Skip to main content
Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health logoLink to Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health
. 2014 Sep 11;2014(1):122–133. doi: 10.1093/emph/eou021

Political influence associates with cortisol and health among egalitarian forager-farmers

Christopher R von Rueden 1,*, Benjamin C Trumble 2, Melissa Emery Thompson 3, Jonathan Stieglitz 3, Paul L Hooper 4, Aaron D Blackwell 2, Hillard S Kaplan 3, Michael Gurven 2
PMCID: PMC4178369  PMID: 25214482

Abstract

Background and objectives: Low social status increases risk of disease due, in part, to the psychosocial stress that accompanies feeling subordinate or poor. Previous studies report that chronic stress and chronically elevated cortisol can impair cardiovascular and immune function. We test whether lower status is more benign in small-scale, relatively egalitarian societies, where leaders lack coercive authority and there is minimal material wealth to contest.

Methodology: Among Tsimane’ forager-horticulturalists of lowland Bolivia, we compare informal political influence among men with urinary cortisol, immune activation (innate and acquired), and morbidity as assessed during routine medical exams.

Results: After controlling for potential confounds, we find that politically influential men have lower cortisol, and that this association is partly attributable to access to social support. Cortisol is positively associated with men’s income, which may reflect chronic psychosocial stress from market involvement. Greater influence is also associated with lower probability of respiratory infection, which is a frequent source of morbidity among Tsimane’. Among men who lost influence over a 4-year period, cortisol and probability of respiratory infection were higher the greater the decline in influence.

Conclusions and implications: Deleterious effects of low status on health are not merely ‘diseases of civilization’ but may result from how (even subtle) status differences structure human behavior.

Keywords: hierarchy, status, cooperation, stress, cortisol

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES

Within diverse human societies, health improves with every step up the socioeconomic ladder [1–3]. The sources of this gradient in health are multiple, and inter-individual differences in healthcare and lifestyle behaviors (e.g. physical activity level and diet) do not fully explain it. Non-human primates demonstrate a similar relationship between status and health that is influenced but not fully explained by activity level or diet [4–8]. Many of the effects that status has on health have been attributed to chronic psychosocial stress that accompanies social subordination and reduced access to resources [2–12].

Among humans, it remains an open question whether links between status, psychosocial stress and health are unique features of institutionalized or dominance-based hierarchies, given the paucity of studies in relatively egalitarian, small-scale societies. The effects of status variation in small-scale societies have significant evolutionary implications since large-scale societies with heritable, material wealth represent only the past 10 millennia of human history. Egalitarian, small-scale societies are characterized by extensive resource pooling across households and minimal disparity in wealth [13, 14]. Adults may differ in their informal political influence, which is based less on dominance than on prestige due to generosity or skill [15, 16]. Modest status differences can affect access to mates and social support [16–18]. Similarly, modest differences in socioeconomic status (SES) relative to neighbors or social contacts influence well-being in large-scale societies [19–21]. The most salient status comparisons are not typically with individuals at the opposite end of the SES spectrum, but with individuals with whom we are in frequent face-to-face contact and in direct competition for resources or respect [22–24].

Here we examine links between informal political influence and biomarkers of stress and disease among semi-sedentary Tsimane’ forager-horticulturalists of Amazonian Bolivia. We measured the influence of 199 men (aged 18–83 years) from four villages using a ranking strategy. Villages are often subdivided into clusters of extended families which may congregate to work or socialize (e.g. resolve disputes and discuss community affairs). While no man wields coercive authority over another, the opinions and desires of more influential men carry more weight during village gatherings. Because influential men have more social support from kin and non-kin [16], they can steer the consensus-based decision-making of community meetings in their favor. These men may also receive deference in other contexts, including mate competition and conflict resolution. Disputes over access to arable land and accusations of extramarital sex, theft or free-riding on community projects are common conflicts among men [25].

First, we test whether Tsimane’ men with less informal political influence have higher urinary cortisol (prediction 1). Basal cortisol levels and the cortisol response to waking are positively associated with psychosocial stress among Westerners [9, 26–28], non-Westerners [29] and non-human primates [4–7]. Cortisol is an important metabolic hormone with many physiological functions in the body; acute and long-term stressors can induce cortisol responses by the adrenal cortex, which prepares the body for action by increasing blood glucose. Chronic psychosocial stress can not only produce relatively high levels of cortisol but in severe cases is associated with ‘burnout’ of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and low levels of cortisol, or reduced circadian variation in cortisol [28]. We also consider alternative explanations to psychosocial stress for an influence–cortisol relationship, including age, body size and lifestyle differences associated with income from sporadic wage labor or horticultural sales.

High status individuals may be less prone to psychosocial stress due to a greater sense of control [30, 31] and to greater recourse to social support to buffer stressful events [26, 32, 33]. For example, a study of government and military leaders finds they have lower cortisol than non-leaders, and this effect is mediated by sense of control [9]. Among non-human primates, subordinates who have less social support and who are subject to frequent harassment exhibit higher levels of cortisol than dominants [4–7]. However, dominants may experience more psychosocial stress and produce higher cortisol levels than subordinates when the hierarchy is unstable, or when dominants have neurotic, anti-social personalities [5, 6]. Under these conditions, high-ranking individuals may adopt greater vigilance to monitor threats to their status and have less recourse to social support.

