While it is apparent that all humans suffer decreased performance on many cognitive tasks with sleep deprivation, there is tremendous inter-individual variability in the magnitude of the impairment.1–3 The amount of impairment for each individual is specific to the type of cognitive task,1–3 and only a minor proportion of the variability is explained by baseline task performance prior to sleep deprivation.1 The amount of susceptibility is a trait-like feature of the individual, which appears to be heritable4 and highly reproducible over time.1,2,5 Despite efforts to identify physiological, behavioral or genetic determinants that modify the response to sleep deprivation, relatively little is known about the molecular mechanisms that underlie these individual differences.5–7
The goal of the study reported in this issue of SLEEP by Arnardottir, Nikonova, et al.8 was to determine whether subjects that are differentially sensitive to the effects of sleep deprivation had differential gene expression patterns in blood. The authors selected 7 resistant and 7 sensitive subjects from a prior study,4 based on their performance on the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT)9 following acute total sleep deprivation. The PVT is a robust measure for this purpose because it tests for vigilant attention, which is particularly sensitive to sleep deprivation, and performance on the PVT is not influenced by subject aptitude, learning or psychometric bias.10 Blood was sampled every 4 hours to measure gene expression using microarray during 24 hours of baseline sleep/wake assessment, 38 hours of continuous wakefulness (i.e., sleep deprivation), and a night of recovery sleep. The expression of 18,983 unique genes was measured at 19 time points for each subject, producing a large and comprehensive dataset of gene expression changes over time. Gene expression changes were compared between the sensitive and resistant subjects at baseline, after sleep deprivation and during recovery. The sensitive and resistant subjects had no difference in PVT performance or gene expression patterns under normal sleep/wake conditions, and both groups made increased PVT lapses with sleep deprivation, but as expected, the magnitude of these lapses was significantly greater in the group of sensitive subjects.
Surprisingly, very few genes altered their expression in a state-specific or linear fashion as a result of sleep deprivation. Previous studies have also only found a limited number of gene expression changes in blood during sleep deprivation, and the magnitude of the expression change is small and variable,11,12 even within an individual, suggesting that these findings are subjected to a large degree of statistical noise. Importantly, there were no differences in state-specific or linear gene expression changes between the sensitive and resistant subject groups. Taken together, these data argue that in humans there is not a straightforward gene expression response to sleep deprivation that explains the differential susceptibility between subjects, at least when gene expression is measured in pooled blood cells. The identification of a universal molecular signature (a gene or set of genes) of differential susceptibility will be challenging, especially given the mixture of cell types in blood, differences in expression patterns in the nervous system vs the periphery, the impact of stress on gene expression profiles after sleep deprivation,13 and the fact that inter-individual differences in sleep deprivation-induced deficits are task-specific,1–3 rather than global individual-specific deficits.
However, differences between the resistant and susceptible groups became apparent when the authors looked at changes to the daily pattern of gene expression. During normal sleep/ wake, they found 4,481 unique genes (23.6% of the genes they assessed) with a significant 24-h rhythm in expression pattern. Based on an important prior finding that sleep restriction causes a reduction in the number of genes with a circadian expression profile,11 the authors specifically compared the expression profile of the rhythmic genes. Resistant subjects had a significant reduction in the amplitude of the daily expression pattern, resulting in a reduced number of genes with a significant rhythmic expression profile, while sensitive subjects had no change in the amplitude or number of rhythmically expressed genes during sleep deprivation.
This finding suggests that behaviorally resistant subjects were able to respond to extended wakefulness by significantly dampening oscillating expression patterns. Subjects that were behaviorally sensitive appear to suffer a transcriptional resistance to the changed internal milieu; they failed to make the necessary molecular adaptations, and continued to express genes in a daily fashion as though they were still on a normal sleep/wake cycle. A recent elegant study showed that desynchrony between sleep and circadian phase (i.e., mistimed sleep) also dampens the amplitude of rhythmic changes in blood gene expression.14 While circadian regulation of the transcriptome allows for specialized cellular functions to optimally match time of day and sleep/wake state, this specialization likely impairs vigilance quality and cognitive functioning when it orchestrates specific cellular, molecular and metabolic processes to occur during sleep. Turning down this specialization (i.e., dampening the circadian regulation to better maintain the “waking molecular signature”) may be advantageous in unanticipated sleep/ wake states and individuals resistant to sleep deprivation may be able to do this more efficiently than sensitive individuals.
At the moment the relationship between a dampened circadian rhythm in gene expression and resistance to sleep loss is only correlational. Testing whether enhancing or dampening circadian rhythmicity of the transcriptome will produce changes in neurobehavioral impairments after sleep deprivation in model systems is required. In addition, the role of specific genes in this process remains unclear. The authors looked specifically at changes in the core clock genes and found no differences at baseline, but did not report on the potential difference between sensitive and resistant subjects in clock gene expression amplitude after sleep deprivation. These molecular elements are attractive candidates because of their role in sleep homeostasis15,16 and in modulating the sensitivity to sleep deprivation.17–19 Expression data from this study will be publically available and can be revisited to identify potential candidate genes and importantly, validate these microarray results with replication and qPCR. Screening for genes involved in epi-genetic regulation would also be interesting because they are known to respond to sleep timing14 and sleep deprivation,20 and play major roles in regulating gene expression and cognitive function.
CITATION
Warby SC, Mongrain V. Resisting sleep deprivation by breaking the link between sleep and circadian rhythms. SLEEP 2014;37(10):1581-1582.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors have indicated no financial conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Van Dongen HP, Baynard MD, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. Sleep. 2004;27:423–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Leproult R, Colecchia EF, Berardi AM, Stickgold R, Kosslyn SM, Van Cauter E. Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2003;284:R280–90. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00197.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Frey DJ, Badia P, Wright KP., Jr Inter- and intra-individual variability in performance near the circadian nadir during sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2004;13:305–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00429.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kuna ST, Maislin G, Pack FM, Staley B, Hachadoorian R, Coccaro EF, Pack AI. Heritability of performance deficit accumulation during acute sleep deprivation in twins. Sleep. 2012;35:1223–33. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Basner M, Rao H, Goel N, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and neurobehavioral dynamics. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23:854–63. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.King AC, Belenky G, Van Dongen HP. Performance impairment consequent to sleep loss: determinants of resistance and susceptibility. Curr Opin Pulm Med. 2009;15:559–64. doi: 10.1097/MCP.0b013e3283319aad. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Goel N, Dinges DF. Predicting risk in space: genetic markers for differential vulnerability to sleep restriction. Acta Astronaut. 2012;77:207–13. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2012.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Arnardottir ES, Nikonova EV, Shockley KR, et al. Blood-gene expression reveals reduced circadian rhythmicity in individuals resistant to sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2014;37:1589–600. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011;34:581–91. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:305–22. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Moller-Levet CS, Archer SN, Bucca G, et al. Effects of insufficient sleep on circadian rhythmicity and expression amplitude of the human blood transcriptome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:E1132–41. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1217154110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pellegrino R, Sunaga DY, Guindalini C, et al. Whole blood genome-wide gene expression profile in males after prolonged wakefulness and sleep recovery. Physiol Genomics. 2012;44:1003–12. doi: 10.1152/physiolgenomics.00058.2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mongrain V, Hernandez SA, Pradervand S, et al. Separating the contribution of glucocorticoids and wakefulness to the molecular and electrophysiological correlates of sleep homeostasis. Sleep. 2010;33:1147–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.9.1147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Archer SN, Laing EE, Moller-Levet CS, et al. Mistimed sleep disrupts circadian regulation of the human transcriptome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111:E682–91. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1316335111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mongrain V, La Spada F, Curie T, Franken P. Sleep loss reduces the DNA-binding of BMAL1, CLOCK, and NPAS2 to specific clock genes in the mouse cerebral cortex. PLoS One. 2011;6:e26622. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Franken P, Dijk DJ. Circadian clock genes and sleep homeostasis. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;29:1820–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06723.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Viola AU, Archer SN, James LM, et al. PER3 polymorphism predicts sleep structure and waking performance. Curr Biol. 2007;17:613–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.He Y, Jones CR, Fujiki N, et al. The transcriptional repressor DEC2 regulates sleep length in mammals. Science. 2009;325:866–70. doi: 10.1126/science.1174443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pellegrino R, Kavakli IH, Goel N, et al. A novel BHLHE41 variant is associated with short sleep and resistance to sleep deprivation in humans. Sleep. 2014;37:1327–36. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Massart R, Freyburger M, Suderman M, et al. The genome-wide landscape of DNA methylation and hydroxymethylation in response to sleep deprivation impacts on synaptic plasticity genes. Transl Psychiatry. 2014;4:e347. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]