.1371/journal.pone.0003971 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wester J. C., Contreras D. (2012). Columnar interactions determine horizontal propagation of recurrent network activity in neocortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 5454–5471 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5006-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White B., Abbott L. F., Fiser J. (2012). Suppression of cortical neural variability is stimulus- and state-dependent. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 2383–2392 10.1152/jn.00723.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]