Second, we test whether the effect of influence on cortisol is mediated by social support and by conflict with other men (predictions 2a,b), as both reduced support and greater conflict can increase psychosocial stress. We also test whether the influence–cortisol relationship is moderated by men’s personality as assessed by scores on the Big Five Inventory (BFI) (prediction 3) or moderated by hierarchy stability (prediction 4). We measure hierarchy stability using longitudinal influence data within one village and comparisons of conflict frequency across four villages. Tsimane’ conflicts are often mediated by third parties, and therefore higher conflict frequency can reduce men’s sense of control or predictability. Even if men do not directly experience conflict, more intra-village conflict may place men at greater risk of gaining or losing influence if conflict resolution does not always favor the status quo. In non-human primates, hierarchy instability is concomitant with increases in dominance interactions [5].

Status-related stress can increase risk of communicable and non-communicable disease, in part through the effects of cortisol and the sympathetic nervous system on energy mobilization, inflammation and acquired immunity [34]. In Western societies, chronic stress has previously been associated with hypertension [35] and biomarkers of inflammation that indicate risk of cardiovascular disease (CVD), including an elevated erythrocyte sedimentation rate and higher levels of C-reactive protein (CRP) [36, 37]. Risk of infectious disease is also increased with chronic stress, due to neuroendocrine suppression of lymphocyte and antibody production [38, 39]. We therefore test whether more influential Tsimane’ men are healthier (prediction 5), based on their blood pressure, sedimentation rate, CRP, lymphocyte counts and clinical diagnoses of cardiovascular abnormalities (e.g. cardiac-type chest pain and arrhythmia), intestinal parasites, respiratory infections and skin infections. Tsimane’ experience frequent infection-related inflammation but minimal hypertension and CVD [40]. Acute infections may contribute most to status-related health outcomes, unlike Western populations where chronic diseases are a principal source of status-related morbidity. Nevertheless, we may still find variation in blood pressure or CVD that is associated with men’s influence.

METHODOLOGY

Study population

The Tsimane’ are forager-horticulturalists living in lowland forests of Bolivia along the Maniqui River and its tributaries. The majority of the diet consists of horticultural products (plantains, rice, corn and sweet manioc), fish and wild game. Less than 10% of the diet is purchased with income from sporadic wage labor with loggers or cattle ranchers or from sale of horticultural and forest products. The population is roughly 15 000 and is growing at 3.6% per year despite relatively high infant mortality. Across all ages, over half of deaths are due to infectious disease, especially respiratory and gastrointestinal infections [41]. The Tsimane’ are dispersed among approximately 95 villages, which range in size from 30 to 700 individuals.

Villagers hold occasional meetings, which are used to plan collective activities, such as clearing of community trails or responding to incursion by illegal loggers. Community meetings are also used to mediate conflicts that were unresolved by the parties directly involved. No individual or group within a community maintains coercive authority over others. Individuals who threaten open debate because they are aggressive, angry or authoritarian are criticized. This includes the village ‘corregidor’ (literally, ‘corrector’). In the late twentieth century, missionaries helped establish the election of ‘corregidores,’ to represent community interests to outside political bodies and to facilitate community meetings. To date, only men have been elected ‘corregidor.’ Their tenure can be as short as 2 months, though most ‘corregidores’ hold their position on the order of 5 years. Like the shamans and informal village leaders of previous generations, ‘corregidores’ and other influential individuals are normatively constrained to lead via consensus-building rather than by fiat. Influence tends to peak when men are in their 30s or 40s, accrues to those who have more support from kin and other allies, and is linked with greater reproductive success [16, 17, 25].

Political influence

Each adult man from four Tsimane’ villages (n = 199 aged 18+ years) was evaluated in terms of his intra-community political influence. The number of adult men in each village ranged from 18 to 73. One village was evaluated in 2008, a second in 2005 and again in 2009, and the others in 2009. To measure men’s political influence, half of the adult men in each village were randomly selected as raters. The raters represented most ages and all extended families within their village. Each rater was shown an array of photographs of men from their community and asked to rank them from highest to lowest according to ‘whose voice carries the most weight during community debates.’ Photos were Polaroids™ of the top-half of each man’s body, set against as neutral a background as possible. No one rated themselves. A block design was used so that no two photographs appeared together in the same array more than once. In the two larger villages, each rater ranked two arrays of nine photographs, and each of the resident men was ranked nine times by nine different evaluators, yielding a range in political influence scores from 9 to 81. For one of the two smaller villages, a similar block design was used producing a range in influence scores from 6 to 36. In the smallest village no block design was employed; each rater compared photos of all of the other men in the community within the same photo array. Influence scores were scaled to match the range in score values from the largest village.

Each rater evaluated the photos with no one else present but C.v.R. The raters were made aware of the confidentiality of their individual rankings. Verbal instructions were translated into the Tsimane’ language from Spanish and then, as a test of the accuracy of translation, back-translated into Spanish by Tsimane’ men from other communities. Our confidence in the validity of the influence measure is strengthened by ethnographic observation. Men ranked as influential spoke more often during community meetings attended by C.v.R. in 2009 (r = 0.53, P < 0.001, n = 73). However, the photo-rankings are based on years of interpersonal relationships and are probably better metrics than researchers’ short-term observations [42].

Disease diagnoses and biomarker analyses

The Tsimane Health and Life History Project is a panel study that collects data from over 90 Tsimane villages in the Beni and La Paz Departments. From 2004 to 2009, qualified physicians would visit each village once per year to diagnose disease and provide treatment. Beginning in 2010, participants were invited from their villages and transported to a nearby clinic where they were housed in the clinic facility until their medical examination. We include for analysis those medical observations made the year before through the year after collection of influence data. This results in 2.9 medical assessments (SD = 1.5) per man ranked on intra-village influence, whether the medical examination was conducted in their village or at the clinic. All infections were diagnosed according to the ‘International Statistical Classification of Disease and Related Health Problems’ (ICD-10). Skin infections were primarily mycoses and leishmaniasis. Respiratory infections included sinusitis, laryngitis, bronchitis, pneumonia and tuberculosis. Cardiovascular diagnoses included murmurs, arrhythmias, cardiac-type chest pain and systolic or diastolic hypertension (>140/90 mm Hg). Systolic and diastolic blood pressures were obtained by physicians using a Welch Allyn Tycos Aneroid 5090 sphygmomanometer and Littman stethoscope; all participants had been seated or supine for ≥20 min when the measurement was taken. Heights and weights were measured using a Seca stadiometer and a digital Tanita weigh scale. First morning void urine and morning fasting venous blood were collected. Lymphocytes were measured on site immediately following venous blood draw. A QBC Autoread Plus Hematology Analyzer (QBC Diagnostics Inc., PA, USA) generated a white blood cell count, which was multiplied by lymphocyte percentage from a manual cell count. A portion of blood was drawn into a Westergreen-Katz tube to the 200 mm mark and left to stand vertically for 1 h to measure sedimentation rate. The remaining blood specimen was allowed to clot before centrifuging at 1500 × g for 10 min to separate serum. Serum and urine specimens were frozen in liquid nitrogen for up to 6 months before transfer on dry ice to the University of New Mexico, and the University of California at Santa Barbara where specimens were stored at −80°C until assay.

At the University of New Mexico, urinary cortisol was measured with enzyme immunoassay using reagents and protocols provided by the University of California at Davis Clinical Endocrinology Laboratory, and specific gravity was used to correct for concentration [43]. Within and between-assay coefficients of variation (CVs) were 5.4% and 10.9%, respectively, for the high (1040 pg/ml) and 9.5% and 12.6% for the low (522 pg/ml) controls. Serum CRP was measured at the University of California at Santa Barbara with an in-house enzyme immunoassay [44]; within and between assay CVs were 7.3% and 10.2% for the high (11.4 mg/l) and 5.3% and 9.2% for the low (6.1 mg/l) controls.

Until 2008, fecal samples were analyzed for the presence of helminth eggs and larvae by direct identification on wet mounts. Duplicate mounts were prepared with 0.9% saline solution and iodine solution, respectively, and examined at 100× and 400× for helminth eggs (hookworm, A. lumbricoides and T. trichiuris), and larvae (S. stercoralis). Beginning in 2007, fecal samples were also preserved in 10% formalin solution, and later quantitatively analyzed using a modified Percoll (Amersham Pharmacia) technique [45]. Individuals were coded as either infected or not infected if helminths were detected by either method.

Other measures

All demographic data used to age individuals come from reproductive history interviews first collected from 2003 to 2005 and updated annually thereafter. Contemporaneous with the political influence rankings, C.v.R. interviewed men concerning their income, which is the reported earnings over the previous year from wage labor for loggers and ranchers and from sales of forest and horticultural products. Yearly average income in US dollars was $504 (range = 0 to $4415). Men also reported the names of other men in the village with whom they have been in conflict during the past 6 months, and the names of men who they consider regular cooperative partners (food exchange, support during conflicts, and cooperation during hunting, fishing and horticultural work). We calculated conflicts and cooperative partners as the number of times men were nominated by others on these measures.

Personality traits were measured via responses to the 43-item Tsimane’ BFI, administered in 2009 and 2010. The BFI was conducted verbally in a private location by a bilingual Tsimane’ research assistant. Responses were given on a Likert scale where 1 corresponds to ‘strongly disagree’ and 5 corresponds to ‘strongly agree.’ The Tsimane’-specific ‘Big Two’ (Prosociality, Industriousness) were derived from exploratory factor analysis and validated against a separate sample of spouse-derived ratings, as described in Gurven et al. [46].

Statistical analyses

Models are generalized estimating equations (GEEs) that include, in addition to the covariates described in the text, subject effects and fixed effects for village. We accounted for subject effects because the models included repeated measures of influence, 4 years apart, on the same individuals within one of the study villages. In all villages, furthermore, the same influence score for a given individual may have appeared up to three times per model, because influence scores were compared with biomarkers and disease diagnoses from the same year, the previous year and the subsequent year. Controlling for year of measurement did not significantly change our results (Supplementary Tables S4 and S5). Of the 199 men evaluated for political influence, 27 men were never seen by clinicians because they were absent on each occasion the medical team visited their village. Men who were and were not seen by clinicians do not differ in political influence (Mann-Whitney U = 1926, z = −1.43, P = 0.154).

The biomarker sample sizes differ from each other because only randomized subsets of urine, blood or feces were assayed. Table 1 presents the means, SDs, number of observations and number of observations per participant for each of the biomarker and health measures. Cortisol was measured for 111 men who provided urine between 2005 and 2010, with an average 1.7 measures per individual (58 men: 1 measure; 33 men: 2 measures; 16 men: 3 measures; and 4 men: 4 measures). Among the men whose cortisol was evaluated from 2 or more years, the single-measures intra-class correlation for their lowest and highest cortisol measures is 0.33 (95% CI = 0.06–0.55, P = 0.008). Men for whom cortisol was not measured (n = 88) score on average 0.24 SDs lower on political influence compared with the men (n = 111) whose cortisol was measured (Mann-Whitney U = 4061, z = −2.041, P = 0.041). Our models of cortisol are restricted to 103 men for whom we have complete, contemporaneous data on relevant confounds (age, body mass index (BMI) and income).

Table 1.

