Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2014 Oct 6;24(19):2257–2266. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.023

The Logic of Circadian Organization in Drosophila

Stephane Dissel 1,2,3, Celia N Hansen 1,2, Özge Özkaya 1, Matthew Hemsley 1, Charalambos P Kyriacou 1, Ezio Rosato 1,
PMCID: PMC4188814  PMID: 25220056

Summary

Background

In the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, interlocked negative transcription/translation feedback loops provide the core of the circadian clock that generates rhythmic phenotypes. Although the current molecular model portrays the oscillator as cell autonomous, cross-talk among clock neurons is essential for robust cycling behavior. Nevertheless, the functional organization of the neuronal network remains obscure.

Results

Here we show that shortening or lengthening of the circadian period of locomotor activity can be obtained either by targeting different groups of clock cells with the same genetic manipulation or by challenging the same group of cells with activators and repressors of neuronal excitability.

Conclusions

Based on these observations we interpret circadian rhythmicity as an emerging property of the circadian network and we propose an initial model for its architectural design.

Graphical Abstract

graphic file with name fx1.jpg

Highlights

  • Locomotor activity rhythms in Drosophila have 24 hr periodicity

  • Different clock neurons promote either longer or shorter activity rhythms

  • Circadian period is an emerging property of a network of diverse oscillators

  • The logic connecting the network is beginning to emerge


Our understanding of the circadian clock relies on the assumption that all rhythmic cells cycle with the same ∼24 hr rhythm that is characteristic of the organism as a whole. Dissel et al. show that the brain of Drosophila harbors a mosaic of cellular periodicities whose integration results in the ∼24 hr clock as we know it.

Introduction

The circadian clock provides the interface between an organism and its 24 hr geophysical environment. As currently accepted, the fly clock is constituted by interlocked negative transcription/translation feedback loops (TTFLs). At the core of the system are the activators CLOCK (CLK) and CYCLE (CYC) that bind to the promoters of the period (per) and timeless (tim) genes, initiating their transcription. After translation, the negative autoregulators PER and TIM become the substrate of several kinases and phosphatases, they dimerize, translocate into the nucleus, and repress the CLK/CYC complex. The second feedback loop is centered around Clk and involves the rhythmic expression of PAR domain protein 1 (Pdp1) and vrille (vri) (reviewed in [1]). The blue-light-sensitive protein CRYPTOCHROME (CRY) regulates photo-responsiveness and flies with altered CRY function display aberrant circadian light entrainment [2, 3, 4, 5] and visual behavior [6]. Moreover, immunofluorescence (IF) and confocal microscopy reveal that α-CRY immunoreactivity (IR) is often found at the level of neuronal projections [7], suggesting that CRY may play additional roles. Null mutants for cry (cry0) show defects in rhythmic behavior under constant light (LL) [8], so it is possible that CRY exerts direct functions in central clock neurons. Indeed, light-activated CRY increases neuronal firing via an unknown mechanism [9].

Clock neurons constitute a network of cells expressing clock genes that are divided into lateral neurons (six dorsal lateral neurons [LNds], four large ventral lateral neurons [l-LNvs], four small ventral lateral neurons [s-LNvs], a single Pdf-null ventral lateral neuron [pn-LNv], and three lateral posterior neurons [LPNs]) and dorsal neurons (∼16 DN1s, 2 DN2s, and ∼40 DN3s) (reviewed in [10]). Although the classic molecular model portrays the clock as cell autonomous, cellular cross-talk appears to be essential for its function [11, 12, 13, 14].

The circadian neurons can be further grouped or differentiated by the activation of promoters of clock and clock-related genes. For instance, all clock neurons express tim [15] whereas the Pigment-dispersing factor (Pdf) promoter is active only in the s- and l-LNvs [16]. A cry 5.5-kb promoter [17] is seemingly expressed in all LNvs, in the LNds, and in two DN1s [11] although Shafer et al. [18] reported further expression in two additional DN1s (called DN1a) and two DN3s. By combining the expression of these promoters, coupled to either GAL4 (to drive transcription of a reporter gene) or GAL80 (to inhibit GAL4 function), it is possible to define subsets of clock cells and to manipulate them selectively by expressing genes whose products alter the electrical properties of the neurons or the running of the clock.

In our study we have introduced local alterations in the neuronal network and have investigated the period of locomotor activity under constant darkness and temperature (DD), an artificial condition where the interactions among neurons is independent from the light-dark (LD) cycle. We did this by initially distinguishing among clock cells based on whether they express both the Pdf and cry promoters (PDF+CRY+), the cry promoter only (PDFCRY+), or neither (PDFCRY). Our results suggest a model for the logic that regulates the neuronal network under these conditions.

Results and Discussion

CRYΔ Lengthens or Shortens the Endogenous Period of Locomotor Activity when Expressed in Different Clock Neurons

CRYΔ, a C-terminal deletion of CRY, renders CRY constitutively active, as revealed in a number of molecular (light-independent binding to PER and TIM) and behavioral (long period of locomotor activity in DD) phenotypes [4, 5]. Based on the discovery that light-activated CRY increases neuronal firing [9], we presumed that CRYΔ could activate neurons and enhance their output in DD, which might be reflected in period changes. We used a tim-GAL4 driver and several UAS-cryΔ lines and confirmed that overexpression of CRYΔ in all clock cells, namely PDF+CRY+ ∩ PDFCRY+ ∩ PDFCRY, results in ∼1 hr lengthening of the endogenous period of locomotor activity compared to controls [5] (Table S1 available online). We noticed that the vast majority of CRYΔ flies had simple rhythmicity (SR) although a few showed complex rhythms (CR, more than one periodicity in a single fly) or arrhythmicity (AR) but in proportions no different from controls.

We asked whether all clock cells contribute to the free running period or whether one group of neurons imposes its own rhythmicity to behavior [13]. We focused on one line (UAS-cryΔ14.6), which will hereafter be referred to as cryΔ.

PDF+CRY+ > CRYΔ (Pdf-GAL4 > UAS-cryΔ) and PDF+CRY+ ∩ PDFCRY+ > CRYΔ (cry13-GAL4 > UAS-cryΔ) flies both revealed a ∼1 hr longer period compared to controls. We obtained a similar value for the PDF+CRY+ ∩ PDFCRY+ ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ genotype (tim-GAL4 > UAS-cryΔ), which argues against the assumption that these three drivers largely differ in their “strength” [13] (Figure 1, Table S1). Then, we expressed CRYΔ in smaller groups of neurons by combining the GAL80 repressor with GAL4 drivers. We used the lines cry-GAL802e3m and Pdf-GAL8096a, each carrying two copies of GAL80, that have been reported to repress UAS-dependent expression in the PDF+CRY+ ∩ PDFCRY+ and the PDF+CRY+ cells, respectively [11]. To confirm the extent of GAL80-mediated inhibition of GAL4 activity, we measured anti-GFP IR of PDF+CRY+ cells in tim-GAL4 > UAS-GFP, in tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-GFP, and in tim-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-GFP flies. For both GAL80 configurations, we were able to confirm a good and equivalent level of repression of GAL4 activity in PDF+CRY+ cells (Figures S1A and S1B). However, in all tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-GFP preparations, we observed a robust anti-GFP signal in three LNds, meaning that GAL80 cannot efficiently inhibit GAL4 in these cells because of lower cry expression (Figures S1A and S1C). GAL80-mediated repression of GAL4 was inefficient also in four DN1ps and in three or four cry-positive DN3s. Conversely, good repression was achieved in the DN1as, in two DN1ps, and in the pn-LNv (Figure S1C and data not shown). Thus, the PDFCRY+ group is heterogeneous and should be divided into two. Henceforth we identify cells that strongly or weakly express cry as PDFCRY‡ and PDFCRY, respectively.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Different Groups of Neurons Affect Rhythmic Behavior

(A) Operational classification of clock neurons according to the expression of the Pdf and cry promoters (see also text and Figure S1).

(B) Period differences between CRYΔ-expressing and control flies. Horizontal black bars refer to the period change (in hours) compared to controls when CRYΔ (UAS-cryΔ14.6) is expressed in particular groups of neurons (shown on the left) as a result of different GAL4/GAL80 combinations (shown on the right).

(C) Average locomotor activity profiles of CRYΔ-expressing flies showing 4 days in LD 12:12 and 12 days in DD. Genotypes and statistics are as reported in Table S1; control CRYΔ was w, UAS-cryΔ14.6.

We tested PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ (cry13-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-cryΔ) and PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ (tim-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-cryΔ) flies; for both genotypes the free running period of locomotor activity was not significantly different from controls. However, when we analyzed PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-cryΔ) flies, we observed a significant 1.2 hr shortening of the period (Figure 1; Table S1). This result cannot be explained in terms of residual GAL4 expression in the PDF+CRY+ cells as we detected similar levels of IR in them when driving GFP in both tim-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a and tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m flies (Figures S1A and S1B). CRYΔ overexpression resulted in a faster rhythm only for the latter genotype, thereby unveiling the contribution of the PDFCRY and PDFCRY groups to period, which would otherwise be inhibited by the PDFCRY‡ cells (this is because the two genotypes above differ in period and in the inclusion of the latter group of cells).

Interestingly, in wild-type flies the DN2 cluster of PDFCRY neurons show faster (∼22 hr) endogenous molecular cycling that is not reflected in the ∼24 hr behavioral rhythms [19]. Under seminatural conditions, they show an advance in molecular cycling compared to other clock neurons, also consistent with a faster period [20, 21]. Conversely, it has been reported that by reducing the contribution of the PDF+CRY+ cells to the network, either by eliminating their main signaling molecule, the neuropeptide PDF [16] (Figure S2), or by reducing their numbers [11] (Table S2), a short activity period is generated. Thus, the overall evidence supports a role for the PDFCRY neurons in generating a short-rhythm phenotype when the balance of the network is tilted in their favor, with the DN2s being a likely but perhaps not exclusive component of this function.

We can rationalize this first set of observations as follows. The overexpression of CRYΔ in PDF+CRY+ cells (Pdf-GAL4 > UAS-cryΔ) results in a longer period. However, these slower-paced cells are not alone in controlling self-sustained behavior because increasing the impact on the network of the PDFCRY and PDFCRY neurons (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-cryΔ) results in a shorter period. The effect is reversed by adding PDFCRY‡ to the ensemble (tim-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-cryΔ), suggesting the latter intervene in a negative control, possibly on the faster PDFCRY neurons (Figure 2A). The simultaneous activation of PDFCRY‡ and PDFCRY neurons (cry13-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-cryΔ) had no consequence in terms of period (Figure 1; Table S1). This suggests a balance, with the PDFCRY‡ neurons inhibiting the faster-paced PDFCRY cells and in turn the PDFCRY neurons inhibiting the slower-paced PDF+CRY+ cells (Figure 2A). Note that in this context, the words “activation” and “inhibition” do not have a physiological connotation but are used as logical operators.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Circadian Logic

We operationally divided the circadian neurons into PDF+CRY+, PDFCRY‡, PDFCRY, and PDFCRY groups (see also Figures 1 and S1).

(A) The PDF+CRY+ cells have a large influence on the network as they can communicate with a significant number of neurons through PDF (green arrows). Probably, the PDFCRY‡ neurons are particularly responsive to PDF signaling (thick green arrow) as they express the PDF receptor (PDFR) at the highest level [37]. Activation of the PDF+CRY+ cells results first in a longer period of locomotor activity and then, as activation increases, in complex rhythms (Figure S3). Activation of the PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY cells results in a shorter period of locomotor activity and then in arrhythmicity. The PDFCRY‡ and the PDFCRY cells have an inhibitory role toward the output of the PDF+CRY+ and the PDFCRY neurons, respectively, thus balancing slower and faster components in the network. This could provide a simple mechanism to achieve phase changes under different environmental conditions.

(B) The PDF+CRY+ also use PDF-independent connections (black arrow) to promote the activity of the PDFCRY cells (see also Figure S2). The latter exert a negative feedback on the former through activation of the PDFCRY neurons. Thus, activation of PDF+CRY+ results in direct (PDF-independent) activation and in indirect (mediated and amplified by PDF and the PDFCRY‡ neurons) repression of the PDFCRY cells. These integrate both pathways and feedback to regulate the activity of the PDF+CRY+ neurons. This could explain how the PDF+CRY+ and PDFCRY groups synchronize together.

(C) Within the PDFCRY group, the N neurons (including the majority of DN1s) are involved in stabilizing behavioral rhythms through synchronization with the PDF+CRY+, which suggests they might have an independent connection with those cells (broken blue line). The F neurons (which include the DN2s and perhaps unrecognized PDFCRY neurons) have an intrinsically faster molecular rhythm. We assume that the signal to inhibit the PDF+CRY+ group can be passed on to the PDFCRY before the PDFCRY‡ cells have time to exert their repression. This would explain how the fast PDFCRY counteract the slower cycling of the PDF+CRY+ neurons, resulting in a 24 hr period. However, the signal for a faster rhythm would not usually reach the N neurons before the repression from the PDFCRY‡ cells takes effect, suggesting a delay in the connection between the F and N groups of PDFCRY neurons. The activation of the PDFCRY and PDFCRY groups (or a reduction in the activation of the PDFCRY‡ cells) would overcome that delay, causing the N cells (such as the DN1s) to cycle in synchrony with the F cells (such as the DN2s); see Figure 4. The N neurons would then pass on the shorter-period signal to the PDF+CRY+ group (such as the s-LNvs, Figure 4), causing a shorter behavioral period.

Additional Manipulations Consolidate the Model

We wanted to show that the effects observed on period are not specific to CRYΔ but are reproducible by other manipulations. Thus, we employed the same GAL4/GAL80 combinations described above, using effectors that have a known and consistent mode of action independent of the type of neuron.

The overexpression of the depolarization-activated bacterial sodium channel NaChBac [19] using tim-GAL4 (PDF+CRY+ ∩ PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY) resulted predominantly in arrhythmicity, confirming the effectiveness of this manipulation (Table S2). Limiting NaChBac expression to the PDF+CRY+ (Pdf-GAL4>UAS-NaChBac) neurons generated ∼50% of flies with complex rhythms, showing a major long-period and a minor short-period component. Moreover, those ∼30%–40% individuals with a single activity rhythm had a period 1.5–2.5 hr longer than controls (Figure 3A; Table S2). NaChBac overexpression in PDF+CRY+ induces longer molecular cycles in these cells and directly increases their output, explaining the origin of the major, slower (longer period) activity component [22, 23]. Immunocytochemistry experiments have also indicated that the DN2s show a faster cycle in both controls and NaChBac-overexpressing flies [19]. Thus, it is tempting to speculate that fast PDFCRY neurons (we cannot exclude that neurons in addition to the DN2s might also be involved) might be indirectly activated through a positive, PDF-independent connection linking the PDF+CRY+ and the PDFCRY cells and be responsible for the minor, short activity component detected in this genotype (Figures 2B and S2).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Locomotor Activity Profiles after Manipulation of Clock Neurons

Average locomotor activity profiles of flies showing 3–4 days in LD 12:12 and 12 days in DD. Genotypes and statistics are reported in Table S2.

(A) NaChBac overexpression, line NaChBac4 is shown. Control: w; +/ UAS-NaChBac4;+/+.

(B) Kir2.1 overexpression, line Kir2.1(III) is shown. Control: w; +/+; UAS-Kir2.1/+.

(C) HID, RPR overexpression. Control: w, UAS-hid,UAS-rpr; +/+;+/+.

(D) CLKΔ overexpression, line CLKΔ1 is shown. Control: w; UAS-ClkΔ1/+; +/+.

Only 13%–25% of PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > NaChBac (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-NaChBac) flies showed single rhythms (Figure 3A; Table S2). This result supports the hypothesis that the PDFCRY cells might inhibit the PDF+CRY+ neurons, explaining why they do not sustain behavioral rhythms in this genotype (Figure 2B). Subsequently we tested PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > NaChBac flies (tim-GAL42xPdf-GAL80 > UAS-NaChBac; Figure 3A; Table S2). Now, about 70%–80% of individuals had a single activity component with a period 0.5–1 hr longer than controls. This implies that the addition of the PDFCRY‡ group (which is not included in the largely arrhythmic tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-NaChBac genotype) to the ensemble of activated neurons results in the inhibition of the output of the PDFCRY cells (because of the longer activity period) and of the PDFCRY cells (because the majority of flies are rhythmic). Possibly, the latter inhibition is indirect, in agreement with our previous hypothesis (Figure 2B).

The expression of NaChBac in PDF+CRY+ ∩ PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY (cry13-GAL4 > UAS-NaChBac) neurons caused a decrease in the number of rhythmic individuals and a shortening in the activity period (now reaching wild-type values for single-rhythm flies) compared to the PDF+CRY+ > NaChBac (PdfGAL4 > UAS-NaChBac) genotype (Figure 3A; Table S2). This result is different from the comparison between the same genotypes for CRYΔ flies, which were both rhythmic with a period about 1 hr longer than controls (Figure 1; Table S1). The different strength of the two activators likely accounts for these discrepancies, which in general result from exaggerated effects on period and on rhythmicity following NaChBac overexpression (Figures S3).

We also applied the opposite manipulation, namely the overexpression of the mammalian inward rectifier K+ channel KIR2.1, a tool for decreasing membrane excitability in vivo [24, 25]. PDF+CRY+ > KIR2.1 (Pdf-GAL4 > UAS-kir2.1) resulted in more than a third of the flies being arrhythmic and in the tendency for a shorter period for those showing a single rhythm. This argues for a disruption in the balance of the network and, in the flies still rhythmic, for a tilt toward the faster PDFCRY neurons. Extending the misexpression of KIR2.1 to include also the PDFCRY‡ and the PDFCRY cells (cry13-GAL4 > UAS-kir2.1) abolished these effects (Figure 3B; Table S2). Finally, we tested the expression of KIR2.1 in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY neurons (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-kir2.1), but this genotype was not viable.

We then overexpressed the proapoptotic genes head involution defective (hid) and reaper (rpr) [26] in PDF+CRY+ cells (Pdf-GAL4 > UAS-hid, UAS-rpr) and observed that 50% of the flies were equally either arrhythmic or showed complex rhythms. The remaining half cycled with a period of locomotor activity ∼1 hr shorter than controls. Expression of HID and RPR in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY cells (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-hid, UAS-rpr) resulted in poor viability and ∼60% of the surviving flies were arrhythmic, further underscoring the importance of these neurons to the network (Figure 3C; Table S2). The remaining flies showed complex or single rhythms, the latter having periods on average intermediate to their corresponding controls (Table S2). Although we did not verify this experimentally, it is likely that viable and rhythmic flies must have largely escaped the apoptotic response triggered by HID and RPR. This explanation is in line with the lethality of the tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-kir2.1 genotype and with previously reported variability in the induction of the apoptotic response in neurons [27].

Driving the production of the dominant-negative CLKΔ mutant [28] in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY cells (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-ClkΔ) resulted in virtually complete arrhythmicity. Thus, as with NachBac and HID-RPR overexpression, disrupting (independently of mechanism) the PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY groups generated a largely arrhythmic profile, which argues against a simple dominant effect of the PDF+CRY+ cells on the neuronal network (Figure 3D; Table S2). The expression of CLKΔ in PDF+CRY+ cells caused 20%–50% arrhythmicity and 10%–30% of complex rhythms, depending on the line. The remaining 40%–50% of flies showed a single rhythm of activity with a period 0.5–1 hr shorter than controls. This is in line with an increased influence of the PDFCRY cells on the network. By expressing CLKΔ in PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY cells in addition to the PDF+CRY+ group (cry13-GAL4 > UAS-ClkΔ), we increased the ratio of single-rhythm flies to 72%–94% and reverted to a period closer to that of the parental control strains, but slightly shorter (Figure 3D; Table S2). Thus, as previously seen with the overexpression of KIR2.1, we could rescue rhythmicity in flies with weakened PDF+CRY+ neurons by reducing the inhibitory influence of the PDFCRY cells on them and by buffering changes in activation of the PDFCRY‡ cells. Again, this shows that the PDF+CRY+ cells do not act alone in determining circadian rhythmicity and its period. We were unable to obtain viable PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CLKΔ (tim-GAL4, ∩ Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-ClkΔ) adults but PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY > CLKΔ (cry13-GAL4,Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-ClkΔ) flies were rhythmic with periods indistinguishable from controls (Figure 3D; Table S2). This result is the same as that observed with CRYΔ, again suggesting that the PDFCRY‡ and the PDFCRY cells inhibit the faster PDFCRY and the slower PDF+CRY+ cycling groups respectively, so the balance of the network is undisturbed (Figure 2B).

Expression of CRYΔ Sets off Nonautonomous Effects on Cellular Clocks

CRYΔ flies with a long behavioral period exhibit a delay in the cycling of clock proteins in PDF+CRY+ neurons [5]. Here we investigated the cellular changes caused by CRYΔ overexpression in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-cryΔ) and in PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY (tim-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-cryΔ) neurons, resulting in short and normal (compensatory) activity rhythms, respectively. We analyzed the cycling of the clock protein PDP1ε in the major classes of clock neurons by IF. Figure 4 shows the staining index (SI) for PDP1 ε at CT0, CT6, CT12, and CT18 (circadian time, CT0 = subjective lights ON, CT12 = subjective lights OFF) during day 2 and 5 in DD.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

PDP1ε Immunoreactivity after Expression of CRYΔ in Different Groups of Neurons

Staining index (SI, error bars correspond to the standard error of the mean) for PDP1ε at CT0, CT6, CT12, and CT18 during day 2 (green) and 5 (magenta) in DD. ANOVA showed a significant effect (p < 0.05) of time of day on SI values, with the following exceptions. Control CRYΔ, DN3s at day 5. PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, LNds at day 5 and DN3s at day 5. PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, LNds at day 5 and DN3s at day 2 and 5. For the two CRYΔ-expressing genotypes, we also tested the interaction among factors by comparing the SI of cell types s-LNvs, DN1s, and DN2s at different time points, at day 2 and 5. (PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, ANOVA, Day, F1,215 = 31.09, p < < 0.01; Time, F3,215 = 24.83, p < < 0.01; Cell type, F2,215 = 6.29, p < 0.01; DayTime, F3,215 = 18.26, p < < 0.01; DayCell type, F2,215 = 0.88, p = 0.42; TimeCell type, F6,215 = 4.50, p < < 0.01; DayTimeCell type, F6,215 = 3.50, p < 0.01. PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, ANOVA, Day, F1,193 = 2.33, p = 0.13; Time, F3,193 = 50.73, p < < 0.01; Cell type, F2,193 = 36.67, p < < 0.01; DayTime, F3,193 = 2.71, p = 0.046; DayCell type, F2,193 = 0.83, p = 0.44; TimeCell type, F6,193 = 3.06, p < 0.01; DayTimeCell type, F6,193 = 0.82, p = 0.56.) PDP1ε did not cycle in l-LNvs, so they are not shown. We did not measure the PDF-negative LNv and the lateral posterior neurons (LPNs) because we were unable to identify them unequivocally in all preparations. For each cluster of neurons, we calculated the average SI per hemisphere (each considered as an independent observation) and we used those values for statistical comparisons. The number of hemispheres analyzed are given below as GENOTYPE, DAY [cell type1 (time points), cell type2 (time points), etc.]. Cell types are s-LNvs, LNds, DN1s, DN2s, DN3s, respectively. Time point are CTO, CT6, CT12, CT18, respectively. Control CRYΔ, DD2 [(9, 10, 11, 8), (9, 8, 11, 7), (10, 9, 11, 9), (10, 6, 10, 8), (10, 10, 10, 6]; DD5 [(11, 9, 12, 14), (11, 9, 11, 14), (9, 10, 9, 11), (10, 5, 9, 10), (10, 9, 10, 12)]. PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, DD2 [(11, 9, 10, 9), (11, 9, 10, 10), (12, 7, 10, 10), (11, 8, 9, 10), (11, 8, 8, 10)]; DD5 [(11, 10, 12, 9), (11, 10, 13, 11), (10, 10, 10, 11), (10, 9, 10, 11), (9, 10, 10, 8)]. PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, DD2 [(9, 13, 10, 8), (9, 10, 10, 8), (8, 7, 10, 8), (8, 5, 10, 7), (8, 7, 9, 8)]; DD5 [(11, 10, 10, 11), (12, 11, 10, 11), (9, 9, 8, 11), (9, 6, 8, 12), (10, 9, 9, 9)]. Genotypes: Control CRYΔ, w, UAS-cryΔ14.6; +/+; +/+. PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, w, UAS-cryΔ14.6; tim-GAL4/+; cry-GAL802e3m/+. PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ, w, UAS-cryΔ14.6; tim-GAL4/Pdf-GAL8096a; +/+.

Control flies (UAS-cryΔ) showed, at day 2, maximum SI at about CT18 for all cell types. At day 5 the SI peaked also at CT18 for s-LNvs, LNds, and DN1s, suggesting that these cells cycle with a period of about 24 hr. Instead the SI maximum was reached earlier (about CT12) by the DN2s, suggesting that these neurons run with a shorter period of ∼22 hr. These cells also showed a pronounced reduction in their cycling amplitude compared to the other clusters. The DN3 did not cycle at day 5. Thus, in control flies, s-LNvs, LNds, and DN1s showed molecular oscillations that were compatible with the corresponding behavioral period attributed to them.

The overexpression of CRYΔ in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY cells (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-cryΔ) targeted directly the DN2s, the large majority of DN1s, the DN3s, and half of the LNds. The DN1s and even more so the DN2s showed a phase advance in PDP1ε immune staining at day 2; at day 5 both groups reached their maximum at around CT6. However, while the DN1s experienced a shortening of their period (in this genotype compared to the control), the DN2s maintained the same faster rhythm seen in wild-type flies. The DN3 did not show significant cycling at day 5. The LNds were considered as a group because we could not distinguish the three PDFCRY‡ from the three PDFCRY cells (we could not discriminate them by anti-CRY staining because both the anti-CRY and the anti-PDP1ε antibodies available to us are made in rabbit), which limits our interpretation of these data. Overall, the LNds showed a reduction in cycling amplitude especially at day 5, perhaps resulting from averaging different cycling profiles in the two groups. The s-LNvs experienced phase advance and shortening of their rhythm such that by day 5 they reached the SI maximum at CT6 (although because of the 6 hr resolution of our data, the actual peak might be a few hours later). Thus, in this genotype, the s-LNvs, the DN1s, and the DN2s were synchronous and showed endogenous cycling compatible with the short behavioral period. A significant “time × cell type” ANOVA interaction (we compared the SI of cell types s-LNvs, DN1s, and DN2s, at different time points, at day 2 and 5; see legend to Figure 4) provides support to the view that the advance in the phase of PDP1ε expression occurred first in the DN2s, then in the DN1s, and finally in the s-LNvs. The changes above appeared first at day 2 and were consolidated at day 5, confirmed with a significant “day × time × cell type” ANOVA interaction (Figure 4).

We then overexpressed CRYΔ in PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY cells (tim-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-cryΔ), namely in the DNs and in both types of LNds (the pn-LNv was also affected but it has not been analyzed here). Extending the expression of CRYΔ to the PDFCRY‡ cells caused a broadening in the expression profile of PDP1ε in the DNs, supporting our hypothesis that the PDFCRY‡ modulate the PDFCRY cells. Especially at day 2, the levels of PDP1ε started to rise earlier (compared to controls) than for PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ flies, but declined later in all dorsal neurons, suggesting a combination of phase advance and phase delay inputs. The DN2s maintained their shorter molecular cycling while the DN1s and seemingly the DN3s moved to a ∼24 hr period. The LNds, also expressing CRYΔ directly, showed the most pronounced phase delay (supported by a significant “time × cell type” ANOVA interaction) and a reduction in cycling amplitude (although smaller than for the previous genotype). Those differences were maintained at day 5, in agreement with a nonsignificant “day × time × cell type” ANOVA interaction (see legend to Figure 4). The profile of PDP1ε IR in the s-LNvs (not expressing CRYΔ) remained unchanged at day 2 and 5, which, compared to the controls, corresponds to a small delay in phase but no change in period. In this genotype, only the sLNvs and the DN1s showed robust endogenous cycling compatible with the behavioral period.

Overall, only the s-LNvs and the DN1s consistently showed cellular cycling matching the behavioral periods of the flies in all genotypes. We suggest that locomotor activity with simple rhythmicity requires synchronization between these two groups of neurons, although the contribution of neurons from other clusters may also be significant. In the PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-cryΔ) flies, the faster rhythm of the DN2s and perhaps of additional unidentified fast PDFCRY cells was shared by DN1s and s-LNvs. In PDFCRY‡ ∩ PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRYΔ (tim-GAL4Pdf-GAL8096a > UAS-cryΔ) individuals, the DN2s were still cycling with a faster pace but, as in wild-type flies, the shorter period did not spread to DN1s and s-LNvs, suggesting this is the role of the highly PDF-responsive PDFCRY‡ cells (included in the second but not in the first genotype). The expressions of CRYΔ in the DN1ps only [29] did not result in faster behavioral rhythms, showing that the shorter period originates from a different group of neurons (Table S3); see Figure 2C for a possible scenario.

Network Perturbations Can Drive Changes in the Period of Locomotor Activity Independently of Development, although Ontogeny Is Not without Effect

The behavioral changes discussed above might depend on development. We employed Pdf-Geneswitch (Pdf-GS) [30] to trigger drug-dependent expression of CRYΔ in PDF+CRY+ neurons either in adults only (acute) or through the whole development (chronic). For acute treatment, flies were fed for 48 hr from the day of eclosion with medium supplemented with either 200 μg/ml of the drug RU486 (induction) or with an equal volume of vehicle (80% ethanol, no induction). Note, all flies were raised on this medium and were then examined (on their respective medium) for locomotor activity rhythms. For chronic treatment, flies were exposed to either drug or vehicle during their whole life and adult locomotor activity was examined accordingly. As controls, we tested (heterozygous) driver and effector flies raised under acute or chronic exposure to drug or on vehicle only (Table S4). Under both treatment conditions, we observed a significant, albeit small, lengthening of the period of locomotor activity for CRYΔ-overexpressing flies compared to controls (Figures 5A and 5B).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Network Perturbations Independently of Development

(A and B) Expression of CRYΔ in PDF+CRY+ neurons after activation of the Geneswitch system by the drug RU486.

(A) Average locomotor activity profiles (3 days LD, 8 days DD) of Geneswitch PDF+CRY+ > CRYΔ flies that were never subjected to drug treatment (DO), that were subjected to treatment as adults only (DA), or that were exposed to the drug since early development (DC).

(B) The period of locomotor activity of Geneswitch PDF+CRY+ > CRYΔ flies and their parental controls were compared across the three different treatments: DO (black), DA (red), and DC (blue). ANOVA showed a nonsignificant effect of genotype (F2,477 = 0.554, p = 0.58) but a significant effect of treatment (F2,477 = 7.22, p < 0.01) and (asterisk) of the interaction term (genotype × treatment, F4,477 = 2.53, p = 0.04). Post-hoc analyses revealed significant differences comparing DA (Bonferroni, p < 0.01) and DC (Bonferroni, p = 0.04) to DO but not between DA and DC (Bonferroni, p = 1).

Genotypes: Geneswitch PDF+CRY+ > CRYΔ, w, UAS-cryΔ14.6; UAS-CD8GFP/+; Pdf-GS/+; Control 1, w; UAS-CD8GFP/+; Pdf-GS/+; Control 2, w, UAS-cryΔ14.6; +/+; +/+. See also Table S4.

(C and D) Expression of the temperature sensitive cation channel TRPA1 in PDF+CRY+ and in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY cells under restrictive (18°C) and permissive (28°C) temperature.

(C) Average locomotor activity profiles (3 days LD, 7 days DD) of both genotypes under both conditions.

(D) The period of locomotor activity of PDF+CRY+ > TRPA1 (top) and PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > TRPA1 (bottom) flies were compared, at the two temperatures (18°C, blue and 28°C, red), to their parental controls. The increase in period length for PDF+CRY+ > TRPA1 flies at higher temperature did not reach significance compared to controls as ANOVA showed a significant effect of genotype (F2,104 = 57.69, p < < 0.01) and temperature (F1,104 = 9.35, p < 0.01) but not of the interaction term (genotype × temperature, F2,104 = 1.45, p = 0.24). Conversely, we observed a significant decrease (asterisk) in period length for PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > TRPA1 flies at higher temperature (ANOVA, genotype, F2,75 = 6.22, p < 0.01; temperature, F1,75 = 6.20, p = 0.02; genotype × temperature, F2,75 = 6.32, p < 0.01). Genotypes: PDF+CRY+ > TRPA1, yw; Pdf-GAL4/+; +/UAS-TrpA1; Control 1, yw; Pdf-GAL4/+; +/+; Control 2 w; +/+; +/ UAS-TrpA1; PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > TRPA1 yw; tim-GAL4/+; cry-GAL802e3m/ UAS-TrpA1; Control 3, w; tim-GAL4/+; cry-GAL802e3m/+. See also Table S4.

As a further test we directed the expression of the temperature-sensitive cation channel TRPA1 in PDF+CRY+ and in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY cells (Figures 5C and 5D; Table S4). This channel is inactive at temperatures below 25°C in Drosophila and originally it was reported as being endogenously expressed in the adult brain in only about a dozen cells, which are not part of the circadian system [31]. However, recent evidence has shown that trpa1 transcription does occur in at least some cells in each cluster of clock neurons but that the effects of trpa1-null mutations on rhythmic behavior are quite modest [32]. Flies overexpressing TRPA1 and (heterozygous) parental controls were raised at 18°C (TRPA1 is inactive) and then tested as adults at 18°C and 28°C (TRPA1 is active). Compared to controls, we would expect a longer and a shorter period for PDF+CRY+ > TRPA1 (Pdf-GAL4 > UAS-TrpA1) and PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > TRPA1 (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-TrpA1) flies at 28°C, respectively. For PDF+CRY+ > TRPA1 flies, the increase in period length with temperature was comparable to controls (Figures 5C and 5D). Conversely, we observed a significant decrease in period length for PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > TRPA1 flies at higher temperature but not for controls (Figures 5C and 5D).

In summary, the manipulation of the circadian network limited to adults can affect the free-running period of locomotor activity. However, the modest effects obtained with adult PDF+CRY+ cells suggest that the ontogeny of the clock is particularly sensitive to the status of these neurons. Similar conclusions were reached by Depetris-Chauvin et al. [30] and Gorostiza and Ceriani [33]. Conversely, here we show that the PDFCRY and the PDFCRY cells appear to be less sensitive to developmental effects (Figures 5C and 5D).

Confirming the Model

Stoleru et al. [13] had shown that overexpression of a constitutive active form of the kinase SHAGGY (SGG), although able to shorten the period of locomotor activity when driven in PDF+CRY+ cells, was unable to elicit a behavioral effect when directed in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY neurons [13]. This was in spite of causing a faster molecular oscillation in some cells, for instance the DN2s [13], and was considered evidence that those cells are not relevant for free-running behavior. This explanation contradicts our model, so we investigated further.

PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > SGG (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-sgg) flies showed a free-running period of locomotor activity no different from controls, in agreement with previous results [13] (Figure 6A; Table S5). However, a visual inspection of the activity profiles revealed that these flies, both in LD and DD, showed earlier anticipation of the actual or subjective dark-to-light transition (Figure 6B). Perhaps this anticipation of phase reflects a tendency for the shorter rhythm to propagate to the network, which instead is prevented by other neuronal groups. Previous work has suggested that there is a physical and functional interaction between CRY and SGG [14]. There is little or no CRY in PDFCRY and PDFCRY neurons, which could preclude SGG from exerting any robust effect. According to our model, the PDFCRY repress the PDF+CRY+ cells; thus, increasing the output of the former should reduce the resistance of the network to the propagation of the faster rhythm. We coexpressed CRY and SGG and indeed observed a faster free-running period of locomotor activity in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > SGG, CRY (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-sgg, UAS-cry) but not in PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRY (tim-GAL4cry-GAL802e3m > UAS-cry) flies (Figure 6C; Table S5). These results reveal that the DN2s are indeed part of the circuit that generates self-sustained behavior, although they do not exclude that the establishment of a faster rhythm might require additional PDFCRY neurons. Implicitly, they also contradict the view of a defined hierarchical organization of the clock under constant conditions.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Ectopic Expression of CRY Reveals a Functional Interaction with SGG

(A) Average locomotor activity profiles (4 days LD, 11 days DD) of PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > SGG flies and controls.

(B) Same data as in (A) but limited to the last 2 days of LD and first 2 days of DD. The profile for PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > SGG flies (blue) shows earlier anticipation of the dark-to-light transitions (blue arrows) compared to the other genotypes (PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY Control, red and SGG Control, green).

(C) Average locomotor activity profiles (4 days LD, 11 days DD) of PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRY, CRY Control and PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > SGG, CRY flies. Only the latter genotype showed a shorter period of locomotor activity (see Table S5). Genotypes: PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > SGG, w, UAS-sgg; tim-GAL4/+; cry-GAL802e3m/ +; SGG Control, w, UAS-sgg; +/+; +/+; PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY Control, w; tim-GAL4/+; cry-GAL802e3m/+; PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > CRY, w; tim-GAL4/+; cry-GAL802e3m/UAS-HAcry16.1; CRY Control, w; +/+; +/UAS-HAcry16.1; PDFCRY ∩ PDFCRY > SGG, CRY, w, UAS-sgg; tim-GAL4/+; cry-GAL802e3m/UAS-HAcry16.1. See also Table S5.

Considering all results reported above, we favor a model based on a flexible network of circadian neurons, which operates under any environmental condition. Our model complements and extends evidence by others of network organization as opposed to hierarchical dominance among neurons under LD and DD conditions [34, 35, 36].

Conclusions

We have used genetic manipulations limited to discrete groups of neurons to unveil the logic governing self-sustained rhythmic locomotor activity in Drosophila. Activity rhythms and observations of molecular cycling in defined cellular groups consistently support a model (Figure 2) where endogenous behavioral rhythmicity is based upon synchronization between PDF+CRY+ and PDFCRY cells. This requires modulation of the PDFCRY cells by the PDF+CRY+ neurons, largely but not exclusively via PDF signaling, and multistep feedback adjustment of the latter group of neurons by the former. The behavioral outcomes of manipulations that alter but do not destroy the equilibrium of the network reveal that the PDF+CRY+ cells have a tendency for driving rhythms longer than 24 hr whereas some PDFCRY cells promote rhythms shorter than 24 hr. However, the organization of the network is such that wild-type flies reach a combined ∼24 hr oscillation. This requires the PDFCRY‡ and the PDFCRY cells that tune, through inhibitory interactions, the relative contribution of the other two groups of neurons to the network. We note that the interposition of the PDFCRY‡ and the PDFCRY cells adds delay and signal amplification to the feedback regulation that links the PDF+CRY+ to the PDFCRY neurons and vice versa.

In conclusion, we have shown that the endogenous behavioral period reflects the nonadditive interplay of all clock cells and not the dominant action of a single group of neurons (i.e., the s-LNvs), as previously suggested. Second, our overall results suggest that intercellular coupling is fundamental for synchronizing different cellular oscillators in a coherent clock, and therefore additional elements must complement the known molecular model focused around transcriptional regulators. Finally, we have shown that excitatory and inhibitory interactions are instrumental for changing the strength and the time delay in the coupling among neurons, resulting in different (collective) behavioral periods. This suggests that contrary to current belief, the period of circadian rhythms is not a fixed feature genetically encoded in a group of neurons. We propose that it is an emerging property of a network of multiple, different circadian oscillators and that it is the wiring of the system, whose logic is genetically encoded, that determines period length. Perhaps not surprisingly, the same principles of negative feedback with amplification and delay, which are central to the TTL model, re-emerge in the constitution of the clock at the intercellular level. We hope this view will inspire formal modeling of the Drosophila circadian network. The development of novel tools will be required to investigate how the known molecular mechanisms translate into different cellular features and for the experimental validation of the “logical” connections we postulate in our model. These matters will be the subject of our future work.

Author Contributions

S.D. and E.R. generated the hypotheses. S.D., C.N.H., and E.R. designed the experiments. S.D., C.N.H., Ö.Ö., M.H., and E.R. performed experiments and analyzed data. S.D., C.P.K., and E.R. wrote the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We thank J. Blau, P. Emery, P. Hardin, K. Moffat, M. Rosbash, F. Rouyer, A. Sehgal, F. Ceriani, and R. Stanewsky for reagents. The α-PDF monoclonal antibody developed by J. Blau was obtained from the Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank developed under the auspices of the NICHD and maintained by Department of Biology, The University of Iowa. We acknowledge funding from the BBSRC (S.D. and C.N.H., grants BB/C003241/1 and BB/F008988/1; M.H., doctoral studentship) and the NC3Rs (Ö.Ö., G1100597).

Published: September 11, 2014

Footnotes

This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/).

Supplemental Information includes three figures, five tables, and Supplemental Experimental Procedures and can be found with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.023.

Supplemental Information

Document S1. Figures S1–S3, Tables S1–S5, and Supplemental Experimental Procedures
mmc1.pdf (746.9KB, pdf)
Document S2. Article plus Supplemental Information
mmc2.pdf (3.3MB, pdf)

References

  • 1.Özkaya Ö., Rosato E. The circadian clock of the fly: a neurogenetics journey through time. Adv. Genet. 2012;77:79–123. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-387687-4.00004-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Stanewsky R., Kaneko M., Emery P., Beretta B., Wager-Smith K., Kay S.A., Rosbash M., Hall J.C. The cryb mutation identifies cryptochrome as a circadian photoreceptor in Drosophila. Cell. 1998;95:681–692. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81638-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Emery P., So W.V., Kaneko M., Hall J.C., Rosbash M. CRY, a Drosophila clock and light-regulated cryptochrome, is a major contributor to circadian rhythm resetting and photosensitivity. Cell. 1998;95:669–679. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81637-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rosato E., Codd V., Mazzotta G., Piccin A., Zordan M., Costa R., Kyriacou C.P. Light-dependent interaction between Drosophila CRY and the clock protein PER mediated by the carboxy terminus of CRY. Curr. Biol. 2001;11:909–917. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(01)00259-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dissel S., Codd V., Fedic R., Garner K.J., Costa R., Kyriacou C.P., Rosato E. A constitutively active cryptochrome in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:834–840. doi: 10.1038/nn1285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mazzotta G., Rossi A., Leonardi E., Mason M., Bertolucci C., Caccin L., Spolaore B., Martin A.J., Schlichting M., Grebler R. Fly cryptochrome and the visual system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:6163–6168. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212317110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Yoshii T., Todo T., Wülbeck C., Stanewsky R., Helfrich-Förster C. Cryptochrome is present in the compound eyes and a subset of Drosophila’s clock neurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 2008;508:952–966. doi: 10.1002/cne.21702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dolezelova E., Dolezel D., Hall J.C. Rhythm defects caused by newly engineered null mutations in Drosophila’s cryptochrome gene. Genetics. 2007;177:329–345. doi: 10.1534/genetics.107.076513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fogle K.J., Parson K.G., Dahm N.A., Holmes T.C. CRYPTOCHROME is a blue-light sensor that regulates neuronal firing rate. Science. 2011;331:1409–1413. doi: 10.1126/science.1199702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Helfrich-Förster C., Yoshii T., Wülbeck C., Grieshaber E., Rieger D., Bachleitner W., Cusamano P., Rouyer F. The lateral and dorsal neurons of Drosophila melanogaster: new insights about their morphology and function. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 2007;72:517–525. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2007.72.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stoleru D., Peng Y., Agosto J., Rosbash M. Coupled oscillators control morning and evening locomotor behaviour of Drosophila. Nature. 2004;431:862–868. doi: 10.1038/nature02926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Grima B., Chélot E., Xia R., Rouyer F. Morning and evening peaks of activity rely on different clock neurons of the Drosophila brain. Nature. 2004;431:869–873. doi: 10.1038/nature02935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Stoleru D., Peng Y., Nawathean P., Rosbash M. A resetting signal between Drosophila pacemakers synchronizes morning and evening activity. Nature. 2005;438:238–242. doi: 10.1038/nature04192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Stoleru D., Nawathean P., Fernández M.P., Menet J.S., Ceriani M.F., Rosbash M. The Drosophila circadian network is a seasonal timer. Cell. 2007;129:207–219. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.02.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sehgal A., Rothenfluh-Hilfiker A., Hunter-Ensor M., Chen Y., Myers M.P., Young M.W. Rhythmic expression of timeless: a basis for promoting circadian cycles in period gene autoregulation. Science. 1995;270:808–810. doi: 10.1126/science.270.5237.808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Renn S.C., Park J.H., Rosbash M., Hall J.C., Taghert P.H. A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell. 1999;99:791–802. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81676-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Emery P., Stanewsky R., Helfrich-Förster C., Emery-Le M., Hall J.C., Rosbash M. Drosophila CRY is a deep brain circadian photoreceptor. Neuron. 2000;26:493–504. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81181-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Shafer O.T., Helfrich-Förster C., Renn S.C., Taghert P.H. Reevaluation of Drosophila melanogaster’s neuronal circadian pacemakers reveals new neuronal classes. J. Comp. Neurol. 2006;498:180–193. doi: 10.1002/cne.21021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nitabach M.N., Wu Y., Sheeba V., Lemon W.C., Strumbos J., Zelensky P.K., White B.H., Holmes T.C. Electrical hyperexcitation of lateral ventral pacemaker neurons desynchronizes downstream circadian oscillators in the fly circadian circuit and induces multiple behavioral periods. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:479–489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3915-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vanin S., Bhutani S., Montelli S., Menegazzi P., Green E.W., Pegoraro M., Sandrelli F., Costa R., Kyriacou C.P. Unexpected features of Drosophila circadian behavioural rhythms under natural conditions. Nature. 2012;484:371–375. doi: 10.1038/nature10991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Menegazzi P., Vanin S., Yoshii T., Rieger D., Hermann C., Dusik V., Kyriacou C.P., Helfrich-Förster C., Costa R. Drosophila clock neurons under natural conditions. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2013;28:3–14. doi: 10.1177/0748730412471303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sheeba V., Fogle K.J., Kaneko M., Rashid S., Chou Y.T., Sharma V.K., Holmes T.C. Large ventral lateral neurons modulate arousal and sleep in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:1537–1545. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sheeba V., Sharma V.K., Gu H., Chou Y.T., O’Dowd D.K., Holmes T.C. Pigment dispersing factor-dependent and -independent circadian locomotor behavioral rhythms. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:217–227. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4087-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Baines R.A., Uhler J.P., Thompson A., Sweeney S.T., Bate M. Altered electrical properties in Drosophila neurons developing without synaptic transmission. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:1523–1531. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-05-01523.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nitabach M.N., Blau J., Holmes T.C. Electrical silencing of Drosophila pacemaker neurons stops the free-running circadian clock. Cell. 2002;109:485–495. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(02)00737-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cusumano P., Klarsfeld A., Chélot E., Picot M., Richier B., Rouyer F. PDF-modulated visual inputs and cryptochrome define diurnal behavior in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1431–1437. doi: 10.1038/nn.2429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Thum A.S., Knapek S., Rister J., Dierichs-Schmitt E., Heisenberg M., Tanimoto H. Differential potencies of effector genes in adult Drosophila. J. Comp. Neurol. 2006;498:194–203. doi: 10.1002/cne.21022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Tanoue S., Krishnan P., Krishnan B., Dryer S.E., Hardin P.E. Circadian clocks in antennal neurons are necessary and sufficient for olfaction rhythms in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2004;14:638–649. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Zhang L., Chung B.Y., Lear B.C., Kilman V.L., Liu Y., Mahesh G., Meissner R.A., Hardin P.E., Allada R. DN1(p) circadian neurons coordinate acute light and PDF inputs to produce robust daily behavior in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:591–599. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Depetris-Chauvin A., Berni J., Aranovich E.J., Muraro N.I., Beckwith E.J., Ceriani M.F. Adult-specific electrical silencing of pacemaker neurons uncouples molecular clock from circadian outputs. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:1783–1793. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hamada F.N., Rosenzweig M., Kang K., Pulver S.R., Ghezzi A., Jegla T.J., Garrity P.A. An internal thermal sensor controlling temperature preference in Drosophila. Nature. 2008;454:217–220. doi: 10.1038/nature07001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lee Y., Montell C. Drosophila TRPA1 functions in temperature control of circadian rhythm in pacemaker neurons. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:6716–6725. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4237-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gorostiza E.A., Ceriani M.F. Retrograde bone morphogenetic protein signaling shapes a key circadian pacemaker circuit. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:687–696. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3448-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sheeba V., Fogle K.J., Holmes T.C. Persistence of morning anticipation behavior and high amplitude morning startle response following functional loss of small ventral lateral neurons in Drosophila. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e11628. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Potdar S., Sheeba V. Large ventral lateral neurons determine the phase of evening activity peak across photoperiods in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Biol. Rhythms. 2012;27:267–279. doi: 10.1177/0748730412449820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Yao Z., Shafer O.T. The Drosophila circadian clock is a variably coupled network of multiple peptidergic units. Science. 2014;343:1516–1520. doi: 10.1126/science.1251285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Im S.H., Li W., Taghert P.H. PDFR and CRY signaling converge in a subset of clock neurons to modulate the amplitude and phase of circadian behavior in Drosophila. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18974. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Document S1. Figures S1–S3, Tables S1–S5, and Supplemental Experimental Procedures
mmc1.pdf (746.9KB, pdf)
Document S2. Article plus Supplemental Information
mmc2.pdf (3.3MB, pdf)

RESOURCES