Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2014 Oct 7;9(10):e109564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109564

Female and Male Moths Display Different Reproductive Behavior when Facing New versus Previous Mates

Yan-Ying Li 1,#, Jin-Feng Yu 1,#, Qin Lu 2, Jin Xu 1,*, Hui Ye 2,*
Editor: Claude Wicker-Thomas3
PMCID: PMC4188817  PMID: 25290195

Abstract

Multiple mating allows females to obtain material (more sperm and nutrient) and/or genetic benefits. The genetic benefit models require sperm from different males to fertilize eggs competitively or the offspring be fathered by multiple males. To maximize genetic benefits from multiple mating, females have evolved strategies to prefer novel versus previous mates in their subsequent matings. However, the reproductive behavior during mate encounter, mate choice and egg laying in relation to discrimination and preference between sexes has been largely neglected. In the present study, we used novel and previous mate treatments and studied male and female behavior and reproductive output in Spodoptera litura. The results of this study do not support the sperm and nutrient replenishment hypotheses because neither the number of mates nor the number of copulations achieved by females significantly increased female fecundity, fertility and longevity. However, females showed different oviposition patterns when facing new versus previous mates by slowing down oviposition, which allows the last male has opportunities to fertilize her eggs and the female to promote offspring diversity. Moreover, females that have novel males present called earlier and more than females that have their previous mates present, whereas no significant differences were found on male courtship between treatments. These results suggest that S. litura females can distinguish novel from previous mates and prefer the former, whereas males generally remate regardless of whether the female is a previous mate or not. In S. litura, eggs are laid in large clusters and offspring competition, inbreeding and disease transfer risks are thus increased. Therefore, offspring diversity should be valuable for S. litura, and genetic benefits should be the main force behind the evolution of female behavioral strategies found in the present study.

Introduction

Mating is very costly due to energy consumption, the risks of disease transmission and predation, and injury caused during mating [1], [2]. Both theoretical [3] and empirical (e.g., [4], [5]) studies have demonstrated that one copulation is adequate for females to obtain their maximum reproductive success in many insect species. However, the majority of female insects prefer to mate multiply, not only with different males (multiple mating) but also with the same male (repeated mating) [1].

Two categories of hypotheses have been developed to explain the evolutionary significance of female multiple mating: to obtain material benefits and to gain genetic benefits. Females may obtain material benefits from multiple mating by one or more of the following ways [1], [6], [7]: 1) sperm replenishment – females remate to obtain adequate sperm to ensure her full load of eggs can be fertilized; 2) nutrient replenishment – females remate to obtain male invested nutrients for longer longevity or higher fecundity; and 3) convenience benefits – females remate to minimize the cost from male harassment.

A number of genetic benefits hypotheses have also been suggested to explain the evolutionary significance of female multiple mating: 1) good genes – paternally derived genes enhanced the attractiveness of offspring or genes from their parents improved the survival of offspring [8], [9]; 2) genetic incompatibility – females can bias paternity and give priority to males with good or more compatible genes to fertilize her eggs [10][13]; 3) genetic diversity – by increasing genetic diversity within progeny, females guard against future environmental uncertainty [14], [15], offspring benefit from enhanced genetic diversity by disease resistance, niche separation [16], [17] and inbreeding avoidance [4], [18].

To maximize the genetic benefits from multiple mating, females have evolved strategies to prefer novel versus previous mates in their subsequent matings [19]. Thus far, nine studies have investigated whether females can discriminate between novel and previous mates in their subsequent matings and found that it is positive in five invertebrate [4], [19][22] and two vertebrate [23], [24] species and negative only in two invertebrate species [25], [26]. Newcomer et al. [27] found that in the pseudoscorpion Cordylochernes scorpioides, females increased offspring viability by discriminating against previous mates. Archer & Elgar [21] and Xu & Wang [4] demonstrate that females of the hide beetle Dermestes maculatus and the Mediterranean flour moth Ephestia kuehniella choose new mates for remating to gain genetic benefits from increased offspring diversity. These two studies [4], [21] also found that a female might delay or slow down oviposition if has a novel male around after her first mating compared to a female still caged with her previous mate after her first mating.

Compared to females, it is generally accepted that multiple mating can significantly improve males’ reproductive success [28], [29]. Studies also found that males of many taxa, including insects, displayed a heightened preference for novel mates combined with a decline in the propensity to remating with the same mate [26]. However, two other studies in insects have found the opposite result: males significantly prefer previous to new mates for subsequent matings [19], [30]. Moreover, very few studies have tested the preference for novel mates in both males and females [26], making the reproductive behavior during mate encounter, mate choice and egg laying in relation to discrimination and preference between sexes be largely neglected.

The tobacco cutworm, Spodoptera litura (Fabricius, 1775) (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae), is one of the most serious agricultural pests worldwide [31]. This insect is a nocturnal moth and all adult sexual activities (courtship, calling, mating and oviposition) take place during the scotophase [32]. Adult moths eclose at dusk, and no matings take place during the night of eclosion. Approximately 70% of mating occurs on the subsequent night after emergence, and those unmated will mate on the third night [32], [33]. Paired insects can mate up to four times, with an average of 1.9±0.4 matings [32]. Females begin to lay eggs on the subsequent night after first mating [32], [33].

In the present study, we allow S. litura females and males to mate the first time during the second night after eclosion and then allow them to encounter novel or previous mates on the subsequent night to test female and male responses to mate-novelty. We also conducted single mating, repeated mating and multiple mating treatments to test the effect of the number of matings and mates on fecundity, fertility, oviposition pattern and longevity. Based on the behavioral and reproductive output test, we discussed the evolutionary significance of the behavioral strategies used by females when facing new versus previous mates.

Materials and Methods

Insects

Spodoptera litura were reared under a 14∶10 h light:dark photoperiod regime, at 26°C and 60–80% relative humidity. Larvae were fed on an artificial diet [34]; adults were reared on a 10% honey solution. This moth was found in Liujia Village, Kunming City of China in July 2012. The present study was conducted in May 2013. The insect has eight generations been in the lab before this study.

Pupae were sexed based on the morphology of exterior parameres [35]. Male and female pupae were caged separately and allowed to emerge to ensure virginity. Newly emerged moths (<12 h old) were collected from the colony and weighed to an accuracy of 0.0001 g using an electronic balance (Sartorius Bp221S, Germany). Mean body weight (mean ± SD) was 148.9±17.3 mg and 189.3±22.8 mg for male and female moths, respectively. Only moths with an average body weight (within one SD of the mean) [33] were used in this study.

Effect of multiple mating on female reproductive behavior and success

On the second night after eclosion, male and female virgin moths were paired in plastic boxes (25 cm long, 15 cm wide, 8 cm high) for the whole night for mating, using one pair per box (n = 150). Females and males were seperated immediately after mating and were reared individually in new boxes (no oviposition was found on this night). At the beginning of the subsequent night (i.e., the third night after eclosion), the one-time mated females and males were used for further tests following different treatments in new boxes: (1) a female was individually caged for the remainder of her lifespan (1 male 1 mating) (n = 13), (2) a female was re-caged with her previous mate for the remainder of their lifespan (Paired) (n = 12), this treatment allows multiple matings to the same male, (3) a female was caged with a novel male for this night (this male had mated once on the previous night with another female) (Novel) (n = 12), (4) a female was caged with a novel male for this night (this male had mated once on the previous night with another female), unmated females in this night were collected and individually caged for the remainder of their lifespan (2 males 1 mating) (n = 12), (5) a female was caged with a novel male for this night (this male had mated once on the previous night with another female), mated females in this night were collected and individually caged for the remainder of their lifespan (2 males 2 matings) (n = 12), and (6) a female was caged with a virgin male by one virgin male per night until death (Multiple) (n = 12). All treatments were conducted on a day-night reversed cycle under above mentioned condition. Each box provided 10% honey solution as food, and a paper strip (15×20 cm) folded in zig-zag fashion was used as an oviposition substratum. Boxes were changed daily for all replications. A 15 W red light was used for illumination during observation.

To test the effect of mate-novelty on behavior in both males and females, the female calling, male courtship and mating during the third night after eclosion (the subsequent night after first mating, one-time mated females were exposed to previous or novel mates on this night) in treatments of Paired (n = 12) and Novel (n = 12) were recorded. The following behaviors were recorded by quickly observing all treatment moths every 10 min: calling – the pheromone gland was expanded and extruded [36]; courtship – the male jumped and fanned his wings around or over the female or if the male exposed his genitalia trying to engage the female’s genitalia; mating – the two moths engaged by the genitalia.

To study the lifelong remating and multiple mating patterns of this insect, the mating events in treatments of Paired and Multiple were recorded daily (all matings occurred in the night and the mating duration (mean ± SD) is 44.1±6.1 min for this insect, and thus matings were recorded by quickly observing treated insects every 30 min [32]).

To test the effect of multiple mating on female fecundity and fertility, both daily and total numbers of eggs laid were counted for each female moth of all treatments except treatment Novel. Females of Novel may mate once or twice during the treatments and thus we designed two other treatments (2 males 1 mating and 2 males 2 matings) as mentioned above to test the effect of the mate-novelty and multiple mating on female oviposition pattern and fecundity independently. Eggs were collected daily (females mated the first time on the second night after eclosion and started to lay eggs on the subsequent night) and incubated in Petri dishes (8.5×1.5 cm) under above mentioned conditions. Eggs having black dots (larval heads) after 2 days of incubation were recorded as fertilized [32]. The number of hatched eggs was recorded 4 days after incubation. Female longevity was also recorded. Dead females were dissected to count the number of spermatophores in their bursa copulatrix under a dissecting microscope.

Statistics

Data on pre-calling duration (the duration between the start of the night and the start of calling), pre-mating duration (the duration between the start of the night and the start of mating), calling duration, hourly percentages of calling females and courting males on the subsequent night after first mating (one-time mated females were exposed to previous or novel mates on this night) were analyzed using ANOVA. Data on percentages of calling females or courting males were arcsin square root transformed. Data on hourly percentages of mated pairs on the subsequent night after first mating were not normally distributed after various transformations and thus were analyzed using a nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis test.

A Pearson chi-squared test was used to analyze the data from mating frequencies. Data on the overall number of eggs laid (fecundity), number of fertilized eggs laid (fertility), fertility rate (number of fertilized eggs laid/number of eggs laid), number of hatched eggs, hatch rate (number of hatched eggs/number of eggs laid) and female longevity were analyzed using a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) as these data were collected from a repeated-measures and they might correlated each other [37]. MANOVA allows effects on both overall dependent variables and each dependent variable to be tested [37]. Prior to MANOVA, data on fertility rate and hatch rate were arcsin square root transformed.

Data on daily eggs laid were analyzed using an ANOVA followed by Fisher’s LSD test. The rejection level was set at α<0.05. All analyses were performed using SAS 9.1 [38]. Unless stated otherwise, all values were reported as mean ± SE.

Results

Behavioral data on the subsequent night after first mating (one-time mated females were exposed to previous or novel mates on this night) revealed that females called at a higher rate when novel males were present (Novel) than that of females when previous mates were present (Paired) (ANOVA: F 1,18 = 4.89, P = 0.040) (Fig. 1a). Further analysis also showed Novel females have significantly shorter pre-calling duration (309±24 min for Paired and 160±29 min for Novel; ANOVA: F 1,22 = 7.08, P = 0.011) but longer calling duration (115±14 min for Paired and 217±18 min for Novel; ANOVA: F 1,22 = 18.97, P<0.0001) than Paired females. However, no significant difference was found on hourly percentages of courting males between Paired and Novel treatments (ANOVA: F 1,18 = 0.09, P = 0.767) (Fig. 1b).

Figure 1. Hourly percentages of calling females (a), courting males (b) and mated pairs (c) in Novel and Paired treatments in S. litura.

Figure 1

Approximately 30% of females mated the second time with the previous or novel males on the subsequent night after first mating (Fig. 1c; Fig. 2), and several females (<20%) mated a second and third time on following days in Paired and Multiple treatments (Figs. 2b & c). There was no significant difference on hourly percentages of mated pairs (Kruskal-Wallis test: H 1,18 = 0.57, P = 0.451) and pre-mating durations (ANOVA: F 1,5 = 0.05, P = 0.829) between treatment Paired and Novel (Fig. 1c). There was also no significant difference in mating numbers on the subsequent night after first mating between treatments of Novel, Paired and Multiple (Pearson chi-squared test: DF = 2, χ2 = 0.363, P = 0.834) (Fig. 2). The number of spermatophores in females was equal to the number of matings observed.

Figure 2. Remating patterns of females in treatment Novel (a), Paired (b) and Multiple (c) treatments in S. litura.

Figure 2

No significant differences were found on fecundity, fertility, fertility rate, hatched eggs, hatch rate and longevity between treatments of Paired, Multiple, 1 male 1 mating, 2 males 1 mating and 2 males 2 matings (MANOVA: F 24,311 = 0.793, P = 0.743) (Table 1). However, significant differences were found between treatments in daily fecundity (Fig. 3). On the first oviposition night (the subsequent night after first mating), females that were caged with novel males (Multiple, 2 males 1 mating and 2 males 2 matings treatments) laid significantly fewer eggs than females caged with their previous mates (treatment Paired) and females were caged individually without males (treatment 1 male 1 mating) (ANOVA: F 4,56 = 4.496, P = 0.003). However, on the second oviposition night, Multiple and 2 males 1 mating females laid significantly more eggs than females of Paired and 1 male 1 mating treatments (ANOVA: F 4,56 = 2.872, P = 0.031).

Table 1. Effect of multiple mating on female reproductive output and longevity in S. litura *.

Parameter Mean ± SE F DF P
Fecundity (no. of eggs laid) 1176±68 0.504 4,56 0.733
Fertility (no. of fertilized eggs laid) 1098±65 0.388 4,56 0.816
Hatched eggs 971±58 0.576 4,56 0.681
Fertility rate (%) 93.9±0.73 0.820 4,56 0.518
Hatch rate (%) 88.9±1.49 1.658 4,56 0.173
Female longevity (days) 6.57±0.18 0.586 4,56 0.674

*There were no significant differences between treatments for any of these parameters.

Figure 3. Daily oviposition patterns in relation to mating treatments in S. litura.

Figure 3

Within the same oviposition night, bars with different letters are significantly different (P<0.05).

Discussion

Mating event data and spermatophore number in females indicate that both sexes of S. litura mate multiply regardless of mating with a novel or previous mate (Figs. 1 & 2). Multiple mating may benefit females through direct benefits such as sperm and/or nutrient replenishment or through reduced costs of harassment (convenience polyandry) [1], [6], [7]. In a number of species of Lepidoptera, females receiving more sperm or male investment show increased fecundity, fertility and longevity [39][43]. However, results of the present study do not support the sperm and nutrient replenishment hypotheses because neither the number of mates nor the number of copulations achieved by females significantly increased female fecundity, fertility and longevity in S. litura. Moreover, results of this study also do not support the convenience hypothesis [44]; even when females were exposed to a virgin male each night for 6 nights, they only mated approximately two times on average (Fig. 1) and did not show reduced longevity compared to females from other treatments (Table 1).

Why do females mate again when they already have sufficient sperm to fertilize all their eggs? Several genetic benefit hypotheses have been suggested to answer this question, such as the good gene [8], [9], genetic incompatibility [10][13] and genetic diversity hypotheses [4], [14][18]. These hypotheses require sperm from different males to fertilize eggs competitively [45] and/or the offspring fathered by multiple males [46]. To maximize genetic benefits from multiple mating, females from Lepidoptera [4] and other taxa [19][24] have evolved strategies to prefer novel versus previous mates in their subsequent matings.