Means, standard deviations (SD), number of observations and number of observations per man (range in parentheses) for the health measures

Health Measure Mean SD Number of observations Number of observations/ man (range)
Cortisol 244 196 pg/ml 155 282 pg/ml 188 1.7 (1–4)
Sedimentation rate 27 mm/h 20 mm/h 274 2.3 (1–6)
C-reactive protein 6.24 mg/l 11.56 mg/l 62 1.0
Lymphocytes 2710 773 263 2.2 (1–6)
Systolic BP 112 mm HG 11.58 mm HG 497 2.9 (1–6)
Diastolic BP 69 mm HG 9.59 mm HG 497 2.9 (1–6)
Cardiovascular abnormality 0.03 0.18 491 2.9 (1–6)
Intestinal parasites 0.85 0.35 260 2.3 (1–6)
Respiratory infection 0.16 0.37 491 2.9 (1–6)
Skin infection 0.09 0.29 491 2.9 (1–6)

Note: Cardiovascular abnormalities, intestinal parasites, respiratory infection and skin infection are binary variables.

Models of disease diagnosis (CVD, skin infection, respiratory infection and intestinal parasites) are binary logistic GEEs; otherwise the models are linear GEEs. To reduce skew, all biomarkers, measures of social support and conflict, and income were logged prior to analysis. The influence and personality measures were centered prior to modeling their interaction to avoid collinearity between main and interaction effects.

RESULTS

Men with more influence produced lower levels of first morning void urinary cortisol (β = −0.17, P = 0.024, n = 103, number of observations = 171). Addition to the model of a binary variable for location of urine collection (village = 0, clinic = 1) did not alter the effect size of influence (β = −0.17, P = 0.021), nor was site of urine collection a significant covariate (β = 0.07, P = 0.733). The addition of controls for age and BMI strengthens the effect of influence (β = −0.20, P = 0.006), and adding men’s income further strengthens the effect (β = −0.27, P = 0.001). This suggests the influence–cortisol relationship is not driven by heterogeneity in age, phenotypic correlations associated with body size or lifestyle differences associated with income. After controlling for those variables, cortisol levels were 0.51 SDs lower among men in the highest versus lowest influence quartile (Fig. 1). While influential men earn more income from sporadic wage labor and sale of horticultural products (β = 0.36, P < 0.001, n = 188, number of observations = 237), income is positively associated with cortisol levels, independent of influence, age and BMI (β = 0.19, P = 0.024, n = 103, number of observations = 171). Cortisol levels were 0.23 SDs higher among men in the highest ($1262/year) versus lowest income quartile ($78/year).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Men’s morning urinary cortisol (n = 103) by influence quartile (1 = most influence). Error bars represent 95% CIs. Cortisol values are residuals controlling for age, BMI and income

‘Does social support or conflict mediate the influence–cortisol relationship?’ More influence is associated with having more cooperative partners, which is associated with lower cortisol (Table 2). In a model of cortisol that controls for age, BMI and income (n = 103, number of observations = 171), cooperative partners (β = −0.20, P = 0.023) and influence (β = −0.18, P = 0.044) remain significant, independent predictors. The 33% reduction in effect size of influence indicates partial mediation by cooperative partners. The male–male dyadic conflicts in which men were involved over the previous 6 months ranged from short-term verbal disagreement or physical fights to long-term, ongoing quarrels (mean = 1.4, range = 0–11). Men with more influence experienced more, not fewer, dyadic conflicts and frequency of conflict is not independently associated with cortisol (Table 2).

Table 2.

Standardized coefficients from a linear GEE model of influence (n = 178) and of cortisol (n = 103)

Independent variable Influence Cortisol
Cooperative partners 0.32*** −0.23**
Conflicts 0.16** −0.12
Age 0.06 −0.05
BMI 0.31*** −0.01
Income 0.16* 0.22*

*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

‘Does the influence–cortisol relationship vary by personality?’ We tested for interactions between men’s influence and their scores on the Big Five personality dimensions of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness, as well as two personality dimensions (the Tsimane’-specific ‘Big Two’) that were derived from exploratory factor analysis [46]. None of these personality dimensions significantly mediated or moderated the effect of influence on cortisol (Supplementary Table S1). The interaction of conscientiousness with influence (Supplementary Fig. S1) has twice as large an effect on cortisol (β = −0.15, P = 0.117, n = 70, number of observations = 129) as the interaction of influence with any other personality measure.

‘Does status instability moderate the influence–cortisol relationship?’ We assessed hierarchy stability in one community by measuring men’s political influence twice, in 2005 and again in 2009. While 7% of men emigrated during that time, the influence of those who remained was strongly predicted by their influence 4 years prior (r = 0.84, P < 0.001, n = 53, number of observations = 53). Gains and losses in influence averaged 10.4% of the range in influence within the village (Fig. 2), and the effect of influence on cortisol was larger in this village (β = −0.33, P = 0.003, n = 45, number of observations = 84) than in the other three villages combined (β = −0.24, P = 0.088, n = 58, number of observations = 87), controlling for age, BMI and income. As might be expected given the relatively minimal changes in influence, we found no effect of change in influence from 2005 to 2009 on change in cortisol (β = −0.06, P = 0.799, n = 22, number of observations = 22) nor on cortisol measured at the end of that period (β = −0.02, P = 0.840, n = 34, number of observations = 59), after controlling for baseline influence. However, among those men who lost influence between 2005 and 2009, greater declines in influence are associated with higher cortisol at the end of that period (β = −0.42, P = 0.033, n = 19, number of observations = 33). This effect is not mediated by men’s age, BMI or income (Supplementary Table S2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Tsimane’ men’s change in political influence over 4 years, as a function of age. Each line represents an individual status trajectory (n = 53) within the village where influence was measured in 2005 and again in 2009

We also assessed hierarchy instability by comparing frequency of dyadic conflict across four villages. More conflict may indicate greater hierarchy instability if conflict resolution causes parties involved to gain or lose influence within the village. Conflict frequency ranged from 0.5 to 1.9 conflicts per man over a 6-month period (Kruskal Wallis X2 across villages = 21.02, P < 0.01). In a model of men’s cortisol that included main effects for influence and village conflict frequency as well as their interaction effect, village conflict frequency did not significantly interact with men’s influence (β = 0.03, P = 0.677, n = 103, number of observations = 171).

‘Does influence affect health outcomes?’ Controlling for age, BMI and income, influential men were less likely to be diagnosed with respiratory infection (OR = 0.73, 95% CI = 0.54–0.99, n = 166, number of observations = 456). Prevalence of respiratory infection in all men was 16%; for men in the top quartile of influence the prevalence drops to 13%. We found no effect of change in influence from 2005 to 2009 on change in respiratory infection (OR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.96–1.21, n = 46, number of observations = 46) nor on odds of respiratory infection at the end of that period (OR = 1.12, 95% CI = 0.56–2.27, n = 50, number of observations = 108), after controlling for baseline influence. However, among men who lost influence between 2005 and 2009, smaller declines in influence are associated with lower odds of diagnosis with respiratory infection at the end of that period, controlling for baseline influence (OR = 0.08, 95% CI = 0.01–0.42, n = 19, number of observations = 43).

Influence was not associated with blood sedimentation rate, CRP, lymphocyte count or diagnosis with intestinal parasites (85.4% prevalence), skin infection (8.9% prevalence) or cardiovascular abnormalities (Table 3). The latter includes systolic or diastolic hypertension (1.8% prevalence) in addition to heart murmur (0.2%), arrhythmia (0.4%) and possible cardiac-type chest pain (0.4%). When the sample was limited to older men (age 40+ years), who have presumably had more time for status to ‘get under the skin’, influence remained a strong, negative predictor of cortisol (β = −0.28, P = 0.004, n = 63, number of observations = 116), and influence also predicted lower systolic (β = − 0.17, P = 0.021, n = 69, number of observations = 185) and diastolic blood pressure (β = −0.16, P = 0.014, n = 69, number of observations = 185). In older men, income predicted higher systolic (β = 0.16, P = 0.026, n = 69, number of observations = 185) and diastolic blood pressure (β = 0.18, P = 0.015, n = 69, number of observations = 185), controlling for age, BMI and influence. Other health measures were not significantly associated with influence or income in older men (Supplementary Table S3).

Table 3.

Standardized effects of influence and income when entered jointly in linear GEE models of biomarkers and in binary logistic GEE models of disease diagnoses, with age and BMI as controls

Effect of influence Effect of income
Health measure n Number of observations Β Β
Cortisol 103 171 −0.27** 0.19*
Sed. rate 114 251 −0.09 −0.07
CRP 58 58 −0.01 −0.09
Lymphocytes 112 241 0.04 −0.04
Systolic BP 166 461 −0.06 0.04
Diastolic BP 166 461 −0.09 0.17
Health measure n Number of observations Effect of influence Effect of income
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Cardiovascular 166 456 1.14 0.74–1.75 1.65 0.81–3.34
Intestinal parasites 108 231 0.84 0.57–1.24 0.90 0.61–1.32
Respiratory infection 166 456 0.73* 0.54–0.99 1.10 0.82–1.47
Skin infection 166 456 1.02 0.69–1.52 0.81 0.57–1.15

Notes: The lymphocytes model also controls for current infection status. Odds ratios (OR) are for increments of 1 SD of influence or income. The sample sizes vary because different random samples of urine, serum and feces were collected and analyzed. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01.

CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS

In an egalitarian small-scale society, greater informal political influence among men is associated with lower cortisol. Controlling for potential confounds (age, BMI and income), the effect size of influence on cortisol was similar in magnitude to the cortisol difference between leaders and non-leaders in a sample from the USA [9]. Longitudinal measures from one village suggest that losses in influence are especially stressful. Despite the relative stasis of the hierarchy during the 4-year period of observation, greater declines in influence associated with higher cortisol at the end of that period. The Tsimane’ are not the only small-scale society to demonstrate a relationship between status and cortisol; villagers from the Caribbean island of Dominica produced higher cortisol the worse their social reputation and the higher their frequency of distress [29]. Lower status in small-scale, relatively egalitarian societies can be as conducive to psychosocial stress as in more stratified societies.

However, there are other possible interpretations. Elevated levels of cortisol are associated with persistent negative emotions [4–7, 9, 26–29], or they can indicate a more positive anticipation of daily demands [47, 48]. Cortisol levels are adaptive responses to prepare organisms for action, and sustained psychosocial stress is not necessarily pathological depending on the source of the stressor and the diurnal profile of cortisol [48, 49]. Furthermore, cortisol is an important metabolic hormone that plays many physiological roles in the body. Even when not subject to psychosocial stress, high-ranking baboon and chimpanzee males can exhibit high cortisol due to acute energetic stress from mate-guarding or contest competition [7, 50]. It is possible that Tsimane’ men lacking influence experienced more frequent energetic stress (independent of income-related lifestyle differences) or positive, excited mood.

Men’s number of labor- and food-sharing partners partially mediated the influence–cortisol relationship, similar to evidence from industrialized populations that inter-individual differences in social support contribute to the SES gradient in psychosocial stress and health [26, 32, 33]. In traditional subsistence societies like the Tsimane’, transfers of food and other resources are critical for managing daily energy shortfalls and, over the longer-term, for managing illness, injury and care of dependent offspring [13, 14]. Investments in political influence may be motivated, consciously or not, as additional insurance against these and other risks, including support during social conflicts [17, 18, 51]. Although the causality remains unclear, it is likely that political influence and social support are mutually reinforcing. Generous gifts of food and labor can increase influence, particularly if they are targeted at influential individuals. Influence can also beget social support as compensation for leadership during village meetings and disputes. In the village where men’s influence was measured twice, increases in political influence over a 4-year interval were associated with increases in social support, independent of contemporaneous changes in traits that could also impact social support including men’s body size, food production and income [25]. After crop loss in that same village, only men who ranked in the top quartile of political influence reported receiving aid for their families from non-kin [25].

Men with higher levels of cortisol participated in fewer male–male conflicts, which differs from studies with US samples where negative social interactions associated with higher morning cortisol [26, 49]. More conflicts were attributed to influential Tsimane’ men, who may be less wary of the consequences of conflict, anticipate lower probability of conflict loss or are at greater risk of conflict due to having more resources (social or material) to contest. We found no effect of village conflict frequency on the relationship between influence and cortisol. Villages may not differ enough in conflict frequency, or the latter may have no significant bearing on the stability of men’s influence. There was no significant moderation, nor evidence of mediation, of the influence–cortisol relationship by men’s personality, even though several personality dimensions associated with cortisol levels in a larger sample of Tsimane’ adults [52].

Differences in political influence may have impacts on Tsimane’ men’s health. Influential men had lower probability of respiratory infection, which is a frequent cause of morbidity among Tsimane’ [41]. Among men who lost influence over time, odds of diagnosis with respiratory infection increased the more influence they lost. Among older men (age 40+ years), influence associated with lower blood pressure, though hypertension was rare. It is possible that differential exposure to psychosocial stress, mediated by the sympathetic nervous system and cortisol, accounts for these effects. Status-related changes in cortisol signaling may directly affect regulation and expression of immune-related genes, as found in non-human primates [53]. Since comparisons of influence with health controlled for age, BMI and income, we discount phenotypic correlations related to body size or income-related lifestyle differences as explanations. Indeed, more numerous social partners and encounters with non-Tsimane would suggest influential men would contract more, not less, respiratory infection. We did not find that influence was a panacea for health ailments; influence did not associate with diagnoses of skin and intestinal infections or cardiovascular abnormalities.

Men who earned more sporadic income had higher levels of cortisol, independent of their age, BMI and influence. When restricted to older men, income was associated with higher blood pressure. These results are similar to findings from traditional communities in central Mexico and in Samoa, where individuals who had adopted Western, modern lifestyles showed more evidence of psychosocial stress [54, 55]. While energetic stress may also associate with income, Tsimane’ who score higher on other indices of ‘modernization’(education, Spanish fluency) do not engage in significantly more (or less) physical activity [56]. Tsimane’ men who earned more income have greater exposure to the culture of San Borja and may have shifted their principal status comparisons to non-Tsimane’ Bolivians, whose lifestyles and material purchases they find difficult to emulate. Alternatively, greater income may have undermined the quality of men’s relationships within their community. Men with more income were more likely to be influential, but they are not as respected as men who excel in traditional skills such as hunting [16]. Also, greater income can produce family disruption, due to spousal conflicts over use of income and accusations of infidelity [57]. A study with a different sample of Tsimane’ villages found that individuals with higher ordinal wealth ranks within their village reported fewer sick days over the previous 2 weeks [58]. Since wealth included purchased material goods, our results suggest that in small-scale societies experiencing modernization, income can produce protective as well as detrimental effects on health.

LIMITATIONS

To better determine the causal relationships among different measures of status and health among the Tsimane’, we need better understanding of the patterning of status acquisition across the life-course. This study is limited to adults, yet childhood and adolescence may be the critical periods for establishing the social ties that beget status and regulate stress throughout life. Children’s perceptions of their competitiveness and desirability within their social worlds may trigger adaptive shifts in life history strategy [59], including altered sensitivity to social threats and long-term reorganization of cortisol responses [60]. There is evidence that Western subjects with low SES in childhood show heightened adrenocortical and inflammatory responses as adults, independent of their current SES [61, 62]. Within small-scale societies where the spatial extent of lifetime mobility is limited, the social relationships of youth may be even more consequential relative to adult SES.

We do not yet know the effect of political influence on Tsimane’ women’s health nor the effects of men’s influence on wives health. However, women married to influential men have larger BMIs, controlling for their age and their children experience lower mortality [17]. Tsimane’ women often have less of a direct voice in community affairs relative to men, due in part to the opportunity costs of the sexual division of labor, including care of multiple dependent offspring. In Western subjects, there is evidence that social support affects the cortisol responses of men and women differently [26, 27], in support of arguments that the sexes have evolved different motivations regarding the nature and number of their social ties [63].

Previous reports indicate that female urinary cortisol profiles vary across the menstrual cycle [64], suggesting that a larger sample (e.g. 10–15 specimens per individual) would be needed to get a true baseline for a sample size of 5–30 cycling female participants [65]. Less is known about the variance of male cortisol values. Here we had between 1 and 4 specimens per man (n =103 men); while more specimens would have been ideal, there is no reason that fewer than 10 specimens per individual should bias these results in one direction or another. If anything, fewer specimens adds additional noise to the sample, and only decreases the probability of finding statistically significant results; that we still find strong associations between cortisol and political influence suggests our results are robust. Future studies will examine acute changes in both cortisol and political influence, during community meetings or other times of political jockeying.

CONCLUSION

Why should informal influence matter in a relatively egalitarian society? The health benefits of status in non-humans and in large-scale societies are due in part to dominance over others, whether physical dominance or institutionally granted authority. In egalitarian societies like the Tsimane’, no adult man has coercive authority over another and there is minimal material wealth to contest. However, status in egalitarian societies is accompanied by social benefits that can prove instrumental in mitigating risks over the long term [17, 18, 25, 51]. Low status may engender psychosocial stress due to lack of such social insurance. In large-scale societies, furthermore, the status comparisons most consequential for psychosocial stress are typically among individuals who are in geographic proximity or who occupy the same social network rather than between individuals at opposite ends of the socioeconomic spectrum [19–24]. The importance of relative status perceptions to human psychology may have its roots in the small-scale societies of our ancestors.

SUPPLEMENTARY DATA

Supplementary data is available at EMPH online.

Supplementary Data

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank the Tsimane’ who participated in this study and the Tsimane’ Health and Life History Project staff and physicians. Adrian Jaeggi and an anonymous reviewer provided helpful comments on earlier drafts.

FUNDING

The National Science Foundation [BCS-0136274, BCS-0422690 and DDIG-0921429] and the National Institutes of Health/National Institute on Ageing [R01AG024 119-01].

Conflict of interest: None declared.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, et al. Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. Am Psychol. 1994;4:15–24. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.49.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wilkinson RG. Mind the Gap. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Marmot M. The Status Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects Our Health and Longevity. New York: Henry Holt; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Abbott DH, Keverne EB, Bercovitch FB, et al. Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Horm Behav. 2003;43:67–82. doi: 10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00037-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sapolsky RM. The physiology of dominance in stable versus unstable social hierarchies. In: Mason W, Mendoza S, editors. Primate Social Conflict. New York: SUNY Press; 1993. pp. 171–83. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sapolsky RM. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science. 2005;308:648–52. doi: 10.1126/science.1106477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gesquiere LR, Learn NH, Simao MC, et al. Life at the top: rank and stress in wild male baboons. Science. 2011;333:357–60. doi: 10.1126/science.1207120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Archie EA, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Social status predicts wound healing in wild baboons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:9017–22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206391109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sherman GD, Lee JJ, Cuddy A, et al. Leadership is associated with lower levels of stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:17903–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207042109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Baum A, Garofalo JP, Yali AM. Socioeconomic status and chronic stress: does stress account for SES effects on health? Ann NY Acad Sci. 1999;896:131–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08111.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kristenson M, Eriksen HR, Sluiter JK, et al. Psychobiological mechanisms of socioeconomic differences in health. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58:1511–22. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00353-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE. Psychological stress and disease. JAMA. 2007;298:1685–7. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.14.1685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hill K, Hurtado AM. Cooperative breeding in South American hunter–gatherers. Proc R Soc B. 2009;276:3863–70. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gurven M, Stieglitz J, Hooper PL, et al. From the womb to the tomb: the role of transfers in shaping the evolved human life history. Exp Gerontol. 2012;47:807–13. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2012.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lewis HS. Leaders and Followers: Some Anthropological Perspectives. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley; 1974. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.von Rueden C, Gurven M, Kaplan H. The multiple dimensions of male social status in an Amazonian society. Evol Hum Behav. 2008;29:402−15. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.von Rueden C, Gurven M, Kaplan H. Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige. Proc R Soc B. 2012;278:2223–32. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gurven M, Allen-Arave W, Hill K, et al. ‘It’s a Wonderful Life’: signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay. Evol Hum Behav. 2000;21:263–82. doi: 10.1016/s1090-5138(00)00032-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wood AM, Boyce CJ, Moore SC, et al. An evolutionary based social rank explanation of why low income predicts mental distress: a 17 year cohort study of 30,000 people. J Affect Disord. 2012;136:882–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Luttmer EFP. Neighbors as negatives: relative earnings and well-being. Q J Econ. 2005;120:963–1002. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Anderson C, Kraus MW, Galinksy AD, et al. The local-ladder effect: social status and subjective well-being. Psychol Sci. 2012;23:764–71. doi: 10.1177/0956797611434537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum Relat. 1954;7:117–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Frank R. Choosing the Right Pond: Human Behavior and the Quest for Status. New York: Oxford University Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Norton MI. All ranks are local: why humans are both (painfully) aware and (surprisingly) unaware of their lot in life. Psychol Inq. 2013;24:124–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.von Rueden C. The acquisition of social status by males in small-scale human societies. (with an emphasis on the Tsimane of Bolivia) (Doctoral dissertation). ProQuest UMI No. 3495771, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Seeman TE, McEwen BS. Impact of social environment characteristics on neuroendocrine regulation. Psychosom Med. 1996;58:459–71. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199609000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Kirschbaum C, Steptoe A. Work stress, socioeconomic status and neuroendocrine activation over the working day. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58:1523–30. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00347-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Chida Y, Steptoe A. Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychol. 2009;80:265–78. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Decker SA. Salivary cortisol and social status among Dominican men. Horm Behav. 2000;38:29–38. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bosma H, Van Jaarsveld CH, Tuinstra J, et al. Low control beliefs, classical coronary risk factors, and socio-economic differences in heart disease in older persons. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60:737–45. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Smith PM, Frank JW, Bondy S, et al. Do changes in job control predict differences in health status? Results from a longitudinal national survey of Canadians. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:85–91. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815c4103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Helliwell JF, Putnam RD. The social context of well-being. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2004;359:1435–46. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Matthews KA, Gallo LC, Taylor SE. Are psychosocial factors mediators of socioeconomic status and health connections? Ann NY Acad Sci. 2010;1186:146–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05332.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Seeman T, Epel E, Gruenewald T, et al. Socio-economic differentials in peripheral biology: cumulative allostatic load. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2010;1186:223–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05341.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Karlamangla AS, Singer BH, Williams DR, et al. Impact of socioeconomic status on longitudinal accumulation of cardiovascular risk in young adults: the CARDIA study. Soc Sci Med. 2005;60:999–1015. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Fuligni AJ, Telzer EH, Bower J, et al. A preliminary study of daily interpersonal stress and C-reactive protein levels among adolescents from Latin American and European backgrounds. Psychosom Med. 2009;71:329–33. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181921b1f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Palmblad J, Karlsson CG, Levi L, et al. The erythrocyte sedimentation rate and stress. Acta Med Scand. 1979;205:517–20. doi: 10.1111/j.0954-6820.1979.tb06094.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Webster Marketon JI, Glaser R. Stress hormones and immune function. Cell Immunol. 2008;252:16–26. doi: 10.1016/j.cellimm.2007.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Segerstrom SC, Miller GE. Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:601–30. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Gurven M, Blackwell AD, Rodriguez DE, et al. Does blood pressure inevitably rise with age? Longitudinal evidence among forager-horticulturalists. Hypertension. 2012;60:25–33. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.111.189100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Gurven M, Kaplan H, Supa Z. Mortality experience of Tsimane Amerindians of Bolivia: regional variation and temporal trends. Am J Hum Biol. 2007;19:376–98. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hill K, Kintigh K. Can anthropologists distinguish good and poor hunters? Implications for hunting hypotheses, sharing conventions, and cultural transmission. Curr Anthropol. 2009;50:369–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Miller RC, Brindle E, Holman DJ, et al. Comparison of specific gravity and creatinine for normalizing urinary reproductive hormone concentrations. Clin Chem. 2004;50:924–32. doi: 10.1373/clinchem.2004.032292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Brindle E, Fujita M, Shofer J, et al. Serum, plasma, and dried blood spot high-sensitivity C-reactive protein enzyme immunoassay for population research. J Immunol Methods. 2010;362:112–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jim.2010.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Eberl M, al-Sherbiny M, Hagan P, et al. A novel and sensitive method to monitor helminth infections by faecal sampling. Acta Tropica. 2002;83:183–7. doi: 10.1016/s0001-706x(02)00089-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gurven M, von Rueden C, Massenkoff M, et al. How universal is the Big Five? Testing the five-factor model of personality variation among forager-farmers in the Bolivian Amazon. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;104:354–70. doi: 10.1037/a0030841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Brandtstadter J, Baltes-Gotz B, Kirschbaum C, et al. Developmental and personality correlates of adrenocortical activity as indexed by salivary cortisol: observations in the age range of 35 to 65 years. J Psychosom Res. 1992;35:173–85. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(91)90072-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Decker SA, Aggott Z. Stress as adaptation? A test of the adaptive boost hypothesis among Batswana men. Evol Hum Behav. 2013;34:55–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, et al. Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:17058–63. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Muller MN, Wrangham RW. Dominance, cortisol, and stress in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004;55:332–40. doi: 10.1007/s00265-020-02872-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Boone J. The evolution of magnanimity: when is it better to give than to receive? Hum Nature. 1998;9:1–21. doi: 10.1007/s12110-998-1009-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Gurven M, von Rueden C, Stieglitz J, et al. The evolutionary fitness of personality traits in a small-scale subsistence population. Evol Hum Behav. 2014;35:17−25. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Tung J, Barreiro LB, Johnson ZP, et al. Social environment is associated with gene regulatory variation in the rhesus macaque immune system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:6490–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202734109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.James GD, Baker PT, Jenner DA, et al. Variation in lifestyle characteristics and catecholamine excretion rates among young Western Samoan men. Soc Sci Med. 1987;25:981–6. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(87)90002-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Dressler WW, Mata A, Chavez A, et al. Arterial blood pressure and individual modernization in a Mexican community. Soc Sci Med. 1987;24:679–87. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(87)90311-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Gurven M, Jaeggi A, Kaplan H, et al. Physical activity and modernization among Bolivian Amerindians. PLoS One. 2013;8:e55679. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Stieglitz J, Gurven M, Kaplan H, et al. Infidelity, jealousy, and wife abuse among Tsimane forager-farmers: testing evolutionary hypotheses of marital conflict. Evol Hum Behav. 2012;33:438−48. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Undurraga E, Nyberg C, Eisenberg DT, et al. Individual wealth rank, community wealth inequality, and self-reported adult poor health: a test of hypotheses with panel data (2002–2006) from Native Amazonians, Bolivia. Med Anthropol Q. 2010;24:522–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1548-1387.2010.01121.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Jackson JJ, Ellis BJ. Synthesizing life history theory with sexual selection: toward a comprehensive model of alternative reproductive strategies. Behav Brain Sci. 2009;32:31–2. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Flinn M, Nepomnaschy PA, Muehlenbein MP, et al. Evolutionary functions of early social modulation of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis development in humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:1611–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Li L, Power C, Kelly S, et al. Life-time socio-economic position and cortisol patterns in mid-life. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:824–33. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, et al. Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:14716–21. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902971106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Benenson JF. The development of human female competition: allies and adversaries. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2013;368:20130079. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Nepomnaschy PA, Altman RM, Watterson R, et al. Is cortisol excretion independent of menstrual cycle day? A longitudinal evaluation of first morning urinary specimens. PLoS One. 2011;6:e18242. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Nepomnaschy PA, Lee TC, Zeng L, et al. Who is stressed? Comparing cortisol levels between individuals. Am J Hum Biol. 2012;24:515–25. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data

Articles from Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES