Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Oct 2.
Published in final edited form as: Subst Use Misuse. 2012 Nov-Dec;47(0):1661–1665. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2012.723898

Steve Sussman on Matilda Hellman’s “Mind the Gap! Failure in Understanding Key Dimensions of an Addicted Drug User’s Life”

Addictive Effects

Steve Sussman 1
PMCID: PMC4181842  NIHMSID: NIHMS629737  PMID: 23186503

Abstract

“Addictive effects” are experiential states sought by individuals that underlie addictive behaviors. Consistent with ideas that addictive effects mimic satiation of appetitive motives, a literature search-derived heuristic catalogue of addictive behaviors is offered and contrasted across four general appetitive-like motives that have been posited as underlying addictive behaviors (dominance, submissiveness, self-pleasure, and nurturance). I suggest, in part, that addictive behaviors are misdirected attempts to satisfy appetitive motives.

Keywords: addictive effects, appetitive motives, appetitive function, addictive behavior catalogue


It remains unclear in the research literature what entity might underlie “addictive effects.” There is some consensus that addictive effects share in common a function to shift subjective experience of self (Larkin, Wood, & Griffiths, 2006). This shift appears to consist of specific types. Subjective reports of such experiential shifts include affect enhancement, arousal enhancement, sedation, cognitive fantasy, or sense of oblivion, as main examples (Schneider & Irons, 2001; Sussman & Sussman, 2011). There may be differential functions associated with those subjective reports. Possibly manipulation of a subjective sense of satiating various bodily needs (“appetitive” effects) may underlie which behaviors may become addictive (Foddy & Savulescu, 2010a, 2010b; Goodman, 1990; Hatterer, 1982; Jacobs, 1986; Newlin, 2002). More precisely, the immediate effect of the behavior may lead one to feel as if some bodily need has been satisfied. Also, as part of an addictive effect, some period of time may occur in which cravings/urges are not operative (Foddy & Savulescu, 2010a, 2010b; Marks, 1990; Orford, 2001); one may experience a temporary sense of physiological fulfillment of the appetitive “need.” During this generally brief period, one may feel self-sufficient or nurtured (Hirschman, 1992; Pearson & Little, 1969). Importantly, an addictive effect appears to involve repeated experiences that elicit eventually a pathological relationship with an appetitive behavior (Schneider & Irons, 2001; Sussman & Sussman, 2011).

Many behaviors may come to serve an appetitive function (e.g., eating behavior), but not all such behaviors qualify as an addictive behavior (e.g., eating an occasional large meal versus binge eating). What differentiates an addictive behavior from a nonaddictive behavior may, at least in part, be a function of the way in which the behavior is expressed. That is, with an addictive behavior, over time, persons may become preoccupied with the behavior, exhibit a loss of control over the behavior, and suffer negative life consequences as a result (Sussman, Lisha, & Griffiths, 2011; Sussman & Sussman, 2011). One may ponder what leads to dysregulation of an appetitive behavior. From an evolutionary perspective, instead of engaging in extensive “work” to satisfy an appetitive motive (e.g., hunting for food, growing one’s own food), the motive may be satisfied too easily and quickly, possibly leading, for some people, to repeated cycles of attempting to satiate the motive with diminishing success, resulting in dysregulation (Koob & LeMoal, 2001; Robinson & Berridge, 2000; Sussman, Reynaud, Aubin & Leventhal, 2011).

Early psychological theorizing suggested that humans experienced innate or acquired instincts in need of satiation, including fear, anger, shyness and sociability, curiosity and secretiveness, acquisitiveness (desire to possess) and rivalry, jealousy and envy, affection, sexual love, parental love (nurturance), play, imitation, and constructiveness (Alcoholics Anonymous, 1976; Angell, 1908). More recent evolutionary theories of human behavior have asserted that adaptation attributes such as self-perceived survival ability and reproductive fitness are reflected biologically in the operations of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system and, hence, are subject to “hijacking” by addictive behaviors, which will induce subjective increases in experience of these attributes (Blum et al., 2012; Newlin, 2002). Addictive behaviors may provide a misleading sense of neurobiological fitness through induction of positive emotional feelings and modulation of arousal (new opportunities associated with increases in arousal, or security or serenity associated with decreases in arousal), which (may falsely) signal biological fitness (Panksepp, Knutson, & Burgdorf, 2002).

Neurobiological-sociocultural theories of ill-health note that, as persons are able to situate in one location and fulfill easily needs for food, shelter, and protection, sedentary habits may accumulate (Fave, Massimini, & Bassi, 2011, p. 25), indicating a competition between cultural and biological fitness. It is possible that persons may exhibit behaviors so as to help them feel as if they are grappling to satiate appetitive needs (Newlin, 2002), in a context that does not provide the arena for application of physiological work to satiate those needs (Blum et al., 2012). It is in this way that numerous addictions may operate and reflect a problem of lifestyle (Sussman, Lisha, & Griffiths, 2011). Certain individuals adapt well to technologically enhanced, sedentary-promoting lifestyles; others do not (Blum et al., 2012). Possibly, up to 50% of a society does not adapt that well—and the resulting patterns of dysregulated behavior that occur may become labeled as addictions (Bechara, 2005; Griffiths & Larkin, 2004; Hatterer, 1982; Holden, 2001; Kourosh, Harrington, & Adinoff, 2010; Marks, 1990; Orford, 2001; Sussman, Lisha, & Griffiths, 2011).

There are many behaviors that could be experienced as appetitive and become manifested as addictive in expression. Popularly discussed behaviors include tobacco, alcohol, and other drug misuse; binge overeating; shopping; Internet use (e.g., online gaming); love and sex; workaholism; exercise; and gambling (Sussman, Lisha, & Griffiths, 2011). However, there are many additional behaviors that might become addictive. An example of a seldom-studied addictive behavior is tanning (Kourosh et al., 2010). Tanning-related behaviors include mirror checking, grooming, picking skin, and UV exposure periods that may be experienced as a rush. Tanning often involves use of salons and timers, and the experience may involve primarily sensory processing. Tanning may be a means to attempt to make oneself more socially dominant, one type of appetitive motive, through attempting to increase one’s physical attractiveness. Further, UV exposure may lead to release of beta-endorphin and serotonin, as well as being a source of Vitamin D, which could be interpreted as fulfilling pleasure and self-nurturance motives. In the next section of this paper, I use a literature search to generate a catalogue of addictions and attempt to place them in a typology of appetitive experience.

Speculation: A Catalogue of Addictions

To generate a (reasonably) exhaustive list of addictions, I engaged in an electronic search focusing on different types of addictions indicated on different Web sites. I used the key words “types of addictions,” “types of addiction,” “addictions list,” and “addiction list” (June 5, 2012), which revealed 113,000, 508,000, 13,800, and 17,600 pages, respectively, in Google (I examined the first 100 in the list for each set of keywords); 462, 787, 4, and 51 pages in Google Scholar (I examined the first 200 pages in the list for each set of keywords); 11, 17, 0, and 0 pages in OVID Medline (1946 to June, week 2, 2012); and 13, 47, 0, and 0 pages in PSYCInfo. Most Web sites or articles focused on one or two types of common addictions. However, there were some Web sites and articles that addressed three or more types of addictions (e.g., http://www.addictionz.com/addictions.htm: 131 types of addictions are suggested, last accessed June 30, 2012; http://www.healthyplace.com/addictions/addictions-information/types-of-addiction-list-of-addictions/: 26 types of addictions are suggested, last accessed June 30, 2012). In a couple of cases, a unique type of addiction was mentioned only or primarily in that source (e.g., regarding plastic surgery addiction: http://www.ranker.com/list/12-biggest-plastic-surgery-addicts/, last accessed June 30, 2012).

I merged specific addictive behaviors together (e.g., there were many types of “hard drug” use categories). After this merging, I calculated 65 different specific addictions. To permit an even more digestible set of addictions, albeit as a tentative heuristic, I grouped these “subcategories” into 16 more general categories (e.g., similarly to what was found by Cook, 1987) as follows:

  1. Drugs: caffeine, tobacco, alcohol, marijuana, various other illicit/hard drugs (e.g., prescription, amphetamines, opioids, cocaine, XTC) [5 subcategories];

  2. Food-related: binge eating, use of diurectics, carbohydrates, hot peppers, fat, chocolate, ice, “inedible objects” (e.g., dirt, toilet paper, chalk, household cleanser, gasoline, tape) [8 subcategories];

  3. Compulsive antisocial behavior (violence): compulsive aggression, compulsive stealing, compulsive fire setting [3 subcategories];

  4. Technology/communications-related: Internet browsing, SNS (social networking sites), texting, online and offline videogames, television [5 categories];

  5. Gambling;

  6. Working;

  7. Social group-related: sex, love, platonic relationships, codependence (people-pleasing, hiding behind others), being “cool,” attention/applause, compulsive helping, maintaining authority/control [8 subcategories];

  8. Physical attractiveness: tanning (tanorexia), teeth whitening, make-up, cosmetic surgery [4 subcategories];

  9. Fantasizing: imagination, isolation, laziness (i.e., underachieving, being sedentary) [3 subcategories];

  10. Exercise-related: aerobics, body building [2 subcategories];

  11. Spiritual obsession: occult, religion, self-help programs, treatment seeking [4 subcategories];

  12. Pain seeking: cutting, self-mutilation, skin picking, trichotillomania (hair pulling), scab picking [5 subcategories];

  13. Shopping;

  14. Thrill/adventure seeking: auto-racing, cruising, dangerous sports, thrills (compulsive sky diving, riding roller coasters) [4 subcategories];

  15. Hoarding: anime/comics/cards, small collectables, rocks, puppets, coins, junk, trivia, technology objects [8 subcategories];

  16. Voyeurism: celebrity or other idolization, gossiping, attending funerals [3 subcategories].

One may speculate that different categories of addictions are associated with different neurobiological motivations (e.g., Greenberg, Lewis, & Dodd, 1999; Rozin & Stoess, 1993; Sussman et al., 2011). Thus, I considered how these 16 addictive behavior categories might operate from an appetitive motives’ perspective. I examined four types of motives in the addictions research literature that might apply. Work with the PROMIS questionnaire (www.s-p-q.com; last accessed July 1, 2012; Haylett, Stephenson, & Lefever, 2004; MacLaren & Best, 2010) has revealed at least two general factors associated with different addictive behaviors, “hedonist” types (illegal drugs, tobacco, prescription drugs, gambling, compulsive sex, alcohol, and caffeine) and “nurturant” types (compulsive helping, work, relationships, shopping, eating behaviors, and exercise). Hedonist-type addictions appear to focus on immediate pleasure, whereas nurturant-type addictions appear to focus on personal fulfillment.

Haylett and colleagues (2004), though not MacLaren and Best (2010), also found some support for dominance and submissive-related factors, possibly nested within hedonist and nurturant factors. Other literature also has focused on addictions as reflecting fight (e.g., dominance, power) or flight (e.g., retreat into fantasy, submission) motives (Blum et al., 2012; Goeders, 2004; Newline, 2002; Rawson & Condon, 2007; Sunderwirth & Milkman, 1991), related to limbic system-based reward, or stimulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA).

As a heuristic exercise, I crossed the 16 addictive behavior categories with four motives, as is depicted in Table 1. I placed each of the 16 behavior categories into pairs of motives, with the assumption that multiple motives operate for any given addictive behavior (e.g., Haylett et al., 2004). There are six possible pairs of the four motives within which the behaviors could be placed. However, “dominance” and “submissive” would not be a plausible combination as they describe polar opposite motivations. Thus, I attempted to place each behavior into the five remaining combinations of appetitive motivation pairs (see Table 1). Avoidance/submissive-pleasure/hedonist behaviors included drug use and pain seeking. Dominance-hedonist behaviors included compulsive violence and adventure thrills. Hedonist-nurturance behaviors included food intake, compulsive use of technology, gambling, social-related (e.g., love, sex), exercise, shopping, and hoarding. Dominance-nurturance behaviors included worka-holism, physical attractiveness seeking, and compulsive voyeurism. Submissive-nurturance behaviors included spiritual obsession and fantasizing.

TABLE 1.

List of Addictions by Motives

Addiction Appetitive Motives
Dominance Avoidance/submissive Pleasure/hedonist Nurturance
Drug intake X X
Food intake X X
Compulsive antisocial behavior (violence) X X
Technology/communications-related X X
Gambling X X
Working X X
Social group-related X X
Physical attractiveness seeking X X
Exercise X X
Spiritual obsession X X
Pain seeking X X
Shopping X X
Adventure thrills X X
Hoarding X X
Fantasizing X X
Voyeurism X X

Note. This listing is a heuristic device and must be interpreted with caution; empirical studies are needed.

Implications

These 16 addictive behavior categories likely represent a vast majority of types of addictions located in the literature, as they were derived from a relatively extensive electronic literature review. Each of these behaviors by definition may exhibit dysregulation features, including repetitive, erratically experienced, phenomenological changes in appetitive motivation that bypasses deliberate processing of information (Sussman et al., 2011). The appetitive motives for which these behaviors may subjectively satiate include achieving a satisfying or pleasurable state (or novelty), nurturance (of self or others), dominance (feeling powerful), or submission (conformity, fitting in to a “pecking” order; e.g., Haylett et al., 2004). I perceived that all 16 behavior categories might be described by one of five appetitive motive combinations. One could withdraw (submission, flight) and feel satisfied/pleasure. Arousal reduction/sedation might be among subjective effects reported. One could dominate and feel pleasure. Subjective effects might include experience of peak moments or arousal enhancement. One could achieve both pleasure and nurturance, subjectively feeling very deeply satisfied perhaps. One could dominate and feel nurturance (maybe feeling self-contained). Finally, one could withdraw and feel nurturance (fantasy). It is hoped that this paper provides additional insight into potential mechanisms underlying phenomenological addictive experiences. However, much empirical research is needed to truly make inroads and integrate addictive behavior experiences with neurobiologically plausible appetitive processes (Stacy, Ames, & Knowlton, 2004). Treatment implications of this work may include lifestyle changes to encourage more use of working memory to satiate appetitive needs (Stacy et al., 2004), safe or “better directed” alternatives to satiation of subjective appetitive needs (e.g., Alcoholic Anonymous, 1976), or possibly learning to accept the inevitability of feeling a sense of “wanting” as a side effect of modern living.

Acknowledgments

This paper was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (no. DA020138). Dr Sussman was invited to write a brief commentary about Dr Hellman’s paper. Upon reading the paper, he was stimulated to write this paper.

Biography

graphic file with name nihms629737b1.gif

Steve Sussman, Ph.D., F.A.A.H.B., F.A.P.A., received his doctorate in social-clinical psychology from the University of Illinois at Chicago in 1984. He is a professor of preventive medicine and psychology at the University of Southern California (USC), and he has been at USC for 27 years. He studies etiology, prevention, and cessation within the addictions arena, broadly defined. He has over 385 publications. His programs include Project Towards No Tobacco Use, Project Towards No Drug Abuse, and Project EX, which are considered model programs at numerous agencies (i.e., CDC, NIDA, NCI, OJJDP, SAMSHA, CSAP, Colorado and Maryland Blueprints, Health Canada, US Department of Energy, and various State Departments of Education). He received the honor of Research Laureate for the American Academy of Health Behavior in 2005, and was also the President there (2007–2008). Also, as of 2007, he received the honor of Fellow of the American Psychological Association (Division 50, Addictions). He is the current editor of Evaluation & the Health Professions (SAGE Publications).

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest

The author reports no conflicts of interest. The author alone is responsible for the content and writing of the article.

References

  1. Alcoholics Anonymous. Alcoholics anonymous. New York: Alcoholics Anonymous World Services; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  2. Angell JR. Psychology: An introductory study of the structure and function of human consciousness. 4. New York: Henry Holt and Company; 1908. The important human instincts; pp. 346–362. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1458–1463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blum K, Chen ALC, Giordano J, Borsten J, Chen TJH, Hauser M, et al. The addictive brain: All roads lead to dopamine. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. 2012;44:134–143. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2012.685407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cook DR. Self-identified addictions and emotional disturbances in a sample of college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 1987;1(1):55–61. [Google Scholar]
  6. Fave AD, Fausto M, Bassi M. Psychological selection and optimal experience across cultures: Social empowerment through personal growth (Cross cultural advancements in positive psychology 2) New York: Springer; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  7. Foddy B, Savulescu J. A liberal account of addiction. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology. 2010a;17:1–22. doi: 10.1353/ppp.0.0282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Foddy B, Savulescu J. Relating addiction to disease, disability, autonomy, and the good life. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology. 2010b;17:35–42. doi: 10.1353/ppp.0.0282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Goeders NE. Stress, motivation, and drug addiction. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2004;13:33–35. [Google Scholar]
  10. Goodman A. Addiction: Definition and implications. British Journal of Addictions. 1990;85:1403–1408. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01620.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Greenberg JL, Lewis SE, Dodd DK. Overlapping addictions and self-esteem among college men and women. Addictive Behaviors. 1999;24:565–571. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(98)00080-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Griffiths MS, Larkin M. Conceptualizing addiction: The case for a “complex systems” account. Addiction Research and Theory. 2004;12:99–102. [Google Scholar]
  13. Hatterer LJ. The addictive process. Psychiatric Quarterly. 1982;54:149–156. doi: 10.1007/BF01064756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Haylett SA, Stephenson GM, Lefever RMH. Covariation in addictive behaviors: A study of addictive orientations using the Shorter PROMIS Questionnaire. Addictive Behaviors. 2004;29:61–71. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(03)00083-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hirschman EC. The consciousness of addiction: Toward a general theory of compulsive consumption. Journal of Consumer Research. 1992;19:155–179. [Google Scholar]
  16. Holden C. “Behavioral” addictions: Do they exist? Science. 2001;294:980–982. doi: 10.1126/science.294.5544.980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jacobs DF. A general theory of addictions: A new theoretical model. Journal of Gambling Behavior. 1986;2:15–31. [Google Scholar]
  18. Koob GF, LeMoal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24:97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kourosh AS, Harrington CR, Adinoff B. Tanning as a behavioral addiction. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2010;36:284–290. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2010.491883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Larkin M, Wood RTA, Griffiths MD. Toward addiction as relationship. Addiction Research and Theory. 2006;14:207–215. [Google Scholar]
  21. MacLaren VV, Best LA. Multiple addictive behaviors in young adults: Student norms for the shorter PROMIS Questionnaire. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:252–255. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.09.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Marks I. Behaviour (non-chemical) addictions. British Journal of Addiction. 1990;85:1389–1394. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Newlin DB. The self-perceived survival ability and reproductive fitness (SPFit) theory of substance use disorders. Addiction. 2002;97:427–445. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00021.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Orford J. Addiction as excessive appetite. Addiction. 2001;96:15–31. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961152.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Panksepp J, Knutson B, Burgdorf J. The role of brain emotional systems in addictions: A neuro-evolutionary perspective and new “self-report” animal model. Addiction. 2002;97:459–469. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00025.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pearson MM, Little RB. The addictive process in unusual addictions: A further elaboration of etiology. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1969;125:1166–1171. doi: 10.1176/ajp.125.9.1166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Rawson RA, Condon TP. Why do we need an Addiction supplement focused on methamphetamine? Addiction. 2007;102(Suppl 1):1–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01781.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: An incentive-sensitization view. Addiction. 2000;95:91–117. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rozin P, Stoess C. Is there a general tendency to become addicted? Addictive Behaviors. 1993;18:81–87. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(93)90011-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Schneider JP, Irons RR. Assessment and treatment of addictive sexual disorders: Relevance for chemical dependency relapse. Substance Use & Misuse. 2001;36:1795–1820. doi: 10.1081/ja-100108428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sunderwirth SG, Milkman H. Behavioral and neurochemical commonalities in addiction. Contemporary Family Therapy. 1991;13:421–433. [Google Scholar]
  32. Stacy AW, Ames SL, Knowlton BJ. Neurologically plausible distinctions in cognition relevant to drug use etiology and prevention. Substance Use & Misuse. 2004;39:1571–1623. doi: 10.1081/ja-200033204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sussman S, Lisha N, Griffiths M. Prevalence of the addictions: A problem of the majority or the minority? Evaluation & the Health Professions. 2011;34:3–56. doi: 10.1177/0163278710380124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sussman S, Reynaud M, Aubin HJ, Leventhal AM. Drug addiction, love, and the Higher Power. Evaluation & the Health Professions. 2011;34:362–370. doi: 10.1177/0163278711401002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sussman S, Sussman AN. Considering the definition of addiction. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2011;8:4025–4038. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8104025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES