Abstract
This paper briefly reviews the social science on “neighborhood effects” as an independent force in shaping poor outcomes, specifically mental illness and criminal behavior, before discussing the implications of that research for understanding the relationship between neighborhoods, race and class. Neighborhood effects research has proliferated in recent years with extensive attention again being focused on the social context of family and individual development and life course. Moreover, recent work has suggested the need to consider the developmental effects of neighborhoods that persist across life-span. This paper will focus specifically on mental illness and criminal behavior as outcomes for understanding neighborhood effects, but will also consider what the structural causes of individual behavior and functioning mean for clinical assessment, especially forensic assessment.
Keywords: death penalty, neighborhood effects, race, poverty, criminal behavior, mental illness
1. Introduction
Neighborhood effects research, using an expansive array of data and analyses, has made significant strides in the last twenty-five years (Raudenbush & Sampson, 1999). The relationship between neighborhoods and poor mental and physical health, although studied for decades (Faris, 1939, Reprinted 1965), now more clearly and strongly links neighborhoods to mortality, heart disease, cancer, low birth weight, infant mortality, childhood illnesses, asthma, depression, anxiety, smoking, diet and nutrition, hypertension, heart disease, suicide, accidental injuries, lead exposure, and numerous other illnesses (Roux & Mair, 2010; Morenoff & Lynch, 2004). Research on the association of family and neighborhood characteristics with delinquency and crime also began more than seventy-five years ago, reaching conclusions about the fundamental role of family life and neighborhood in behavioral problems that have been confirmed over the decades since (Glueck & Glueck, 1950; Healy & Bronner, 1936; Sampson & Laub, 1993; Shaw & McKay, 1969). Nevertheless, serious research issues remain concerning both how to measure neighborhood effects and how to interpret results that suggest associations (Roux, 2008; Oakes, 2004).
Recently, Sampson (2008) articulated an important conceptual framework for understanding the mechanisms by which neighborhoods effect individuals (Sampson, 2008). This framework argues for a dual import to neighborhoods: first, as the situational context of family and individual life - which has long been how neighborhoods are viewed (Bronfenbrenner, 1977); but second, as influencing the developmental and enduring early life course that shapes long-term development, behavior and health throughout the life of the individual regardless of subsequent neighborhood stability or individual mobility (Sampson, 2008). In considering outcomes such as mental illness and criminal behavior, this dual framework suggests important possibilities for understanding and preventing illness and crime, and therefore, is also important for clinical and forensic neuropsychiatric practice.
In this paper, we first briefly review some of the research related to neighborhoods and mental health in adults and children. We next focus more specifically on the research related to psychosis, child abuse and witnessing violence, neurotoxicant exposure and finally criminal behavior. As discussed throughout these sections, issues of race/ethnicity and poverty are interwoven into the research findings, but we also address more directly race and class and neighborhoods in considering the pathways and mechanisms by which neighborhoods may be associated with these outcomes.
2. Mental Illness and Neighborhoods
Historically, mental illness has been seen as a condition of the individual alone. Individuals, rather than families or communities, are diagnosed with mental illnesses, except in certain rare disorders, like shared delusional disorder, where several individuals, or even a community may suffer shared psychiatric symptoms. Certainly, few would disagree that mental illness is embodied in the individual. As a result, mental illness is susceptible to the individualistic fallacy which assumes that individual-level outcomes should be attributed solely to individual characteristics or traits (Silver, 2000). However, families and neighborhoods have clear associations with, and in some instances appear to be causally related to, mental illnesses. Current research finds independent, statistically significant effects when examining neighborhoods and mental illness. As Oakes writes: “It would be shocking to learn that [social] contexts did not somehow impact health. The question is about magnitude, mechanism, and mutability” (Oakes, 2004) at p.1929. The effect sizes are often small, leading to debate as to the significance of association. Yet, the question for people interested in identification, prevention, and treatment of mental illness and shifting the career course of offenders, as well as those interested in understanding the neurodevelopment and life course, is not to parse the exact contribution of one risk factor while holding all else constant, but rather to understand the interaction and mediation of associated risks that act together, both the direct and indirect effects of neighborhoods, to shape the life experiences of those who become and are mentally ill (Hafeman, 2008).
Further, while neighborhoods are the collection of individuals, suggesting that aggregated individual data might explain the neighborhood, they are also more than the sum of the individual parts. Neighborhoods have quantifiable characteristics above and beyond the aggregated individual level. For instance, vacancy rates in residential areas are a measure of the neighborhood characteristics, not the individuals who do or do not live in the area. Similarly, a neighborhood which is densely poor, may in fact have some individuals who are exceptionally wealthy. To simply use mean or median income as a measure of the neighborhood may misrepresent the lived experience of those in the neighborhood, thereby missing or mischaracterizing the effect of social context on that lived experience. Additionally, the rate of change over time of a neighborhood may be quite different than the duration of residence of any set of individuals in that area, reflecting qualities other than the aggregated sum of residents at any given point in time. Finally, neighborhoods may change by virtue of policy (changes in zoning, increased infrastructure investment to improve walkability, or the lifting of restrictive covenants) or by virtue of adjacent neighborhood changes (increased public transportation or expansion of services in the adjacent neighborhood), influences which have no individual level corresponding characteristic.
Neighborhoods, then, are more than an aggregate of individual level characteristics, but that does not sufficiently define what constitutes “neighborhood” or what should be measured to capture its characteristics. Most simply, neighborhood are the utilized space in the daily functioning of people and families, the place where people live and move to work or play or school, to shop, to interact with other people. Yet, such a simply definition would mean that every person has his or her own “neighborhood,” making the concept incoherent and quantitatively useless. Some researchers have used statistically available geographic boundaries (such as census tracts) or the aggregation of individual characteristics (the place where most people have a high socio-economic status). Some have proposed thorough approaches to defining neighborhoods for research purposes (Aronson, Wallis, O'Campo, & Schafer, 2007; Azrael et al., 2009; L. Weiss, Ompad, Galea, & Vlahov, 2007), but few studies utilize such careful and time intensive approaches. While our review is bounded by the definitions used by the researchers who conducted the studies, the conceptual approach to neighborhoods is predicated on the idea that neighborhoods are defined by physical space, shared social norms and expectations, social networks, and institutional structures.
Structural and distal causes of functioning and behavior affect groups and populations. Clinical and forensic practice often becomes over-focused on the individual, losing out on important influences and risks that increase the rates and risks for mental illness, as well as the possibilities of intervention and prevention. Thus, when we examine individuals and seek to understand why this person got this disease at this time, we are often unable to examine and understand the mechanisms by which an illness is distributed within a population (Rose, 1985). By understanding the role of neighborhoods and communities in mental illness, we are able to compare not just individuals to other individuals, but social contexts in which those individuals developed and lived and became ill.
2.1 Adults
Truong and Ma (2006) reviewed the literature on adult mental health and neighborhoods (Truong & Ma, 2006). They found 27/29 studies reported statistically significant neighborhood effect on mental health for adults. The studies used differing measures of neighborhood and outcomes, making comparisons difficult across the group of studies, but in general, the evidence supported the finding that neighborhoods have an independent effect on the incidence of mental illness, specifically: symptoms of depression, psychological distress, anxiety and psychosis. The studies fell into three groupings regarding the proposed mechanisms of these effects: structural characteristics of the neighborhoods (i.e., socio-demographic make-up), neighborhood disorder (i.e., perceived safety and social and/or physical “uncivility”), and environmental stressors (i.e., stressors and resources).
The United States Office of Housing and Urban Development (HUD) sought to directly test whether concentrated poverty caused worse mental health, employment, and school outcomes by conducting an experiment which randomized people into types of housing options. The Moving to Opportunity (MTO) project randomized a sample of people living in neighborhoods of concentrated poverty in five major cities, into three groups: the first group, a control group who continued to be eligible for public housing; the second group was referred to as the Section 8 group, received a Section 8 housing voucher without geographic restriction; and the third group, the experimental group, was given a Section 8 housing voucher that restricted the density of poverty in the census tract to which they were permitted to move (Kling, Liebman, & Katz, 2007). Although much debate about how to interpret the results, and whether the experimental design was adequate to find results given how few people opted to move or moved into neighborhoods that offered better opportunity (Aliprantis & Richter, 2012), has followed the MTO project, a few clear findings emerge. Adult mental health, for those who moved out of concentrated poverty (a small portion of the experimental group), improved significantly and those improvements have persisted over time (Kling et al., 2007).
Other studies have found similar, more robust, relationships between adult mental well-being and neighborhood effects. For instance, a ten-year longitudinal study of British civil servants examined neighborhood deprivation and social fragmentation, independent of individual socio-economic status, and found each associated with poorer mental functioning. The differences over time widened between those in more-compared-to-less fragmented neighborhoods as well as those in more-compared-to-less deprived neighborhoods, indicating a cumulative negative effect of deprivation and fragmentation (Stafford, Gimeno, & Marmot, 2008).
2.2 Children and Adolescents
As with adults, children's mental health and behavioral problems are also associated with neighborhood effects, most typically concentrated disadvantage (Caughy, Nettles, & O'Campo, 2008; Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn, & Earls, 2005). Children living in poverty were more likely than non-poor children to have a psychiatric disorder (Costello, Compton, Keeler, & Angold, 2003); psychological distress is higher among those who live in high poverty neighborhoods (Schulz et al., 2000); and those living in poverty have substantially worse physical health (Aber, Bennett, Conley, & Li, 1997). Poverty's consequences continue to effect children as they develop, with lowered education attainment, heightened risk of accidents, increased school drop-out, decreased IQ, increased risk for child maltreatment and neglect and increased behavior problems (Baydar, Brooks-Gunn, & Furstenberg, 1993; Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994; Furstenberg, Brooks-Gunn, & Morgan, 1987). When children moved from poverty to wealth in a natural experiment, after four years of affluence, psychiatric symptoms of previously poor children declined to match those children who had never been poor. This effect was strongest in the area of behavioral symptoms, e.g. conduct and oppositional disorder (Costello et al., 2003).
However, the evidence that changing neighborhoods improves child mental health is not always observed. For instance, children in the MTO had different outcomes of moving to less densely poor neighborhoods depending on gender and victimization, with girls' mental health improving but boys from vulnerable families experiencing a worsening mental health (Osypuk, Schmidt, et al., 2012; Osypuk, Tchetgen, et al., 2012). These findings suggest that neighborhoods in and of themselves are not the answer to mental health problems, but support the notion that they must be considered as influential variables when assessing risk and causation, and considered as part of the mechanism of both good and poor mental health outcomes.
Similar research explains that child problem behaviors may be explained, at least in part by measures of concentrated disadvantage. While controlling for individual family economic factors, the research indicates that neighborhood economic disadvantage has a significant impact on maladaptive behaviors. Thus, a child whose family's economic status is above the poverty line, but who lives in a neighborhood of concentrated disadvantage, is at heightened risk for behavior problems. Living in a disadvantaged neighborhood is an independent risk for behavior problems for children over time. This appears most significant during the transition from childhood to adolescence (Kalff et al., 2001; Schneiders et al., 2003).
Studies of twins raised apart found that variability in intelligence among children is related to the socioeconomic status of the family in which the twin was raised: for those raised in poverty, their poverty accounted for 60% of the variance in IQ scores while genes accounted for nearly none of the variance; for those raised in affluence, the results showed the opposite, with genes accounting for about 60% of variance (Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron, D'Onofrio, & Gottesman, 2003).
Other studies have found pre-frontal cortex deficits, manifested as executive dysfunction, related to socio-economic status (SES), although most of them have relied on global measures of SES and not sought to differentiate the influence of more closely defined neighborhood factors from SES. Nevertheless, impairments have been noted in cognitive flexibility, language performance and working memory associated with low SES (Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry, & Knight, 2009). Conversely, collective efficacy may underlie resilience in some children and may lower the incidence of mental illness (Xue et al., 2005).
3. Psychosis and Neighborhood
While depression and anxiety in relation to chaotic and fear-inducing neighborhood conditions makes a kind of simple conceptual sense (Cutrona, Wallace, & Wesner, 2006), the relationship between neighborhoods and psychosis is less intuitive. Nevertheless, many studies have now found strong associations between psychosis and neighborhood effects. A recent review found an increased incidence of schizophrenia across many countries, although primarily in Europe, for migrants (Cantor-Graae, 2007). A meta-analysis of 50 studies examining first and second generation migrants found a relative risk of 2.9 schizophrenia, with second generation immigrants having an RR of 4.5 and immigrants of color having an RR of 4.8 (Cantor-Graae & Selten, 2005).
Another review, which specifically examined urbanicity and neighborhood effects, found that both are associated with psychosis but that methodological problems in the reviewed studies undermine the ability to reach conclusions regarding neighborhood effects. Of the 44 studies reviewed, urbanicity was found to increase the risk of psychosis between two- and four-fold; and, despite methodological problems, many of the individual neighborhood effects studies found significant associations but were unable to reach broader conclusions (March et al., 2008).
Some of the current hypotheses regarding why the rates of psychosis are higher for some migrants include discrimination and social isolation. That is, that the social experience of migration, especially for migrants of color moving into countries which are predominantly white, contributes to the vulnerability and onset of psychosis. For instance, a study of psychosis in The Hague found the experience of racial/ethnic discrimination raised the incidence of psychosis, with high discrimination experiences leading to 4 incidence rate ratios compared to 1.2 incidence rate rations for very low discrimination exposure. Neighborhood measures included rates of long-term unemployment, income, poor quality of housing, and level of education. This study suggests that the perception of discrimination may contribute to increased risk for psychosis (Veling et al., 2007). Similarly, in a prospective study of the incidence of psychosis, immigrants living in neighborhoods where their own ethnic group comprised only a small percent of the population had higher incidence, suggesting that social isolation and the experience of exclusion may also play a role in the development of psychosis (Veling et al., 2008).
Moreover, a case-control study of first episode psychosis found that cases were more socially disadvantaged and isolated than controls. Six domains were included for assessing disadvantage and isolation: education, employment, living arrangements, housing, relationships and social networks. As the number of indicated risk factors rose, the incidence of psychosis rose as well. The initial risk for psychosis was generally similar for White British and Black Caribbean immigrants, but Black Caribbean immigrants had higher exposure to the indicated risk factors (Morgan et al., 2008).
One hypothesis, which needs further research, suggests that the higher rates of psychosis and the higher rates of child abuse that are associated with neighborhood effects may be related to each other. A recent study found a dose-response relationship in a prospective study between childhood trauma and the risk of psychosis, with an odds ratio for child abuse predicting psychosis of 7.3 percent (Janssen et al., 2004). Others have reported this association as well (Larkin & Morrison, 2006).
4. Childhood Physical /Sexual Abuse, Witnessing Violence and Neighborhoods
The long-term mental health consequences of childhood physical and sexual abuse and witnessing violence are well-established (Follette, Polusny, Bechtle, & Naugle, 1996; Herman, 1997; Kaplan et al., 1998; Malinoskyrummell & Hansen, 1993; Martinez & Richters, 1993; Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Roberts, O'Connor, Dunn, Golding, & Team, 2004; Van der Kolk, McFarlane, & Weisæth, 1996). Parental abuse of children has historically been viewed solely as a failure of parenting; not an unreasonable view, but one that may be too narrow when seeking to understand and prevent such abuse (Molnar, Buka, Brennan, Holton, & Earls, 2003). More recently, researchers have found considerable agreement across a range of studies that neighborhood disadvantage is also associated with child maltreatment. That is, neighborhoods have a significant effect on the incidence of childhood physical and sexual abuse even when controlling for individual and family differences (Coulton, Crampton, Irwin, Spilsbury, & Korbin, 2007). Abuse has been associated with economic and family resources, residential instability and geographic proximity to neighborhoods of concentrated disadvantage (Coulton, Korbin, Su, & Chow, 1995). Concentrated disadvantage and community violence significantly predicted parent to child aggression (Molnar et al., 2003). The measure of the concentrated disadvantage of the neighborhood typically refers to the percentage of residents below the poverty line, the percentage on family assistance, the percentage of female headed households, the percentage of unemployed, the percentage of children under 18, and the percentage of African Americans (Coulton, Korbin, & Su, 1999; Coulton et al., 1995; Molnar et al., 2003).
The statistical findings of the effect of neighborhoods on the incidence of abuse is significant but small because family factors interact with neighborhood factors. The interaction makes sense, because although the family is the actual mechanism of the maltreatment, neighborhood-level factors such as economic and family resources, residential instability, household make-up, and geographic proximity to concentrated poverty areas are all associated with the occurrence of maltreatment. Thus, while this research indicates that neighborhoods do not primarily cause child maltreatment, neighborhood factors are part of the understanding of how and when and why child maltreatment occurs. Neighborhoods with the highest maltreatment rates were those with high combinations of poverty, unemployment, racial segregation, abandoned housing, population loss, lack of child care, few elderly residents, and which border other neighborhoods with high density poverty. These structural factors explain a significant part of the statistical variance in maltreatment across neighborhoods (Coulton et al., 1995).
Moreover, witnessing community violence has been shown to be associated with both behavioral and psychological problems in youth. The individual effects of exposure to community violence (depression, withdrawal, dissociative coping, aggression, substance abuse, stress, post-traumatic stress disorder and other physiological deficits) are significant (Buka, Stichick, Birdthistle, & Earls, 2001; Salzinger, Feldman, Stockhammer, & Hood, 2002). Witnessing community violence is related to child maltreatment and poor outcomes as both the “severity of neglect and victimization by violence in the community are significant predictors of children's functioning” (p. 246) (Lynch & Cicchetti, 1998). The neighborhood can create increased risk for both exposure to violence and for physical and sexual abuse within the family (Garbarino & Sherman, 1980).
A community survey found that where there were lower than expected rates of child abuse there was higher reported satisfaction with neighborhoods (Garbarino & Sherman, 1980). They suggested that neighborhood, and community member perception of the neighborhood, was an important factor in child maltreatment. Similarly, a neighborhood with a high degree of collective efficacy may in fact provide a protective effect against child abuse. Meaning, where the neighborhood has strong social cohesion, that cohesion may provide a protection for children against the violence of a parent (Silk, Sessa, Morris, Steinberg, & Avenevoli, 2004). Neighborhoods have been associated with resiliency as well, meaning, neighborhood advantage may assist abused children in more quickly overcoming and coping with the abuse (DuMont, Widom, & Czaja, 2007).
5. Neurotoxicant Exposures
Exposure to neurotoxicants, in particular pesticides, metals and solvents, is also a pervasive neighborhood-level problem that has significant effects on developmental course (Landrigan et al., 1999). For instance, exposure to DDT in utero has been found to cause significant neuro-developmental delays (Eskenazi et al., 2006). Similarly, childhood exposures to pesticides and flame retardants have been associated with persistent neuro-developmental delays (Eskenazi, Bradman, & Castorina, 1999; Eskenazi et al., 2013). The neighborhood level issues result from the unequal distribution of environmental hazards such that they disproportionately expose some people more than others (Cole & Foster, 2001). The effect of lead, for instance, has clearly been shown to be unequally distributed by both race and class, as well as being related to a host of negative health and mental health outcomes (Hu, Shih, Rothenberg, & Schwartz, 2007). The likelihood and extent of exposure to neurotoxicants is dependent, at least in part, on neighborhood factors such as segregation and density of poverty and public policy related to the zoning of hazards and housing; these factors in turn shape the developmental course of those who live in neighborhoods with high levels of neurotoxic agents (Bullard, Johnson, & Torres, 2000).
Researchers have sometimes viewed neurotoxicant exposure as a confound of poverty. This appears to be an inaccurate inference, however, and may lead to a false attribution of effects (Bellinger, 2008). Instead, Bellinger has argued that lead exposure is an effect modification with potential direct influence on dose-response relationships, and therefore on health outcomes (Bellinger, 2000). Therefore, rather than “controlling” for context when assessing the effect of neurotoxicant exposure on health and behavior, this research suggests a more careful consideration of the effect modification of neighborhood level effects when considering the outcomes of exposure.
Both high exposures (including poisoning) and chronic, low-level exposures are associated with a host of negative health and mental health effects. A wealth of research indicates that chronic pesticide exposure is associated with decreased cognitive, psychomotor and psychiatric functioning (Kamel & Hoppin, 2004). These changes can persist throughout the life time of the exposed person and can be dramatic (Ecobichon & Joy, 1994; Feldman, 1999).
For children, from in utero to adolescence, the human body is less able to physiologically eliminate pesticides compared to adults. The absorption in children is more than 70% compared to absorption in adults of 30% of many chemical agents. In addition, because the central nervous system is developing during the course of adolescence and vulnerable to toxic mutation, exposure prior to and during adolescence alters the development and functioning of the brain to a greater degree (Rice & Barone, 2000).
Perhaps more than any other agent, the neurocognitive effects of lead have been studied extensively. Declines in IQ scores have been demonstrated over many years (Canfield, Henderson, et al., 2003; Lanphear et al., 2005); reduction in brain volume, specifically in frontal gray matter and the anterior cingulate cortex, has more recently been shown (Cecil et al., 2008); behavioral problems, including arrests, as well as neuropsychological impairments such as spatial attention, executive functioning, attention, working memory and learning (Canfield, Kreher, Cornwell, & Henderson, 2003; Surkan et al., 2007; Wright et al., 2008). Lead exposure has also been associated with depression, anxiety, irritability and anger (Shih, Hu, Weisskopf, & Schwartz, 2007).
In adults, the cumulative exposure to lead is associated with increased neurocognitive decline. The most significant association between bone-lead level and cognitive decline was found in total cognitive score, spatial ability, learning and memory, and in executive functioning over time compared to controls (Khalil et al., 2009). The consequences of cumulative life-time lead exposure are exacerbated by neighborhood level psychosocial hazards. The combination resulted in diminished cognitive functioning in executive function and language areas for adults. The lead exposure-cognitive impairment demonstrated dose-response characteristics and has a plausible biological mechanism (Glass et al., 2009).
Pesticides have also been shown to significantly increase the risk for mental illness based on both chronic exposure and poisoning. While pesticide poisoning increased the risk of depression by an odds ratio of 2.57%, chronic exposure increased the odds ration by 1.54 percent (Beseler et al., 2008). Pesticides (primarily organophosphates and organochlorines) are also associated with a host of symptoms that may be short- or long-term in duration: irritability, depression, anxiety, mood lability, agitation, memory impairment, confusion, hallucinations, academic deficits, hyperactivity, poor concentration, paranoia, dissociation and somatic complaints (Brown, 2002).
Finally, environmental deprivation may have a potentiating effect on neurotoxicant exposure deficits. Put more positively, environmental enrichment can serve as a tool to assist exposed children in being resilient. Heightened stress (such as exposure to violence or negative neighborhood factors) and heightened maternal stress also appear to negatively interact with neurotoxic exposure (Cory-Slechta, Virgolini, Thiruchelvam, Weston, & Bauter, 2004; Weiss & Bellinger, 2006). In this way, a mechanism by which neurotoxic exposure and the other neighborhood level factors discussed can be understood to relate to worsened mental health outcomes.
6. Criminal Behavior
As discussed above, immigration studies first found a “race” effect for psychosis, but later research suggests that the perception of discrimination and migration status itself, as well as neighborhood effects, may be the more important mechanisms in understanding the observed findings. This is not to argue for a more limited view of the importance of race and class, but rather for a more precise measurement -- and as a result of so doing, to shift the individual-level markers of race and class back to the structural-level of their operation and effect and to understand for what they stand as proxies (Manly, 2006; Manly & Echemendia, 2007).
No area of research more so than studies of criminal behavior has been confounded by issues of race and class. Having reviewed some of the neighborhood effects literature on mental health, the question arises as to how neighborhood effects may relate to criminal behavior. Are the same factors observed in the health and mental health research found in the research on criminal behavior?
Recently, the role of race and class in the perception of neighborhood disorder has attracted attention, with some research suggesting that the racial and class make-up of a neighborhood shapes people's perceptions of disorder more than trash, graffiti or broken windows (Franzini, Caughy, Nettles, & O'Campo, 2008). This research runs directly counter to the “broken window” theory of crime, first posited in 1982 (Wilson & Kelling, 2011), which suggested that public disorder of any sort leads to criminal behavior (Kelling & Coles, 1996). This theory argues that the unrepaired window leads to the breakdown of community social control, and therefore increased crime. The idea that race and class perceptions, rather than disorder, effect residents' view of neighborhoods undermines the proposition that disorder is causative of violence.
However, when the question of race and crime are analyzed directly, the results indicate that neighborhood disadvantage rather than race explains significantly more variation in crime rates. Individual differences (family poverty status, IQ, and impulsivity) accounted for about 6% of variance between white and African-American crime, whereas neighborhood disadvantage (including racial segregation) explained 60% of the difference (Sampson, Morenoff, & Raudenbush, 2005). Further, variation between neighborhoods is more significant to understanding crime than race, meaning that crime rates for whites and African-Americans are almost identical when controlling for neighborhood level differences (Peterson, Krivo, & Hagan, 2006). These findings echo those of McNulty and Bellair who reported that differences between neighborhoods, rather than people, explained criminal youth violence (McNulty & Bellair, 2003).
In fact, in an analysis of Chicago neighborhoods and homicide, researchers found that spatial proximity to violence, collective efficacy and measures of affluence/resource inequality were the most significant predictors of variations in homicide rates (Morenoff, Sampson, & Raudenbush, 2001). This research challenges the view that minor disorder leads to major crime, finding instead that low collective efficacy neighborhoods tend to be higher in both disorder and criminal activity (Sampson & Raudenbush, 1999; Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997a). Most interestingly, this research has shown that where collective efficacy is high, that is, where neighbors have shared expectations and the neighborhood has a strong sense of cohesion, even where poverty is concentrated, crime is low; and in a analysis of homicides, the findings are even stronger that the combination of collective efficacy and measures of inequality are exceptionally strong predictors of homicides (Morenoff et al., 2001).
Relatedly, research has shown that impulsive boys were at greater risk for juvenile offending if they lived in densely poor neighborhoods compared to impulsive boys who lived in better neighborhoods (Lynam et al., 2000). As suggested by this research, the interaction between individual factors, such as mental illness, and neighborhood effects, such as dense poverty or collective efficacy, suggests that context is critical to understanding behavior and outcomes of concern such as criminal offending.
7. Neighborhood Effects Mechanisms and Implications
The research reviewed here on crime and mental illness suggests that neighborhood effects must be considered if the determinants of behavior and functioning are to be understood. Neighborhoods are defined by physical space, shared social norms and expectations, social networks, and institutional structures, and neighborhoods do appear to play a role in the mechanism by which some people develop mental illnesses, and behavioral and functional impairments. Sampson (2008) argues for understanding neighborhood effects mechanisms on individual behavior in two ways: first, in the situational context of life-course in a place; and second, in the developmental and enduring effects that neighborhoods exercise on early life course that may persist throughout the life of the individual regardless of neighborhood stability or individual mobility (Sampson, 2008). Certainly the known long-term effects of childhood exposure to violence, childhood exposure to neurotoxicants and the onset and course of many serious mental illnesses, support the notion of studying neighborhood effects in this way. That the effects long outlast changed circumstances supports the concept of examining both context and developmental course.
The literature on neighborhood effects would appear to point to collective efficacy and concentrated disadvantage as the ways in which individuals are shaped by neighborhoods. Collective efficacy, which refers to the level of mutual trust and cohesion among residents and their willingness to work toward the common good is related to the structural characteristics of the neighborhood. Collective efficacy includes a shared willingness and capacity for people in a neighborhood to intervene informally (exercise informal social control) in neighborhood activities to promote social good. Research has shown that where collective efficacy is high, that is, where neighbors have shared expectations and the neighborhood has a strong sense of cohesion, even where poverty is concentrated, crime is low (Sampson et al., 1997a).
Alternatively, in neighborhoods in which collective efficacy is low, residents may feel isolated and have little belief in the neighborhood's capacity to improve negative situations, such as drug dealing or crime. In such neighborhoods residents are less willing to enforce conventional behaviors or provide control for inappropriate activities that occur (Earls & Carlson, 2001; Sampson, Morenoff, & Earls, 1999).
Neighborhood effects research has found that concentrated disadvantage and residential instability explain 70% of neighborhood variation in how willing people are to help their neighbors, intervene on their behalf or protect other people's children (Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997b).
A recent summary of neighborhood research finds that
[T]he evidence is solid on the ecological differentiation of American cities along socio-economic and racial lines, which in turn corresponds to the spatial differentiation of neighborhoods by multiple child, adolescent, and adult behaviors. These conditions are interrelated and appear to vary in systematic and theoretically meaningful ways with hypothesized social mechanisms such as informal social control, trust, institutional resources and routines, peer-group delinquency, and perceived disorder. An important take-away of our assessment is that these and other neighborhood-level mechanisms can be measured reliably with survey, observational, and archival approaches.
(p. 473) (Sampson, Morenoff, & Gannon-Rowley, 2002).
Neighborhood processes can and should be treated as ecological or collective phenomena rather than as individual-level perceptions or traits. Collective efficacy is a measure of informal social control and mutual dependence, where people believe that members of their community will assist them when they are in need. Nevertheless, collective efficacy has an independent effect on both contextual and life-course development of mental illness and criminal behavior. This effect is significant enough that it should not be overlooked or studied by proxy measures.
Similarly, concentrated disadvantage, rather than vague notions of socioeconomic status or other proxy markers, have demonstrated robust impact on the context and life-course of individuals as indicated when looking at mental illness and criminal behavior as outcomes. Although often studied through proxy measures that fail to adequately address how and why concentrated disadvantage operates, significant findings point to concentrated disadvantage having life-course effects (Sampson, 2008).
Perhaps equally significant in this research are the questions that are raised about how we understand race and class. Both have been used variously as proxy measures to the detriment of more nuanced understandings. Each needs to be deconstructed if we are to explore the actual mechanisms of a host of behavioral, psychological and health outcomes.
The evidence that the proxy measures of race and class have clear health and behavior consequences is not undermined by seeking a better understanding of what is meant when those categories are used to group people. Residential segregation is the clearest means by which these structural factors shape people's lives by defining their access to, and the quality of, medical and social services, employment, education, food, mobility, environmental hazards and a host of disadvantage (Acevedo-Garcia, Osypuk, McArdle, & Williams, 2008).
Racial segregation in housing not only affects individuals but also acts as a social and neighborhood structural barrier. A study of urban Atlanta found substantial race-based discrimination in housing and work, including a spatial mismatch (poor people who need entry level jobs are unable to live in proximity to those jobs) and housing segregation (inability to move based on race), which resulted in a concentration of poor people into densely poor areas (Sjoquist, 2000). These neighborhood factors shape the lives of people by narrowing their options and teaching mean lessons about what it means to be poor or a person of color (W. J. Wilson, 1996). Nationally, African Americans among all racial groups are most physically segregated from jobs (Stoll & Raphael, 2002). In terms of mental health care, in 2001, the Surgeon General of the United States issued a report finding serious disparities in mental health care and treatment for people of color. The report found that people of color are less likely than whites to receive services and more likely to receive poor quality services when they do (Health and Human Services, 2001). In addition, perceptions of discrimination are associated with poorer mental and physical health (Williams, Neighbors, & Jackson, 2003).
Further, consideration of neighborhood disadvantage accounts for some of the race/ethnic and socioeconomic position differences in health, particularly in hypertension. When controlling for neighborhood effects, a significant amount of the statistical difference in the incidence of hypertension that appears based on racial/ethnic status goes away (Morenoff et al., 2007).
When looking at violence in patients recently discharged from a psychiatric hospital, researchers found that, after controlling for individual factors (such as age, diagnosis, prior arrests), the concentrated disadvantage of the neighborhood into which the patient was discharged was predictive of future violence. That is, the risk that a patient discharged from locked facility would engage in future violence increased 2.7 times if the person was discharged into a neighborhood of concentrated disadvantage compared to a less disadvantaged one (Silver, 2000; Silver, Mulvey, & Monahan, 1999). But they also found that “the significant association between African-American racial status and violence was completely eliminated when neighborhood disadvantage was controlled” (p. 405) (Silver, 2001).
In a study of stereotype threat, researchers found that simply asking African-Americans to record their race before a test significantly lowered test performance compared to whites and compared to African-American controls (Steele & Aronson, 1995). Whatever else this research says, it provides some insight into how structural barriers to equality, perhaps ones that are not context-driven but which shaped the developmental and experiential life-course of the test subjects, act on individuals. Simply examining how these subjects scored on tests would lead to false conclusions and useless intervention strategies. Similarly, research that relies on race and class as proxies about crime, violence or mental illness are missing critical information which might re-shape the understanding of cause and course of behavior and illness. Instead, neighborhood effects, in their specific mechanisms of action, can assist in deconstructing those proxy measures such that a more accurate and meaningful understanding can be sought.
How we understand race and class effects should be broadened to look at both the context effect of discrimination, isolation and perception, as well as the long-term effect on mental health and criminal behavior. When Sampson argued for this dual understanding, it was in order that the research begin to take into account both mechanisms (Sampson, 2008). Neighborhood effects have both a contextual consequence as well as a long-term developmental consequence, and the outcomes discussed here also demonstrate evidence of such influences. Yet, we often ignore the developmental and course aspects of both, which means we are missing important prevention and intervention evidence as well as misconstruing the results of the data. How we understand neighborhood effects, both collective efficacy and concentrated disadvantage, should also be helping us understand the context and life course of individual behavior and illness.
This should also affect how forensic assessment is conducted. Competent forensic neurobehavioral assessment requires a thorough multigenerational social history, along with the integration of information obtained from multiple sources, across a number of disciplinary approaches, each of which assess different aspects of behavior and functioning (Woods, Freedman, & Greenspan, 2012). It is no longer adequate to consider the individual out of context or to seek to explain behavior and functioning without regard to the causal effects of structural forces. Forensic assessment, in our view, must include an assessment of those structural forces to be meaningful and valid.
This means that popular forensic views about individuals characteristics and actions will need to be replaced with scientifically reliable and valid evidence of the dynamic forces which shape the life course. Behavior and functioning will need to assessed from the viewpoint of the causal forces that shape an individual from place in which they are born, to the cultural and ethnic biases they have faced, to the opportunities available or denied. Competent forensic assessment should consider the ways in which neighborhood effects, as one example of the structural forces demonstrated to affect behavior and functioning in mental illness and crime, alter the life trajectory of the individual.
Acknowledgments
Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by NIMH Training Grant 5T32MH-13043-40 (DF).
References
- Aber JL, Bennett NG, Conley DC, Li JL. The effects of poverty on child health and development. Annual Review of Public Health. 1997;18:463–483. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.18.1.463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.18.1.463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Acevedo-Garcia D, Osypuk TL, McArdle N, Williams DR. Toward a policy-relevant analysis of geographic and racial/ethnic disparities in child health. Health Aff (Millwood) 2008;27(2):321–333. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.27.2.321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1077/hlthaff.27.2.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aliprantis D, Richter FGC. Marginal neighborhood effects from Moving to Opportunity: Working Paper 12-08R. Reserve Bank of Cleveland; 2012. Retrieved from www.clevelandfed.org/research/workpaper/2012/wp1208r.pdf Federal. [Google Scholar]
- Aronson RE, Wallis AB, O'Campo PJ, Schafer P. Neighborhood mapping and evaluation: a methodology for participatory community health initiatives. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2007;11(4):373–383. doi: 10.1007/s10995-007-0184-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10995-007-0184-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Azrael D, Johnson RM, Molnar BE, Vriniotis M, Dunn EC, Duncan DT, Hemenway D. Creating a Youth Violence Data System for Boston, Massachusetts. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology. 2009;42(3):406–421. [Google Scholar]
- Baydar N, Brooks-Gunn J, Furstenberg FF. Early Warning Signs of Functional Illiteracy - Predictors in Childhood and Adolescence. Child Dev. 1993;64(3):815–829. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02945.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02945.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellinger DC. Effect modification in epidemiologic studies of low-level neurotoxicant exposures and health outcomes. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22(1):133–140. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(99)00053-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0892-0362(99)00053-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellinger DC. Lead neurotoxicity and socioeconomic status: Conceptual and analytical issues. Neurotoxicology. 2008;29(5):828–832. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2008.04.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2008.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beseler CL, Stallones L, Hoppin JA, Alavanja MCR, Blair A, Keefe T, Kamel F. Depression and Pesticide Exposures among Private Pesticide Applicators Enrolled in the Agricultural Health Study. Environ Health Perspect. 2008;116(12):1713–1719. doi: 10.1289/ehp.11091. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.11091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bronfenbrenner U. Toward an Experimental Ecology of Human-Development. American Psychologist. 1977;32(7):513–531. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.32.7.513. [Google Scholar]
- Brown JS. Environmental and chemical toxins and psychiatric illness. 1st. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Buka SL, Stichick TL, Birdthistle I, Earls FJ. Youth exposure to violence: Prevalence, risks, and consequences. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2001;71(3):298–310. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.71.3.298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0002-9432.71.3.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullard RD, Johnson GS, Torres AO. Sprawl city: Race, politics, and planning in Atlanta. Washington, D.C: Island Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Canfield RL, Henderson CR, Cory-Slechta DA, Cox C, Jusko TA, Lanphear BP. Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 mu g per deciliter. New England Journal of Medicine. 2003;348(16):1517–1526. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022848. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa022848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canfield RL, Kreher DA, Cornwell C, Henderson CR. Low-level lead exposure, executive functioning, and learning in early childhood. Child Neuropsychology. 2003;9(1):35–53. doi: 10.1076/chin.9.1.35.14496. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/chin.9.1.35.14496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantor-Graae E. The contribution of social factors to the development of schizophrenia: A review of recent findings. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry-Revue Canadienne De Psychiatrie. 2007;52(5):277–286. doi: 10.1177/070674370705200502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantor-Graae E, Selten JP. Schizophrenia and migration: A meta-analysis and review. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(1):12–24. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caughy MOB, Nettles SM, O'Campo PJ. The effect of residential neighborhood on child behavior problems in first grade. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2008;42(1-2):39–50. doi: 10.1007/s10464-008-9185-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10464-008-9185-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cecil KM, Brubaker CJ, Adler CM, Dietrich KN, Altaye M, Egelhoff JC, Lanphear BP. Decreased brain volume in adults with childhood lead exposure. Plos Medicine. 2008;5(5):741–750. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0050112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole LW, Foster SR. From the ground up: environmental racism and the rise of the environmental justice movement. New York: New York University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Cory-Slechta DA, Virgolini MB, Thiruchelvam M, Weston DD, Bauter MR. Maternal stress modulates the effects of developmental lead exposure. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112(6):717–730. doi: 10.1289/ehp.6481. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.6481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costello EJ, Compton SN, Keeler G, Angold A. Relationships between poverty and psychopathology - A natural experiment. Jama-Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;290(15):2023–2029. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.290.15.2023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coulton CJ, Crampton DS, Irwin M, Spilsbury JC, Korbin JE. How neighborhoods influence child maltreatment: A review of the literature and alternative pathways. Child Abuse Negl. 2007;31(11-12):1117–1142. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.03.023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coulton CJ, Korbin JE, Su M. Neighborhoods and child maltreatment: A multi-level study. Child Abuse Negl. 1999;23(11):1019–1040. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(99)00076-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0145-2134(99)00076-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coulton CJ, Korbin JE, Su M, Chow J. Community-Level Factors and Child Maltreatment Rates. Child Dev. 1995;66(5):1262–1276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00934.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutrona CE, Wallace G, Wesner KA. Neighborhood characteristics and depression - An examination of stress processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15(4):188–192. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00433.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00433.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV. Next steps in understanding the multilevel determinants of health. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2008;62(11):957–959. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.064311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2007.064311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diez Roux AV, Mair C. Neighborhoods and health. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186(1):125–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05333.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05333.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DuMont KA, Widom CS, Czaja SJ. Predictors of resilience in abused and neglected children grown-up: The role of individual and neighborhood characteristics. Child Abuse Negl. 2007;31(3):255–274. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.11.015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic Deprivation and Early-Childhood Development. Child Dev. 1994;65(2):296–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00752.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Earls F, Carlson M. The social ecology of child health and well-being. Annual Review of Public Health. 2001;22:143–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.22.1.143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.22.1.143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ecobichon DJ, Joy RM. Pesticides and neurological diseases. 2nd. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Eskenazi B, Bradman A, Castorina R. Exposures of children to organophosphate pesticides and their potential adverse health effects. Environ Health Perspect. 1999;107:409–419. doi: 10.1289/ehp.99107s3409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eskenazi B, Chevrier J, Rauch SA, Kogut K, Harley KG, Johnson C, Bradman A. In utero and childhood polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE) exposures and neurodevelopment in the CHAMACOS study. Environ Health Perspect. 2013;121(2):257–262. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1205597. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.1205597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eskenazi B, Marks AR, Bradman A, Fenster L, Johnson C, Barr DB, Jewell NP. In utero exposure to dichlorodiphenyltrichloroethane (DDT) and dichlorodiphenyldichloroethylene (DDE) and neurodevelopment among young Mexican American children. Pediatrics. 2006;118(1):233–241. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-3117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-3117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faris RaHD. Mental disorders in urban areas. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1939. Reprinted 1965. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman RG. Occupational and environmental neurotoxicology. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Follette VM, Polusny MA, Bechtle AE, Naugle AE. Cumulative trauma: The impact of child sexual abuse, adult sexual assault, and spouse abuse. J Trauma Stress. 1996;9(1):25–35. doi: 10.1007/BF02116831. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02116831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franzini L, Caughy MOB, Nettles SM, O'Campo P. Perceptions of disorder: Contributions of neighborhood characteristics to subjective perceptions of disorder. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2008;28(1):83–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.08.003. [Google Scholar]
- Furstenberg FF, Brooks-Gunn J, Morgan SP. Adolescent Mothers and Their Children In Later Life. Family Planning Perspectives. 1987;19(4):142–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2135159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garbarino J, Sherman D. High-Risk Neighborhoods and High-Risk Families - The Human-Ecology Of Child Maltreatment. Child Dev. 1980;51(1):188–198. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1980.tb02525.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glass TA, Bandeen-Roche K, McAtee M, Bolla K, Todd AC, Schwartz BS. Neighborhood Psychosocial Hazards and of the Association Cumulative Lead Dose With Cognitive Function in Older Adults. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;169(6):683–692. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwn390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwn390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glueck S, Glueck ET. Unraveling juvenile delinquency. New York: Commonwealth Fund; 1950. [Google Scholar]
- Hafeman DM. A sufficient cause based approach to the assessment of mediation. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;23(11):711–721. doi: 10.1007/s10654-008-9286-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10654-008-9286-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Healy W, Bronner AF. New light on delinquency and its treatment; results of a research conducted for the Institute of human relations. New Haven: Pub for the Institute of human relations by Yale university press; 1936. [Google Scholar]
- Herman JL. Trauma and recovery. Rev. New York: BasicBooks; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Hu H, Shih R, Rothenberg S, Schwartz BS. The epidemiology of lead toxicity in adults: Measuring dose and consideration of other methodologic issues. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115(3):455–462. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9783. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.9783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janssen I, Krabbendam L, Bak M, Hanssen M, Vollebergh W, de Graaf R, van Os J. Childhood abuse as a risk factor for psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2004;109(1):38–45. doi: 10.1046/j.0001-690x.2003.00217.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0001-690X.2003.00217.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalff AC, Kroes M, Vles JSH, Hendriksen JGM, Feron FJM, Steyaert J, van Os J. Neighbourhood level and individual level SES effects on child problem behaviour: a multilevel analysis. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2001;55(4):246–250. doi: 10.1136/jech.55.4.246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.55.4.246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamel F, Hoppin JA. Association of pesticide exposure with neurologic dysfunction and disease. Environ Health Perspect. 2004;112(9):950–958. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.7135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan SJ, Pelcovitz D, Salzinger S, Weiner M, Mandel FS, Lesser ML, Labruna VE. Adolescent physical abuse: Risk for adolescent psychiatric disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;155(7):954–959. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.7.954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelling GL, Coles CM. Fixing broken windows: restoring order and reducing crime in our communities. New York: Martin Kessler Books; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Khalil N, Morrow LA, Needleman H, Talbott EO, Wilson JW, Cauley JA. Association of Cumulative Lead and Neurocognitive Function in an Occupational Cohort. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(1):10–19. doi: 10.1037/a0013757. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kishiyama MM, Boyce WT, Jimenez AM, Perry LM, Knight RT. Socioeconomic Disparities Affect Prefrontal Function in Children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21(6):1106–1115. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kling JR, Liebman JB, Katz LF. Experimental analysis of neighborhood effects. Econometrica. 2007;75(1):83–119. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0262.2007.00733.x. [Google Scholar]
- Landrigan PJ, Claudio L, Markowitz SB, Berkowitz GS, Brenner BL, Romero H, Wolff MS. Pesticides and inner-city children: Exposures, risks, and prevention. Environ Health Perspect. 1999;107:431–437. doi: 10.1289/ehp.99107s3431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanphear BP, Hornung R, Khoury J, Yolton K, Baghurstl P, Bellinger DC, Roberts R. Low-level environmental lead exposure and children's intellectual function: An international pooled analysis. Environ Health Perspect. 2005;113(7):894–899. doi: 10.1289/ehp.7688. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.7688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larkin W, Morrison AP. Trauma and psychosis: new directions for theory and therapy. London, New York: Routledge; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Lynam DR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Wikstrom POH, Loeber R, Novak S. The interaction between impulsivity and neighborhood context on offending: The effects of impulsivity are stronger in poorer neighborhoods. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(4):563–574. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.109.4.563. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.109.4.563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch M, Cicchetti D. An ecological-transactional analysis of children and contexts: The longitudinal interplay among child maltreatment, community violence, and children's symptomatology. Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10(2):235–257. doi: 10.1017/s095457949800159x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s095457949800159x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malinoskyrummell R, Hansen DJ. Long-Term Consequences of Childhood Physical Abuse. Psychol Bull. 1993;114(1):68–79. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.114.1.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manly JJ. Deconstructing race and ethnicity - Implications for measurement of health outcomes. Medical Care. 2006;44(11):S10–S16. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000245427.22788.be. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.mlr.0000245427.22788.be. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manly JJ, Echemendia RJ. Race-specific norms: Using the model of hypertension to understand issues of race, culture, and education in neuropsychology. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007;22(3):319–325. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- March D, Hatch SL, Morgan C, Kirkbride JB, Bresnahan M, Fearon P, Susser E. Psychosis and Place. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2008;30(1):84–100. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/epirev/mxn006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez P, Richters JE. The nimh community violence project Childrens distress symptoms associated with violence exposure. Psychiatry-Interpersonal and Biological Processes. 1993;56(1):22–35. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1993.11024618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maughan A, Cicchetti D. Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on children's emotion regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment. Child Dev. 2002;73(5):1525–1542. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00488. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNulty TL, Bellair PE. Explaining racial and ethnic differences in serious adolescent violent behavior. Criminology. 2003;41(3):709–748. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2003.tb01002.x. [Google Scholar]
- Molnar BE, Buka SL, Brennan RT, Holton JK, Earls F. A multilevel study of neighborhoods and parent-to-child physical aggression: results from the project on human development in Chicago neighborhoods. Child maltreatment. 2003;8(2):84–97. doi: 10.1177/1077559502250822. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1077559502250822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morenoff JD, House JS, Hansen BB, Williams DR, Kaplan GA, Hunte HE. Understanding social disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control: The role of neighborhood context. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;65(9):1853–1866. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.038. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morenoff JD, Lynch JW. What makes a place healthy? Neighborhood influences on racial/ethnic disparities in health over life course. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Morenoff JD, Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW. Neighborhood inequality, collective efficacy, and the spatial dynamics of urban violence. Criminology. 2001;39(3):517–559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2001.tb00932.x. [Google Scholar]
- Morgan C, Kirkbride J, Hutchinson G, Craig T, Morgan K, Dazzan P, Fearon P. Cumulative social disadvantage, ethnicity and first-episode psychosis: A case-control study. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38(12):1701–1715. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291708004534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oakes JM. The (mis)estimation of neighborhood effects: causal inference for a practicable social epidemiology. Social Science & Medicine. 2004;58(10):1929–1952. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.08.004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osypuk TL, Schmidt NM, Bates LM, Tchetgen-Tchetgen EJ, Earls FJ, Glymour MM. Gender and Crime Victimization Modify Neighborhood Effects on Adolescent Mental Health. Pediatrics. 2012;130(3):472–481. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-2535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osypuk TL, Tchetgen EJT, Acevedo-Garcia D, Earls FJ, Lincoln A, Schmidt NM, Glymour MM. Differential Mental Health Effects of Neighborhood Relocation Among Youth in Vulnerable Families Results From a Randomized Trial. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69(12):1284–1294. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson RD, Krivo LJ, Hagan J. The many colors of crime: inequalities of race, ethnicity, and crime in America. New York: New York University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Sampson RJ. Ecometrics: Toward a Science of Assessing Ecological Settings, With Application to the Systematic Social Observation of Neighborhoods. Sociological Methodology. 1999;29(1):1–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0081-1750.00059. [Google Scholar]
- Rice D, Barone S. Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: Evidence from humans and animal models. Environ Health Perspect. 2000;108:511–533. doi: 10.1289/ehp.00108s3511. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3454543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts R, O'Connor T, Dunn J, Golding J, Team AS. The effects of child sexual abuse in later family life; mental health, parenting and adjustment of offspring. Child Abuse Negl. 2004;28(5):525–545. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.07.006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose G. Sick Individuals and Sick Populations. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1985;14(1):32–38. doi: 10.1093/ije/14.1.32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/14.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salzinger S, Feldman RS, Stockhammer T, Hood J. An ecological framework for understanding risk for exposure to community violence and the effects of exposure on children and adolescents. Aggression and Violent Behavior. 2002;7(5):423–451. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1359-1789(01)00078-7. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ. Moving to inequality: Neighborhood effects and experiments meet social structure. American Journal of Sociology. 2008;114(1):189–231. doi: 10.1086/589843. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/589843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Laub JH. Crime in the making: Pathways and turning points through life. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Earls F. Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review. 1999;64(5):633–660. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2657367. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Gannon-Rowley T. Assessing “neighborhood effects”: Social processes and new directions in research. Annual Review of Sociology. 2002;28:443–478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.28.110601.141114. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Raudenbush S. Social anatomy of racial and ethnic disparities in violence. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(2):224–232. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.037705. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2004.037705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW. Systematic social observation of public spaces: A new look at disorder in urban neighborhoods. American Journal of Sociology. 1999;105(3):603–651. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/210356. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997a;277(5328):918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW, Earls F. Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science. 1997b;277(5328):918–924. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.918. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.277.5328.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneiders J, Drukker M, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC, van Os J, Nicolson NA. Neighbourhood socioeconomic disadvantage and behavioural problems from late childhood into early adolescence. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2003;57(9):699–703. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.9.699. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.57.9.699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulz A, Williams D, Israel B, Becker A, Parker E, James SA, Jackson J. Unfair treatment, neighborhood effects, and mental health in the Detroit metropolitan area. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2000;41(3):314–332. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2676323. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Health and Human Service. Mental Health Culture, Race, and Ethnicity: A Supplement to Mental Health. A Report of the Surgeon General; Rockville (MD): 2001. Mental Health: Culture, Race, and Ethnicity: A Supplement to Mental Health A Report of the Surgeon General. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaw CR, McKay HD. Juvenile delinquency and urban areas; a study of rates of delinquency in relation to differential characteristics of local communities in American cities. Rev. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Shih RA, Hu H, Weisskopf MG, Schwartz BS. Cumulative lead dose and cognitive function in adults: A review of studies that measured both blood lead and bone lead. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;115(3):483–492. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9786. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.9786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silk JS, Sessa FM, Morris AS, Steinberg L, Avenevoli S. Neighborhood cohesion as a buffer against hostile maternal parenting. Journal of Family Psychology. 2004;18(1):135–146. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver E. Race, neighborhood disadvantage, and violence among persons with mental disorders: The importance of contextual measurement. Law and Human Behavior. 2000;24(4):449–456. doi: 10.1023/a:1005544330132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1005544330132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver E. Guest editorial: Neighborhood social disorganization as a cofactor in violence among people with mental disorders. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 2001;45(4):403–406. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0306624x01454001. [Google Scholar]
- Silver E, Mulvey EP, Monahan J. Assessing violence risk among discharged psychiatric patients: toward an ecological approach. Law Hum Behav. 1999;23(2):237–255. doi: 10.1023/a:1022377003150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sjoquist DL. The Atlanta paradox. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Stafford M, Gimeno D, Marmot MG. Neighbourhood characteristics and trajectories of health functioning: a multilevel prospective analysis. European Journal of Public Health. 2008;18(6):604–610. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckn091. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckn091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele CM, Aronson J. Stereotype Threat and The Intellectual Test-Performance of African-Americans. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(5):797–811. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoll M, Raphael S. Modest progress: The narrowing mismatch between Blacks and jobs in the 1990s. Washington, DC: The Brookings Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Surkan PJ, Zhang A, Trachtenberg F, Daniel DB, McKinlay S, Bellinger DC. Neuropsychological function in children with blood lead levels < 10 mu g/dL. Neurotoxicology. 2007;28(6):1170–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2007.07.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Truong KD, Ma S. A systematic review of relations between neighborhoods and mental health. Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics. 2006;9(3):137–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkheimer E, Haley A, Waldron M, D'Onofrio B, Gottesman II. Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychol Sci. 2003;14(6):623–628. doi: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Kolk BA, McFarlane AC, Weisæth L. Traumatic stress: the effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society. New York: Guilford Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Veling W, Selten JP, Susser E, Laan W, Mackenbach JP, Hoek HW. Discrimination and the incidence of psychotic disorders among ethnic minorities in The Netherlands. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;36(4):761–768. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym085. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/dym085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veling W, Susser E, van Os J, Mackenbach JP, Selten JP, Hoek HW. Ethnic density of Neighborhoods and incidence of psychotic disorders among immigrants. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165(1):66–73. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss B, Bellinger DC. Social ecology of children's vulnerability to environmental pollutants. Environ Health Perspect. 2006;114(10):1479–1485. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1289/ehp.9101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss L, Ompad D, Galea S, Vlahov D. Defining neighborhood boundaries for urban health research. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2007;32(6):S154–S159. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.02.034. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2007.02.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Neighbors HW, Jackson JS. Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: Findings from community studies. Am J Public Health. 2003;93(2):200–208. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.2.200. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/ajph.93.2.200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson JQ, Kelling GL. “Broken Windows” Atlantic Monthly 1982 2011 [Google Scholar]
- Wilson WJ. When work disappears: the world of the new urban poor. 1st. New York: Knopf: Distributed by Random House Inc.; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Woods GW, Freedman D, Greenspan S. Neurobehavioral assessment in forensic practice. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 2012;35(5-6):432–439. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.09.014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright JP, Dietrich KN, Ris MD, Hornung RW, Wessel SD, Lanphear BP, Rae MN. Association of prenatal and childhood blood lead concentrations with criminal arrests in early adulthood. Plos Medicine. 2008;5(5):732–740. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0050101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xue YG, Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J, Earls FJ. Neighborhood residence and mental health problems of 5-to 11-year-olds. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(5):554–563. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]