Abstract
This is the protocol for a review and there is no abstract. The objectives are as follows:
To evaluate the potential beneficial and adverse effects of psychological interventions for people with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder and to make recommendations for future areas of research.
BACKGROUND
Description of the condition
Obsessive-compulsive personality disorder is a subcategory of personality disorder. The current edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; APA 2000) defines personality disorder as: “an enduring pattern of inner experience and behaviour that deviates markedly from the expectations of the person’s culture, is pervasive and inflexible, has an onset in adolescence or early adulthood, is stable over time, and leads to distress or impairment”.
Obsessive-compulsive personality disorder is identified by traits that include perfectionism, rigidity and stubbornness, and miserliness. It is diagnosed, according to DSM-IV-TR (APA 2000), as follows. Individuals demonstrate a pervasive pattern of preoccupation with orderliness, perfectionism, and mental and interpersonal control, at the expense of flexibility, openness and efficiency, beginning by early adulthood and present in a variety of contexts, as indicated by four (or more) of the following:
is preoccupied with details, rules, lists, order, organisation or schedules to the extent that the major point of the activity is lost;
shows perfectionism that interferes with task completion (e.g. is unable to complete a project because his or her own overly strict standards are not met);
is excessively devoted to work and productivity to the exclusion of leisure activities and friendships (not accounted for by obvious economic necessity);
is over conscientious, scrupulous and inflexible about matters of morality, ethics or values (not accounted for by cultural or religious identification);
is unable to discard worn-out or worthless objects even when they have no sentimental value;
is reluctant to delegate tasks or to work with others unless they submit to exactly his or her way of doing things;
adopts a miserly spending style toward both self and others; money is viewed as something to be hoarded for future catastrophes;
shows rigidity and stubbornness.
The tenth revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) refers to this disorder as ‘anankastic personality’ (WHO 1992).
According to DSM-IV-TR (APA 2000) the prevalence of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder has been estimated from community samples to be around 1% and from clinical samples around 3% to 10%. Samuels 2002 calculated the prevalence of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder using DSM-IV criteria in a community sample to be 1.2%. As is the case with many other personality disorders, the prevalence of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder is generally higher in clinical populations (Zimmerman 2005). Those most likely to receive a diagnosis are white, married, employed males (Nestadt 1991).
Nigg 1994 noted in their reviews that while evidence concerning the inheritance of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder is mixed, some research suggests that trait obsessiveness in the normal range is moderately heritable. The condition is associated with other Axis II personality disorders, such as paranoid, avoidant and borderline personality disorder (Pfohl B 1995). However, Zimmerman 2005 found an elevated odds ratio of comorbidity with obsessive compulsive personality disorder for paranoid, schizoid and narcissistic but not borderline personality disorder. A common question in the literature on obsessive-compulsive personality disorder concerns the nature of its relationship to the similarly named obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (DSM-IV and ICD-10). The classic distinction between these disorders is that obsessions and compulsions in OCD are thought to be ego-dystonic (i.e. perceived as originating from outside the self or unacceptable to the self) whereas obsessive-compulsive personality disorder character traits are thought to be ego-syntonic (i.e. perceived as originating from within the self and consistent with and acceptable to the self) (Pollak 1987; Stein 1993). These boundaries are not always firm, however these two disorders are generally regarded as separate and distinct (Stein 1993).
According to DSM-IV-TR (APA 2000), individuals with anxiety disorders, including social phobia and specific phobia, have an increased likelihood of meeting the criteria for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. In addition, OCD and eating disorders, anorexia nervosa in particular, have received special attention re garding their relationship to obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. Some research suggests that obsessive-compulsive personality disorder traits may predispose people to develop an eating disorder. For example, studies investigating the co-morbidity between obsessive-compulsive personality disorder and eating disorders have yielded estimates ranging from 3.3% to 60% (Serpell 2002).
Finally, personality disorders such as obsessive-compulsive personality disorder are also a significant source of psychiatric morbidity, accounting for more impairment in functioning than major depressive disorder alone (Skodol 2002).
Description of the intervention
Psychological therapies encompass a wide range of interventions but for this review may be broadly classified into three main categories:
Behavioural interventions, including cognitive behavioural therapy and classical behaviour therapy.
Psychodynamic therapy.
Interventions specifically developed for other personality disorders, or that have been found to be effective for personality disorders, such as dialectical behaviour therapy, therapeutic community therapy and nidotherapy.
Psychological interventions have been the mainstay of treatment for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. Some of the interventions can be administered in the form of individual or group-based therapy, or both. Drugs may also be given as an adjunctive intervention.
How the intervention might work
Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) based treatments place emphasis on encouraging the patient to challenge their core beliefs. A review of the evidence for this form of intervention concluded that “the overall evidence in favour of cognitive behavioural therapy in the treatment of personality disorder is therefore relatively slim, with much of the evidence coming from one research group, but it has involved more patients than any other form of treatment” (Bateman 2004). Research on the effectiveness of CBT for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder is mixed (Beck 2004).
Psychodynamic therapies (which include analytic psychotherapy, transference-focused psychotherapy and group psychotherapy) aim to help the patient understand and reflect on his or her inner mental processes and make links between past and current difficulties. Very little research has been published assessing the efficacy of dynamic psychotherapies specifically for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. One of the prominent studies was done by Winston 1994 and involved an active and confrontational approach (short-term dynamic and brief adaptional psychotherapy). Other studies, showing limited evidence for the efficacy of psychodynamic psychotherapy, come from Bateman 2001, Barber 1997, Barber 1996, Winston 1994 and Piper 1993. Despite this lack of evidence, insight-oriented, dynamic therapies, including psychoanalysis, have been recommended as the treatment of choice for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (Baer 1990; Jenkie 1998; Salzman 1980; Sperry 2003; Stein 1993).
Dialectical behavioural therapy (DBT) is a complex psychological intervention which was developed using some of the principles of CBT (Linehan 1993) and may help change behaviour by improving skills and the ability to contain difficult feelings. It is currently popular, but the evidence for its efficacy is less clear with some reviewers considering that its only proven benefit appears to be in the reduction of self-harm episodes (Bateman 2004). Cognitive analytic therapy (CAT) is a brief psychological therapy utilising ideas from psychodynamic psychotherapy, cognitive therapy and cognitive psychology (Denman 2001).
Therapeutic community treatments involve patients (also known as residents) not only having therapy together but also working and living together in a shared, therapeutic environment. This provides them with an opportunity to “explore intrapsychic and interpersonal problems and find more constructive ways of dealing with distress” (Campling 2001). Therapeutic community treatment is the only single-treatment modality for severe personality disorder that has been subject to a meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. This demonstrated the effectiveness of the treatment (Lees 1999).
Nidotherapy is a formalised, planned method for achieving environmental change to minimise the effect of the patient’s disorder upon themselves and others. The effectiveness of this treatment has not yet been established. Unlike most other therapies it aims to fit the immediate environment to the patient rather than change the patient to cope with the existing environment (Tyrer 2007). The evidence for the effectiveness of group therapy in obsessive-compulsive personality disorder is mixed. Wells 1990 supports group situations for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder individuals, allowing them to share power and relinquish control, suggesting that they may be more receptive to peer feedback. However, others have noted that obsessive-compulsive personality disorder individuals might resist group therapy or can be disruptive (Millon 1996; Philips 1994).
Turkat 1985 and Sperry 2003 argue that obsessive-compulsive personality disorder individuals may benefit from social skills training, especially in associated interpersonal problems.
Why it is important to do this review
Obsessive-compulsive personality disorder is an important condition that has a considerable impact on individuals and families. Although psychodynamic therapy has been considered as the treatment of choice for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, there are relatively few well-controlled studies establishing the efficacy of this approach. Indeed as Barber 1997 noted there has been very little research investigating any form of treatment for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder.
Personality disorders are a significant source of psychiatric morbidity, accounting for more impairment in functioning than major depressive disorder (Skodol 2002). In addition, Newton-Howes 2006 reported that combined depression and personality disorder is associated with a poorer outcome than depression alone.
This is a neglected area of research, as pointed out by Grilo 2004, which has received little empirical attention. We hope a Cochrane Review of psychological treatments for obsessive-compulsive personality disorder will highlight areas where more work is needed and hopefully stimulate research interest.
OBJECTIVES
To evaluate the potential beneficial and adverse effects of psychological interventions for people with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder and to make recommendations for future areas of research.
METHODS
Criteria for considering studies for this review
Types of studies
Controlled trials in which participants have been randomly allocated to an experimental group and a control group, where the control condition is either treatment as usual, waiting list or no treatment. We will include all relevant randomised controlled trials, with or without blinding.
Types of participants
Men or women, 18 years old or over, with a diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive personality disorder defined by any operational criteria, such as DSM-IV or ICD10. We will include studies of people diagnosed with co-morbid personality disorders or other mental health problems, other than the major functional mental illnesses (i.e. schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder or bipolar disorder). The decision to exclude persons with co-morbid major functional illness is based on the rationale that the presence of such disorders (and the possible confounding effects of any associated management or treatment) might obscure whatever other psychopathology (including personality disorder) might be present.
Types of interventions
We will include studies of psychological interventions, both group and individual-based, that meet the criteria of the categories of psychological interventions listed above.
We will sub-classify psychological interventions into single-modality and complex psychological interventions. Single-modality psychological interventions are those that only involve one specific type of intervention. Such interventions include cognitive analytic therapy.
Complex psychological interventions are those that involve more than one modality of treatment (for example, group therapy plus individual therapy) and include dialectical behaviour therapy and psychodynamic psychotherapy with partial hospitalisation (Campbell 2000).
We will include studies of psychological interventions where medication is given as an adjunctive intervention.
We will not include studies comparing two or more different therapeutic modality groups but without a control group.
Types of outcome measures
Primary and secondary outcomes are listed below in terms of single constructs. We anticipate that a range of outcome measures will have been used in the studies included in the review (for example, obsession may be measured by a self-report instrument or by an external observer). We will include outcomes that are measured at post-treatment or follow-up periods (or both) of up to six months, six to 12 months, and more than 12 months.
Primary outcomes
Recovery, as measured by obsessive-compulsive symptom levels. We will accept validated clinician-rated scales such as the National Institute of Mental Health Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (NIMH-OCS) (CCSG 1991), or self-rating scales such as the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS) (Goodman 1989) and the Maudsley Obsessive Compulsive Inventory (MOCI) (Hodgson 1977).
Adverse events, measured as incidence of overall adverse events and of the three most common adverse events(dichotomous outcome, measured as numbers reporting).
In addition we will use the following outcome measures to assess social functioning:
Global state/functioning(continuous outcome), measured through improvement on the Global Assessment of Functioning numeric scale (GAF; APA 2000) or Clinical Global Impression of Severity scale (CGI-S).
Social functioning(continuous outcome), measured through improvement in scores on the Social Adjustment Scale (SAS-SR; Weissman 1994), the Social Functioning Questionnaire (SFQ; Tyrer 2005) or a similar validated instrument or scale producing a composite score of severity of overall burden, such as the Symptom Checklist-90 (revised) (SCL-90R).
Quality of life, using measures such as the SF36 (Ware 1993) or European Quality of Life instrument (EuroQoL Group 1990)).
Secondary outcomes
Participant discontinuation rates starting from the point of randomised allocation.
Depressive symptoms (using validated scales such as the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAMD) (Hamilton 1969) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck 1961).
Anxiety symptoms (using validated scales such as the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAMA) (Hamilton 1959), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (Spielberg 1983) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck 1988).
Absence of treatment response (score of ‘not improved’ or ‘little improved’) or treatment response (score of ‘very much improved’ or ‘much improved’ on all scales).
Satisfaction with treatment (using the Client Satisfaction Questionnaire (CSQ-8, Attkisson 1982) or similar validated instruments).
Search methods for identification of studies
Electronic searches
We will search the following electronic databases: the Cochrane Developmental, Psychosocial and Learning Problems Group Register, the Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL) (The Cochrane Library, current issue); MEDLINE (1950 to current); EMBASE (1980 to current); CINAHL (1982 to current); PsycINFO (1872 to current); the Cochrane Schizophrenia Group register of trials related to Forensic Mental Health; ASSIA; BIOSIS; COPAC; Dissertation Abstracts; IBSSISI-Proceedings; ISI-SCI (Science Citation Index); ISI-SSCI (Social Sciences Citation Index); OpenSIGLE; Sociological Abstracts; ZETOC; UK Clinical Trials Gateway*; ClinicalTrials.gov*; Action Medical Research*; King’s College London (UK)*; ISRCTN Register*; The Wellcome Trust Register*; NHS Trusts Clinical Trials Register*; NHS R&D Health Technology Assessment Programme Register (HTA)*; and NHS R&D Regional Programmes Register* (*will be searched using the metaRegister of Controlled Trials (http://www.controlled-trials.com/mrct/)).
The search terms for MEDLINE are shown Appendix 1. We will search MEDLINE in combination with the Cochrane Collaboration’s search strategy for identifying reports of controlled trials as detailed in Section 6.4.11 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2008). Similar strategies to identify participants and controlled trials will be developed for the other databases.
Searching other resources
Handsearching
We will search the reference lists of included and excluded studies for additional relevant trials. We will also examine bibliographies of systematic review articles published in the last five years to identify relevant studies.
Requests for additional data
We will contact authors of relevant studies to enquire about other sources of information and the first author of each included study for information regarding unpublished data. We will contact a representative from all major pharmaceutical companies to request information about any published/unpublished trials.
Data collection and analysis
Selection of studies
This review is part of a larger series of reviews of personality disorders, therefore we will carry out selection of studies in two stages. In the first stage, two authors (JS and NH) will read titles and abstracts independently by and assess these against the inclusion criteria to identify all studies carried out in participants with personality disorder, regardless of any specific personality disorder(s) diagnosed. In the second stage, two independent authors (BV and SG) will assess full copies of studies identified in stage one against the inclusion criteria. This second stage assessment will identify not only trials with participants diagnosed with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, but also trials with participants having a mix of personality disorders for which data on a subgroup with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder may be available.
We will resolve uncertainties concerning the appropriateness of studies for inclusion in the review through consultation with a third author (CD). Authors will not be blinded to the name(s) of the study author(s), their institution(s) or publication sources at any stage of the review.
Data extraction and management
Two authors (HJ and RA) will extract data independently using a data extraction form. We will enter data into RevMan 5 (RevMan 2008). Where data are not available in the published trial reports, we will contact the authors and ask them to supply the missing information.
Assessment of risk of bias in included studies
For each included study, two authors (HJ and MF) will independently complete the Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias (Higgins 2008, section 8.5.1). Any disagreement will be resolved through consultation with a third author (RA). We will assess the degree to which:
the allocation sequence was adequately generated (sequence generation);
the allocation was adequately concealed (allocation concealment);
knowledge of the allocated interventions was adequately prevented during the study (blinding);
incomplete outcome data were adequately addressed;
reports of the study were free of suggestion of selective outcome reporting; and
the study was apparently free of other problems that could put it at high risk of bias.
We will allocate each domain one of three possible categories for each of the included studies: ‘Yes’ for low risk of bias, ‘No’ for high risk of bias, and ‘Unclear’ where the risk of bias is uncertain or unknown.
Measures of treatment effect
For dichotomous (binary) data, we will use the odds ratio (OR) with a 95% confidence interval (CI) to summarise results within each study. The odds ratio is chosen because it has statistical advantages relating to its sampling distribution and its suitability for modelling, and it is a relative measure so can be used to combine studies. For continuous data, such as the measurement of impulsiveness and aggression on a scale, we will compare the mean score for each outcome as determined by a standardised tool between the two groups to give a mean difference (MD), again with a 95% CI. Where possible, we will make these comparisons at specific follow-up periods: (1) within the first month, (2) between one and six months, and (3) between six and 12 months. Where possible, we will present endpoint data. Where both endpoint and change data are available for the same outcomes, then we will only report the former.
We will report continuous data that are skewed in a separate table, and will not calculate treatment effect sizes to minimise the risk of applying parametric statistics to data that depart significantly from a normal distribution. We define skewedness as occurring when, for a scale or measure with positive values and a minimum value of zero, the mean is less than twice the standard deviation (Altman 1996).
We will use the weighted mean difference (WMD) where the same outcome measures are reported in more than one study. We will use the standardised mean difference (SMD) where different outcome measures of the same construct are reported.
Unit of analysis issues
Cluster-randomised trials
Where trials have used cluster-randomisation, we anticipate that study investigators will have presented their results after appropriately controlling for clustering effects (robust standard errors or hierarchical linear models). If it is unclear whether a cluster-randomised trial has used appropriate controls for clustering, we will contact the study investigators for further information. Where appropriate controls were not used, we will request individual participant data and re-analysed data using multilevel models which control for clustering. Following this, we will meta-analyse effect sizes and standard errors in RevMan using the generic inverse method (Higgins 2008). If appropriate controls were not used and individual participant data are not available, we will seek statistical guidance from the relevant Cochrane Methods Group and external experts as to which method to apply to the published results in attempt to control for clustering. If there is insufficient information to control for clustering, we will enter outcome data into RevMan using individuals as the units of analysis, and then use sensitivity analysis to assess the potential biasing effects of inadequately controlled clustered trials (Donner 2001).
Cross-over trials
When conducting a meta-analysis combining the results of cross-over trials, we will use the inverse variance methods recommended by Elbourne (Elbourne 2002). Where data presented from a cross-over trial are restricted (and more information is not available from the original investigators) we will use the presented data within the first phase only, up to the point of cross-over.
Multi-arm trials
We will include all eligible outcome measures for all trial arms in this review. Where there are more than two arms of the trial that meet the inclusion criteria, two or more intervention arms and/or two or more control group arms, there are a number approaches that can be used. These include combining arms to create one single pair-wise comparison; selecting one pair for comparison only; splitting the ‘shared’ group into two or more groups to provide two or more comparisons; including two or more correlated comparisons or undertaking a multiple-treatment meta-analysis. In the event of a multi-arm trial meeting our inclusion criteria we will seek statistical advice on which of these options is the most appropriate for the study.
Dealing with missing data
We will contact the original investigators to request any missing data and information on whether or not data can be assumed to be missing at random.
For dichotomous data we will report missing data and drop-outs for each included study and will report the number of participants who are included in the final analysis as a proportion of all participants in each study. We will provide reasons for missing data in the narrative summary and will assess the extent to which the results of the review could be altered by the missing data by, for example, a sensitivity analysis based on consideration of ‘best-case’ and ‘worst-case’ scenarios (Gamble 2005). Here, the ‘best-case’ scenario is that where all participants with missing outcomes in the experimental condition had good outcomes, and all those with missing outcomes in the control condition had poor outcomes, and the ‘worst-case’ scenario is the converse (Higgins 2008, section 16.2.2).
For missing continuous data, we will provide a qualitative summary. The standard deviations of the outcome measures should be reported for each group in each trial. If these are not given, we will impute standard deviations using relevant data (for example, standard deviations or correlation coefficients) from other, similar studies (Follmann 1992) but only if, after seeking statistical advice, to do so is deemed practical and appropriate.
Where missing data might be assumed to be missing at random then it may be appropriate to analyse only the available data. Where it may not be possible to assume this, i.e. not missing at random, it might be possible to impute missing data with replacement values, for example last observation carried forward. Other methods of imputing missing values with uncertainty or using statistical models will only be used on the advice and with the assistance of a statistician.
Assessment of heterogeneity
We will assess the extent of between-trial differences and the consistency of results of any meta-analysis in three ways: by visual inspection of the forest plots, by performing the Chi2 test of heterogeneity (where a significance level less than 0.10 will be interpreted as evidence of heterogeneity), and by examining the I2 statistic (Higgins 2008; section 9.5.2). The I2 statistic describes approximately the proportion of variation in point estimates due to heterogeneity rather than sampling error. We will consider I2 values of less than 40% as indicating that heterogeneity may not be important, that values in the range 30% to 60% may represent moderate heterogeneity, values in the range 50% to 90% may represent substantial heterogeneity and that 75% to 100% may represent considerable heterogeneity. We will attempt to identify any significant determinants of heterogeneity categorised as moderate or high.
We cannot state in advance what our preferred method of dealing with heterogeneity, if present, will be as this will be contingent on the data. However, we will first check that the data have been correctly entered. There are a number of methods that can be used for handling heterogeneity and these include the following. The studies can be examined to explore the reason for heterogeneity but this is not recommended where there are few studies. A random-effects model could be used. Where a study paper is an outlier, sensitivity analysis can be carried out with and without the study. Other strategies include ignoring the heterogeneity, not performing a meta-analysis and changing the effect measure. We will seek statistical advice before using any of these methods.
Assessment of reporting biases
We will draw funnel plots (effect size versus standard error) to assess publication bias if sufficient studies are found. Asymmetry of the plots may indicate publication bias, although it may also represent a true relationship between trial size and effect size. If such a relationship is identified, we will examine the clinical diversity of the studies further as a possible explanation (Egger 1997).
Data synthesis
We will carry out meta-analysis by intervention type and we will not include different interventions in the same meta-analysis. We will undertake meta-analysis of the data using both fixed and random-effects models. Meta-analyses may be conducted to combine comparable outcome measures across studies. In carrying out meta-analysis, the weight given to each study will be the inverse of the variance so that the more precise estimates (from larger studies with more events) are given more weight. We will group outcome measures by length of follow up.
Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity
If sufficient studies are found, we will undertake subgroup analysis to examine the effect on the primary outcomes of:
participants’ principal diagnosis(e.g. personality disorder, eating disorder, OCD);
setting (inpatient, outpatient/community); and
individual or group-based therapy.
Where we find a number of studies with participants aged less than 18 years, we will perform sensitivity analysis to explore the effect of including/excluding this younger sample.
Sensitivity analysis
If there are sufficient data, we will undertake sensitivity analyses to investigate the robustness of the overall findings in relation to certain study characteristics. We plan a priori sensitivity analyses for:
concealment of allocation;
blinding of outcome assessors; and
extent of drop-outs.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
None.
SOURCES OF SUPPORT
Internal sources
Nottinghamshire Healthcare NHS Trust, UK.
External sources
German Federal Ministry of Education and Research Grant no 01KG0812, Germany.
NHS Cochrane Collaboration Programme Grant Scheme, UK.
Appendix 1. MEDLINE search strategy
The search terms for MEDLINE will be as follows:
exp personality disorders/
exp anankastic personality disorder
exp obsessive compulsive personality disorder/
exp compulsive personality disorder/
exp anal character/
exp passive-aggressive personality disorder/
obsessive
compulsive
anal retentive or anal character
anankastic
(DSM and (Axis II))
or/1-11
We will search MEDLINE in combination with the Cochrane Collaboration’s search strategy for identifying reports of controlled trials as detailed in Section 6.4.11 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2008).
We will develop similar strategies to identify participants and controlled trials for the other databases.
HISTORY
Protocol first published: Issue 5, 2010
Footnotes
DECLARATIONS OF INTEREST None.
Additional references
- Altman 1996 .Altman DG, Bland JM. Detecting skewedness from summary information. BMJ. 1996;313:1200. doi: 10.1136/bmj.313.7066.1200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- APA 2000 .American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association; Washington DC: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Attkisson 1982 .Attkisson CC, Zwick R. The client satisfaction questionnaire. Psychometric properties and correlations with service utilization and psychotherapy outcome. Evaluation and Program Planning. 1982;5:233–7. doi: 10.1016/0149-7189(82)90074-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baer 1990 .Baer L, Jenike MA, Ricciardi JN, Holland AD, Seymour RJ, Minichiello WE. Standardized assessment of personality disorders in obsessive compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1990;47:826–30. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1990.01810210034005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barber 1996 .Barber JP, Muenz LR. The role of avoidance and obsessiveness in matching patients to cognitive and interpersonal psychotherapy: empirical findings from the treatment for depression collaborative research program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:951–8. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.5.951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barber 1997 .Barber JP, Morse JQ, Krakauer ID, Chittams J, Critis-Christoph K. Change in obsessive-compulsive and avoidant personality disorders following time-limited supportive expressive therapy. Psychotherapy. 1997;34:133–43. [Google Scholar]
- Bateman 2001 .Bateman P, Fonagy P. Treatment of borderline personality disorder with psychoanalytically oriented partial hospitalisation: an 18 month follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158:36–42. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.1.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bateman 2004 .Bateman AW, Tyrer P. Psychological treatment for personality disorders. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment. 2004;10:378–88. [Google Scholar]
- Beck 1961 .Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1961;4:561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck 1988 .Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988;56:893–7. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck 2004 .Beck AT, Freeman A, Davis DD. Cognitive Therapy of Personality Disorders. 2nd Edition Guilford Press; New York: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell 2000 .Campbell M, Fitzpatrick R, Haines A, Kinmouth AL, Sandercock P, Spiegeihalter D, et al. A framework for the design and evaluation of complex interventions to improve health. BMJ. 2000;321:694–6. doi: 10.1136/bmj.321.7262.694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campling 2001 .Campling P. Therapeutic communities. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment. 2001;7:365–72. [Google Scholar]
- CCSG 1991 .Clomipramine Collaborative Study Group Clomipramine in the treatment of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1991;48:730–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810320054008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denman 2001 .Denman C. Cognitive analytical therapy. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment. 2001;7:243–56. [Google Scholar]
- Donner 2001 .Donner A, Piaggio G, Villar J. Statistical methods for the meta-analysis of cluster randomization trials. Statistical Methods in Medical Research. 2001;10(5):325–38. doi: 10.1177/096228020101000502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger 1997 .Egger M, Davey-Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elbourne 2002 .Elbourne DR, Altman DG, Higgins JP, Curtin F, Worthington HV, Vail A. Meta-analyses involving cross-over trials: methodological issues. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;31(1):140–9. doi: 10.1093/ije/31.1.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- EuroQoL Group 1990 .EuroQoL Group EuroQoL: a new facility for measurement of health-related quality of life. Health Policy. 1990;16:199–208. doi: 10.1016/0168-8510(90)90421-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Follmann 1992 .Follmann D, Elliot P, Suh I, Cutler J. Variance imputation for overviews of clinical trials with continuous response. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 1992;45(7):769–73. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(92)90054-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gamble 2005 .Gamble C, Hollis S. Uncertainty method on best-worst case analysis in a binary meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2005;58(6):579–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman 1989 .Goodman WK, Price LH, Rasmussen SA, Mazure C, Fleischmann RL, Hill CL, et al. The Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale 1, development, use and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1989;46:1006–11. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grilo 2004 .Grilo CM. Factor structure of DSM-IV criteria for obsessive compulsive personality disorder in patients with binge eating disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2004;109:64–8. doi: 10.1046/j.0001-690x.2003.00223.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton 1959 .Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. British Journal of Medical Psychology. 1959;32:50–5. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton 1969 .Hamilton M. Standardised assessment and recording depressive symptoms. Psychiatria, Neuralgia, Neurochirurgia. 1969;72(2):201–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins 2008 .Higgins JPT, Green S, editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.0.1 [updated September 2008] The Cochrane Collaboration; 2008. Available from www.cochrane-handbook.org. [Google Scholar]
- Hodgson 1977 .Hodgson RJ, Rachman S. Obsessive-compulsive complaints. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1977;15:389–95. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(77)90042-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkie 1998 .Baer L, Jenkie MA. Obsessive compulsive disorders: practical management. 3rd Edition Mosby; New York: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Lees 1999 .Lees J, Manning N, Rawlings B. Therapeutic effectiveness: a systematic international review of therapeutic community treatment for people with personality disorder and mentally disordered offenders (CRD report 17) University of York NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Linehan 1993 .Linehan MM. The skills training manual for treating borderline personality disorder. Guilford Press; New York: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Millon 1996 .Millon T, Davis RD. Disorders of personality: DSM-IV and beyond. Wiley; New York: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Nestadt 1991 .Nestadt G, Romanoski AJ, Brown CH, Chahal R, Merchant A, Folstein MF, et al. DSM-III compulsive personality disorder: an epidemiological survey. Psychological Medicine. 1991;21:461–71. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700020572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newton-Howes 2006 .Newton-Howes G, Tyrer P, Johnson T. Personality disorder and the outcome of depression: meta-analysis of published studies. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;188:13–20. doi: 10.1192/bjp.188.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nigg 1994 .Nigg JT, Goldsmith HH. Genetics of personality disorders: perspectives from personality and psychopathology research. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:346–80. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfohl B 1995 .Pfohl B, Blum N. Obsessive compulsive personality disorder, the DSM IV personality disorder. Guilford Press; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Philips 1994 .Phillips KA, Gunderson JG. The American Psychiatric Press Textbook of Psychiatry. 2nd Edition American Psychiatric Press; Washington DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Piper 1993 .Piper WE, Rosie JS, Azim HF. A randomised trial of psychiatric day treatment for patients with affective and personality disorders. Hospital and Community Psychiatry. 1993;44:757–63. doi: 10.1176/ps.44.8.757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak 1987 .Pollak JM. Relationship of obsessive-compulsive personality to obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review of the literature. Journal of Psychology. 1987;121:137–48. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1987.9712651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- RevMan 2008 .The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration . Review Manager (RevMan). 5.0. The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration; Copenhagen: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Salzman 1980 .Karasu TB, Bellak L, editors. Specialized techniques in individual psychotherapy. Brunner-Mazel; New York: 1980. Psychotherapy with the obsessive personality. [Google Scholar]
- Samuels 2002 .Samuels J, Eaton WW, Bienvenu OJ, Brown CH, Costa PT, Nestadat G. Prevalence and correlates of personality disorders in a community sample. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;180:536–42. doi: 10.1192/bjp.180.6.536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serpell 2002 .Serpell L, Livingstone A, Neiderman M, Lask B. Anorexia nervosa: obsessive-compulsive disorder, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, or neither? Clinical Psychology Review. 2002;22:647–69. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(01)00112-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skodol 2002 .Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, McGlashan TH, Dyck IR, Stout RL, Bender DS, et al. Functional impairment in patients with schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, or obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:276–83. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperry 2003 .Sperry L. Handbook of diagnosis and treatment of DSM-IV-TR personality disorders. 2nd Edition Brunner-Routledge; New York: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Spielberg 1983 .Spielberg CD, Gorsuch RL, Luchene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto, CA: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Stein 1993 .Stein DJ, Hollander E. The spectrum of obsessive compulsive related disorders. Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disorders. 1993;0:241–62. [Google Scholar]
- Turkat 1985 .Turkat ID, Maisto SA. In: Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: a step by step treatment manual. Barlow DH, editor. Guilford Press; New York: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Tyrer 2005 .Tyrer P, Bateman AW. The Social Functioning Questionnaire: a rapid and robust measure of perceived functioning. Internal Journal of Social Psychiatry. 2005;51:265–75. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyrer 2007 .Tyrer P, Kramo K, Miloseka K, Seive Wright H. The place for nidotherapy in psychiatric practice. Psychiatric Bulletin. 2007;31:1–3. [Google Scholar]
- Ware 1993 .Ware JE, Snow KK, Kosinski M, Gandek B. SF-36 Health Survey Manual and Interpretation Guide. New England Medical Centre; Boston, MA: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Weissman 1994 .Weissman MM, Bland RC, Canino GJ, Greenwald S, Hwu HG, Lee CK, et al. The cross national epidemiology of obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1994;55(Suppl):5–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wells 1990 .Wells MC, Glickauf-Hughes C, Buzzell V. Treating obsessive-compulsive personalities in psychodynamic/interpersonal group therapy. Psychotherapy. 1990;27:366–79. [Google Scholar]
- WHO 1992 .World Health Organization . The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. WHO; Geneva: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Winston 1994 .Winston A, Laikin M, Pollack J, Samstag LW, McCullough L, Muran JC. Short-term psychotherapy of personality disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:190–4. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman 2005 .Zimmerman M, Rothschild L, Chelminski I. The prevalence of DSM-IV personality disorders in psychiatric outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1911–18. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; * Indicates the major publication for the study