In this study, we observed that females showed different oviposition patterns when caged with a novel male on the subsequent night after first mating in comparison with females caged with previous mates (Fig. 3). On the first oviposition night (the subsequent night after first mating, one-time mated females were exposed to previous or novel mates on this night), females that were caged with novel males laid significantly fewer eggs than females caged with previous mates and solitary females, suggesting delayed oviposition by females when novel mates are present. By contrast, on the second oviposition night, females that were caged with novel males laid significantly more eggs than females caged with previous mates and solitary females. A similar oviposition pattern has also been found in the hide beetle D. maculatus [21] and the Mediterranean flour moth E. kuehniella [4]. Females of D. maculatus do not lay eggs until they have mated several times with different males for genetic benefits [21], which may because they have a long adult lifespan (>40 days). In S. litura, female lifespan is short (approximately 10 days) and their best mating period is during the first few days after eclosion due to the decline of sex pheromone production [47]. This may explain why S. litura females chose to lay eggs quickly when no males were around or in the presence of their previous mates.

In the present study, we recorded male courtship behavior and female calling behavior in females encountering novel males and females encountering their previous mates on the subsequent night after first mating (Fig. 1). The results demonstrated females that have novel males present called early and more often than females that have previous mates present, whereas no significant differences were found in male courtship between treatments. In addition, our previous study [32] has demonstrated that males showed similar courtship patterns on the second and third night after eclosion when paired with the same female. These results suggested that females of S. litura can distinguish between novel and previous mates and prefer novel versus previous mates, whereas males generally like to remate with both the same and different females. However, such female preference for new mates did not result in significantly higher remating rates in females caged with new mates than females caged with previous mates (Figs. 1 & 2). This is different than the results found in other species where females mated more often with novel males than with previous mates (e.g. [4], [19]). In S. litura, this may be because: 1) although a normal mating is enough for females to fertilize their whole eggs (Table 1), it is better to remate several times to ensure enough sperm reserves because they may have mated with a male with a low number of sperm in the first mating (recent mated or immature male) (e.g. [48]), 2) females have a very short reproductive period [47], and 3) males generally like to remate with both the same and different females (Fig. 1b). As a consequence, females will mate again with the same male when no novel males are available.

A previous study of females mated with two males in S. litura has showed a last male sperm precedence pattern [49]. S. litura females usually mate the first time on the second night after eclosion and approximately 60% of females can mate a second time in the subsequent two days [32], [49]. Sperm need several hours (>3 h) to reach the spermatheca and become mature; thus, females usually do not lay eggs on the same night after mating [32], [49]. In the present study, in twice mated females with two males, therefore, eggs laid on the first oviposition night should be mainly fathered by the first male and the eggs laid after the first oviposition night should be mostly fathered by the second male (Fig. 2). Therefore, multiple mating in females has enhanced the progeny genetic diversity in S. litura. Increased genetic diversity within a brood can increase offspring fitness by reducing competition due to different genotypes better partitioning limited resources [14] and by reducing disease transfer as different genotypes may have different susceptibilities or resistance to parasites or pathogens (e.g. [16]). Although not yet tested experimentally, it is possible that enhanced genetic diversity within a brood may be a way of inbreeding avoidance if offspring live together and sibling matings occur often [18]. In S. litura, eggs are laid in clusters (a cluster contains 50–300 eggs) that are several layers thick and are covered with hair from the female’s abdomen, which can prevent predation of the eggs by natural enemies [50]. However, offspring competition, inbreeding and disease transfer risks are thus increased due to offspring living together in a high population density. Therefore, genetic diversity in progeny is valuable for S. litura and thus genetic benefits should be the main force behind the evolution of female behavioral strategies found in the present study.

The present study also found that males generally remate regardless of whether the female is a previous mate or not, which is different to previous studies where males in most species prefer novel mates [26] and in some cases prefer previous mates [28], [29]. Males’ preferring novel mates for mating will significantly improve males’ reproductive success [28], [29]. One possible explanation for males’ preferring previous mates for mating may be that males can increase their reproductive success by minimizing the opportunity for postcopulatory sexual selection [19]. Similarly, the no preference for new or previous mates in S. litura males may also the consequence of conflict between the sexes, in which females actively to mate with different mates to maximize the benefits from multiple mating and males seeking to minimize sperm competition [19], [49]. Further studies are needed to explore the evolutionary significance of such male behavior.

Data Availability

The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are within the paper.

Funding Statement

Funding was provided by 31160434, http://www.nsfc.gov.cn/, National Natural Science Foundation Program of China. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Arnqvist G, Nilsson T (2000) The evolution of polyandry: multiple mating and female fitness in insects. Anim Behav 60: 145–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Daly M (1978) Cost of mating. Am Nat 112: 771–774. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Bateman A (1948) Intrasexual selection in Drosophila . Heredity 2: 349–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Xu J, Wang Q (2009) A polyandrous female moth discriminates against previous mates to gain genetic diversity. Anim Behav 78: 1309–1315. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Wang Q, Davis LK (2006) Females remate for sperm replenishment in a seed bug: evidence from offspring viability. J Insect Behav 19: 337–346. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Simmons LW (2001) Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Princeton: Princeton University Press. 434 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Thonhauser KE, Raveh S, Hettyey A, Beissmann H, Penn DJ (2013) Why do female mice mate with multiple males? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67: 1961–1970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kokko H, Brooks R, McNamara JM, Houston AI (2002) The sexual selection continuum. Proc Biol Sci 269: 1331–1340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Suzaki Y, Katsuki M, Miyatake T, Okada Y (2013) Male courtship behavior and weapon trait as indicators of indirect benefit in the bean bug, Riptortus pedestris . Plos One 8: e83278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Thornhill R, Alcock J (1983) The evolution of insect mating systems: Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. 547 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Zeh JA, Zeh DW (1996) The evolution of polyandry I: Intragenomic conflict and genetic incompatibility. Proc Biol Sci 263: 1711–1717. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Madsen T, Shine R, Loman J, Hakansson T (1992) Why do female adders copulate so frequently. Nature 355: 440–441. [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Caspers BA, Krause ET, Hendrix R, Kopp M, Rupp O, et al. (2014) The more the better - polyandry and genetic similarity are positively linked to reproductive success in a natural population of terrestrial salamanders (Salamandra salamandra). Mol Ecol 23: 239–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Yasui Y (1998) The ‘genetic benefits’ of female multiple mating reconsidered. Trends Ecol Evol 13: 246–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Yasui Y (2001) Female multiple mating as a genetic bet-hedging strategy when mate choice criteria are unreliable. Ecol Res 16: 605–616. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Tooby J (1982) Pathogens, polymorphism, and the evolution of sex. J Theor Biol 97: 557–576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Robinson GE (1992) Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Annu Rev Entomol 37: 637–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Cornell SJ, Tregenza T (2007) A new theory for the evolution of polyandry as a means of inbreeding avoidance. Proc Biol Sci 274: 2873–2879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Zeh JA, Newcomer SD, Zeh DW (1998) Polyandrous females discriminate against previous mates. P Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 13732–13736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Bateman PW (1998) Mate preference for novel partners in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus . Ecol Entomol 23: 473–475. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Archer MS, Elgar MA (1999) Female preference for multiple partners: sperm competition in the hide beetle, Dermestes maculatus (DeGeer). Anim Behav 58: 669–675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Ivy TM, Weddle CB, Sakaluk SK (2005) Females use self-referent cues to avoid mating with previous mates. Proc Biol Sci 272: 2475–2478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Eakley AL, Houde AE (2004) Possible role of female discrimination against ‘redundant’ males in the evolution of colour pattern polymorphism in guppies. Proc Biol Sci 271: S299–S301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. LaDage LD, Ferkin MH (2007) Do female leopard geckos (Eublepharis macularius) discriminate between previous mates and novel males? Behaviour 144: 515–527. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Fromhage L, Schneider JM (2005) No discrimination against previous mates in a sexually cannibalistic spider. Naturwissenschaften 92: 423–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Tan CKW, Lovlie H, Greenway E, Goodwin SF, Pizzari T, et al. (2013) Sex-specific responses to sexual familiarity, and the role of olfaction in Drosophila . Proc Biol Sci 280: 20131691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Newcomer SD, Zeh JA, Zeh DW (1999) Genetic benefits enhance the reproductive success of polyandrous females. P Natl Acad Sci USA 96: 10236–10241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Bateman AJ (1948) Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila . Heredity 2: 349–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Jones AG, Ratterman NL (2009) Mate choice and sexual selection: What have we learned since Darwin? P Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 10001–10008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Xu J (2010) Reproductive behaviour of Ephestia kuehniella Zeller (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae). Palmerston North, New Zealand: Massey University. 208 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Armes NJ, Wightman JA, Jadhav DR, Rao GVR (1997) Status of insecticide resistance in Spodoptera litura in Andhra Pradesh, India. Pestic Sci 50: 240–248. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Li C, Yu J-F, Xu J, Liu J-H, Ye aH (2012) Reproductive rhythms of the tobacco cutworm, Spodoptera litura (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). GSTF J BioSci 2: 25–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Yu JF, Li C, Xu J, Liu JH, Ye H (2014) Male accessory gland secretions modulate female post-mating behavior in the moth Spodoptera litura . J Insect Behav 27: 105–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Li G, Chen Q, Pang Y (1998) Studies of artificial diets for the beet armyworm, Spodoptera exigua . Acta Sci Nat Univ Sunyatseni 4: 1–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Li W, Zou WJ, Wang LH (2006) The bionomics and control of Prodenia litura in Kunming. Southwest China J Agric Sci 19: 85–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Raina AK, Werginb WP, Murphyb CA, Erbe EF (2000) Structural organization of the sex pheromone gland in Helicoverpa zea in relation to pheromone production and release. Arthropod Struct Dev 29 343–353. [PubMed]
  • 37.Scheiner SM (2001) MANOVA: multiple response variables and multispecies interactions. In: Scheiner SM, Gurevitch J, editors. Design and analysis of ecological experiments. 2 ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 99–115.
  • 38.SAS I (2006) User’s manual. Cary: SAS Institute Inc. 5136 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Jimenez-Perez A, Wang Q, Markwick N (2003) Remating behavior of Cnephasia jactatana Walker females (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae). J Insect Behav 16: 797–809. [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Wang Q, Millar JG (1997) Reproductive behavior of Thyanta pallidovirens (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae). Ann Entomol Soc Am 90: 380–388. [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Watanabe M (1988) Multiple matings increase the fecundity of the yellow swallowtail butterfly, Papilio xuthus L., in summer generations. J Insect Behav 1: 17–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Wedell N (1996) Mate quality affects reproductive effort in a paternally investing species. Am Nat 148: 1075–1088. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Wilson N, Tufton TJ, Eady PE (1999) The effect of single, double, and triple matings on the lifetime fecundity of Callosobruchus analis and Callosobruchus maculatus (Coleoptera: Bruchidae). J Insect Behav 12: 295–306. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Parker GA (1984) Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating strategies. In: Smith RL, editor. Sperm competition and the evolution of animal mating systems. London: Academic Press. 2–60.
  • 45. Simmons LW (2005) The evolution of polyandry: sperm competition, sperm selection, and offspring viability. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 36: 125–146. [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Jennions MD, Petrie M (2000) Why do females mate multiply? A review of the genetic benefits. Biol Rev 75: 21–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Sun F, Hu Y-Y, Du J-W (2002) The sex pheromone communication system of Spodoptera litura (Fabricius). Acta Entom Sin 45: 404–407. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Xu J, Wang Q (2011) Seminal fluid reduces female longevity and stimulates egg production and sperm trigger oviposition in a moth. J Insect Physiol 57: 385–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Seth RK, Kaur JJ, Reynolds SE (2002) Sperm transfer during mating, movement of sperm in the female reproductive tract, and sperm precedence in the common cutworm Spodoptera litura . Physiol Entomol 27: 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Fukuda T, Wakamura S, Arakaki N, Yamagishi K (2007) Parasitism, development and adult longevity of the egg parasitoid Telenomus nawai (Hymenoptera: Scelionidae) on the eggs of Spodoptera litura (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). B Entomol Res 97: 185–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are within the paper.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES