Summary
Many patients with circumscribed brain injuries, such as those caused by stroke or focal trauma, return to driving after a period of acute recovery. These persons often have chronic residual cognitive deficits that may impact on driving safety, but little is known about their driving behavior in the real world. Extant studies tend to rely on driving simulators or controlled on-road drives. These methods of observation are not able to capture the complexities of the typical driving environment, and may not accurately represent a driver’s usual behavior on the road. The current study used a video event-activated data recorder (VEADR) system to observe drivers with focal brain lesions in their normal daily driving environment over a three-month period. In the context of primarily safe driving behavior, we were able to document a number of relatively infrequent and hitherto unobserved high risk behaviors and traffic violations. These findings demonstrate the feasibility and value of sampling real-world driving in neurologic patient populations such as those with focal brain lesions, and highlight the critical importance of evaluating unsafe driving behaviors which may occur with insufficient frequency to be captured by relatively brief simulator or controlled on-road evaluations.
INTRODUCTION
Driving is a complex task that places demands on multiple neurocognitive systems. In addition to the attentional, perceptual and motor demands, drivers must monitor and evaluate multiple external factors, such as tracking the location of surrounding vehicles, judging when it is safe to pass a slower vehicle, and obeying traffic signals and signs (Trick, Enns, Mills, & Vavrik, 2004). Driving clearly places major demands on executive functions such as attentional control, decision making, planning, and execution of actions. Acquired impairment in these functions due to brain injury or disease can have a detrimental impact on driving (Anstey, Wood, Lord, & Walker, 2005; Lundqvist et al., 1997; Marcotte et al., 2004; Rizzo et al., 2004). Drivers with executive dysfunction may also have difficulty formulating and executing necessary actions based on current driving conditions (e.g., traffic density, surrounding vehicle positions, traffic signals/signs; Uc & Rizzo, 2008).
Many studies have examined the effects of executive dysfunction on driving, such as in persons with Alzheimer’s disease (Grace et al., 2005; Uc & Rizzo, 2008), Parkinson’s disease (Grace et al., 2005; Uc & Rizzo, 2008; Uc et al., 2007), or Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome (Verstraeten, 2007). These studies found that deficiencies in executive functioning predicted impaired driving. While these studies support the link between executive dysfunction and poor driving performance, they are also methodologically limited. For example, these studies have assessed driving performance during brief on-road tests in instrumented vehicles (IV) or in driving simulators. Simulator-based driving assessments are limited in reproducing the complex and varied conditions of real roads and alter the decision-making environment of participants, encouraging them to make riskier choices than in their own cars on the road (Horrey & Wickens, 2006; Rizzo et al., 2004). In contrast, IV-based driving assessments under the supervision of a trained researcher may induce the opposite behavior. Drivers may be inclined to drive more cautiously than they normally would, conscious that they are being observed and are not driving their own vehicle. In addition, assessments are often conducted in optimal weather and traffic conditions to minimize risks to participants (Dawson, Anderson, Uc, Dastrup, & Rizzo, 2009). These methodological limitations constrain the usefulness and accuracy of driver fitness measures obtained from simulator- and IV-based assessments, and findings based on those methods are likely to lead to biased conclusions on correlates and predictors of driver fitness.
With technological advances, however, it is now possible to conduct naturalistic assessments of driver fitness while people drive their own cars during the course of everyday activities. To date, only a few large-scale naturalistic driving studies have been conducted, including the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study conducted by the Virginia Tech Transportation Institute. These studies have captured video images and on-board data including various kinematic measures, headway detection, side obstacle detection, and lane-keeping. This approach to monitoring daily driving habits has allowed for a more realistic view of driver behavior over the course of time in real-world situations (Hanowski, et al., 2000; Neale, Dingus, Klauer, Sudweeks, & Goodman, 2006; Stutts et al. 2003). Naturalistic assessments of driver performance is an essential direction for current and future research that will overcome many of the methodological weaknesses associated with simulator and IV-based assessments of driver fitness.
The few studies that have relied on naturalistic assessments of driving were conducted with neurologically normal participants. The current pilot study investigated the feasibility of using a VEADR system to collect information on naturalistic driving behavior in persons with focal brain lesions and residual cognitive impairments. We aim to gather observations of naturalistic driving behaviors in order to identify factors that contribute to driver safety and characterize mobility patterns among persons with neurologic injury or disease.
METHOD
Participants
Participants were six currently licensed drivers aged 49 to 83 years with focal brain lesions, recruited from a study currently being conducted by our research team on driving in advancing age or from the registry of persons with focal brain lesions maintained in the project “Anatomical Substrates of Complex Behavior” at the University of Iowa Department of Neurology. Potential participants were excluded if they had any confounding medical conditions such as dementia, major psychiatric disease, vestibular disorders, or substance abuse. Use of prescription medications was allowed with the exception of stimulants, antihistamines, narcotics, anxiolytics, anticonvulsants, and neuroleptics. Participants were not excluded because of visual defects unless they had corrected visual acuity worse than 20/50. Informed consent was obtained in accordance with the guidelines of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Iowa.
Design and Procedure
The VEADR instrumentation package, (DriveCam, San Diego; plus GPS) was installed in the participants’ primary vehicle for a period of 90 days in four drivers. One participant drove for only 30 days with the data collection equipment installed, and one participant drove for two and a half months. The VEADR package was installed so as to minimally interfere with the participants’ driving ability, and participants were instructed to drive as they normally would.
The DriveCam is approximately the size of a deck of playing cards, mounted behind the rear-view mirror. The system is triggered during ignition starts, abrupt steering and braking maneuvers made by the driver (greater than 0.3 g-force) and at set intervals. When triggered, the system captures 12 seconds of audio and video from the forward view and of the driver and vehicle interior. The GPS tracker is a small device plugged into the vehicle's on-board diagnostic (OBD) system to record speed and position data at set intervals.
Coding
Each triggered event video was reviewed based on a coding scheme developed by Carney, McGehee, Lee, Reyes, and Raby (2010). In addition to categorizing basic elements such as weather and road type/condition, videos were coded for two broad categories of events: driver behaviors (Table 1) and vehicle control (Table 2). Each category contained several specific outcomes, with the possibility of coding multiple outcomes in a single video.
Table 1.
Examples of Driver Behavior
Driver Behavior | Description | Count |
---|---|---|
Reckless driving | Risky, intentional behavior by the driver; don’t appear to be concerned about the consequences of their driving; assertive, forcing others to respond to their actions; taking hands off wheel | 7 |
Driving while fatigued | Yawning; blank /fixed staring; rubbing eyes; slack facial muscles | 49 |
Distracted driving | ||
Music distraction | Too loud | 399 |
Cognitive distraction | Looked but did not see; talking/singing | 123 |
Object / Animal distraction | Pet in vehicle; reaching for object | 42 |
Cell phone distraction | Talking/listening on phone; dialing phone; reaching for/answering phone; texting | 42 |
In-vehicle distraction | Adjusting radio or other devices | 34 |
Eating / smoking | Eating or drinking while driving; lighting/smoking cigarette | 6 |
Personal hygiene | Brushing hair; brushing teeth; removing/adjusting jewelry; applying makeup | 19 |
Inattention to the road | Looking in mirrors (side/rearview), out window; or down inside the vehicle | 43 |
Table 2.
Description of the various vehicle control outcomes observed during event triggers
Vehicle Control | Description | Count |
---|---|---|
Running a red light | Entering the intersection after light has turned red; failing to stop before right turn on red | 17 |
Running a stop sign | No reduction in speed before sign; failing to come to a complete stop (i.e., rolling through intersection) | 93 |
Excessive speed on straight-aways | Exceeding the speed limit | 1 |
Improper turns | Accelerating through turn; cutting the corner; turning too fast; wide turns; turning from wrong lane | 1545 |
Crossing lane boundaries | Turning down a one-way street; drifting across center line; drifting onto shoulder | 18 |
Avoiding maneuvers | Avoiding another vehicle, pedestrian, or object in the road | 156 |
RESULTS
This system allowed us to characterize each patient’s pattern of driving mobility, including time spent in vehicle, miles traveled, geographic distance covered, patterns of repetitive route following, and driving environment (urban v. rural, road type and speed zones). Fewer than 10% of triggered and randomly timed observations represented unsafe driving or traffic violations. However, in the context of primarily safe and normal driving behavior, the VEADR system captured numerous instances of unsafe driving. Key driving behavior and vehicle control events observed over approximately 450 driving days are presented in Tables 1 and 2. The monitoring system was able to capture several types of in-vehicle safety-relevant behaviors, including cell phone use, grooming behavior, and distraction by pets, food, or loud music. In addition, safety-critical vehicle maneuvers were captured, including running red lights and stop signs, going the wrong way on one-way streets, and crossing the center line. Several brief driving profiles are presented below to illustrate the manner in which this observation and coding system can characterize individual patients’ real-world driving behaviors.
Participant 200, a 70 year old male, suffered a right insula/basal ganglia stroke in 1995. On two instances, the subject was seen responding positively to a situation in which another vehicle entered his path. In both cases, the subject had to respond quickly to avoid the other car. The subject was also recorded showing inattentiveness during driving on two occasions. During one event, he attempted to grab an object out of view of the camera, though he appeared to be in complete control of the car. However, during another instance of inattentiveness the subject was waving and looking out the side window during a right turn. He swung wide while turning and had to quickly correct in order to avoid running into an approaching car head-on.
Participant 201 is a 73 year old male who suffered at left parietal stroke in 2000. Over the course of three months, the subject failed to come to a complete stop at a stop sign on 8 occasions. These events were not overt inattention to the stop sign, but were instances where the driver approached the intersection and rolled through the stop sign at less than 5 mph. In general, these events were captured by the system’s low trigger setting (0.3 g), picking up abrupt braking, acceleration, or hard steering maneuvers, and did not represent reckless driving.
Participant 202, a 75 year old female who suffered a bilateral frontal stroke in 2001, routinely failed to come to a complete stop at a stop sign near her home, accounting for the majority of this type of event across all subjects. On 9 occasions, the participant either rolled though a stop sign, similar to the previous subject, or failed to stop completely when making a right turn on a red stoplight. Like subject 201, the instances only occurred when no other car was preceding though the intersection.
Participant 203 (83 year old female; left basal ganglia stroke in 2002) also did not come to a complete stop at stop signs. On 14 occasions in the three-month period, the subject rolled through stop signs at a relatively slow speed (< 5mph). She was also witnessed running two red lights. On both occasions, she entered the intersection just after the light turned red. She did not appear to be aggressive and did not engage in any distracting behaviors.
Participant 204 (57 year old female; left frontal stroke in 2000) had six instances of driving the wrong way down the road. All such instances occurred along the same roadway, which transitioned from an undivided to divided street. The driver repeatedly chose to drive a short distance down the wrong side of the boulevard in order to turn into a parking lot, rather than driving until she reached a point where she could cross the division. Like the above participants, she often failed to come to a complete stop at stop signs, rolling through at 4–5 mph. She also ran four red lights over 90 days of driving, three on left turns (just as the light turned red) and once on a right turn (similar to participant 200/202). In addition, she was captured having cell phone conversations 33 times and three times looking away from the road to dial her phone three times.
Finally, participant 206 (49 year old male; right frontal trauma in 2008) commonly drove late at night and/or with loud music on. GPS data also indicated a repeated pattern of late-night stops in the vicinity of taverns. In general, this driver had a more aggressive style compared to the other participants, and we consequently recorded nearly three times as many triggered events from his vehicle. Over the three month period, the VEADR captured four instances where he ran a red light. He also commonly rolled through stop signs and tended to take turns at high speeds. In addition, he engaged in several interesting driver behaviors, such as talking on a cell phone, brushing his teeth, and eating while driving.
DISCUSSION
These findings indicate that naturalistic studies of the driving behaviors and vehicle control patterns of patients with chronic neurologic disease can provide an important source of information regarding safety risk and large-scale mobility of these individuals. Such studies are an important addition to driving simulation and controlled-route drives in instrumented vehicles for these patients. The behaviors we observed in this naturalistic driving assessment captured the diversity and complexity of actual driving situations, as well as many safety-critical but relatively infrequent events that are often not seen in simulator and/or IV-based assessments.
Typically, data collection in the lab lasts for an hour or less, greatly reducing the likelihood that a crash or major traffic violation will occur during this time. Instead, researchers rely on self-report questionnaires to obtain information on driving behaviors of at-risk drivers. However, self-report data is often inaccurate, especially considering that subjects may have memory or cognitive deficits. Crash data, vehicle usage information and driver strategy are more likely to be captured in a naturalistic setting rather than during a controlled laboratory drive (Rizzo, Robinson, & Neale, 2007). Furthermore, the future of naturalistic observation of drivers could allow for a more individualized approach to evaluating and modifying driving behaviors following brain injury or disease. Targeting a driver’s strengths and weaknesses over time in a naturalistic setting can allow researchers to curb potentially dangerous behaviors that might not emerge in a shorter controlled drive within the lab.
While we did not observe any crashes or near-crashes in the current study, the instrumentation package employed in this study proved to be practical and reliable, and it elicited few complaints from participants. A major concern in sampling driving over an extended time frame as in the current study is how best to set the triggers to capture important events while not generating impractical amounts of data reflecting only normal behavior. The majority of the events we observed at lower levels (e.g., 0.3–0.35) reflected hard braking or turning, but nothing that would suggest reckless or unsafe driving. However, some of the illegal maneuvers captured by the VEADR, such as running a red light, occurred at lower g-force triggers. Supplementing g-force triggers with random sampling helps address this issue, but it remains likely that we missed many low g-force safety-relevant events. GPS-guided geocentric triggers may provide a more efficacious supplement to measuring safety relevant events in future studies.
AKNOWLEDGEMENT
This study was supported by awards AG 17177 and AG 15071 from the National Institute on Aging (NIA).
REFERENCES
- Anstey KJ, Wood J, Lord S, Walker JG. Cognitive, sensory and physical factors enabling driving safety in older adults. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:45–65. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carney C, McGehee DV, Lee JD, Reyes ML, Raby M. Using an event-triggered video intervention system to expand the supervised learning of newly licensed adolescent drivers. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100(6):1101. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.165829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson JD, Anderson SW, Uc EY, Dastrup E, Rizzo M. Predictors of driving safety in early Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 72(6):521–527. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000341931.35870.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grace J, Amick MM, D’Abreu A, Festa EK, Heindel WC, Ott BR. Neuropsychological deficits associated with driving performance in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11:766–775. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanowski RJ, Wierwille WW, Garness SA, Dingus TA. Impact of local/short haul operations on driver fatigue: Final project report (DOT-MC-00-203) Washington, DC: Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Horrey WJ, Wickens CD. Examining the impact of cell phone conversations on driving using met-analytic techniques. Human Factors. 2006;48(1):196–205. doi: 10.1518/001872006776412135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lundqvist A, Alinder J, Gerdle B, Levander S, Roennberg J. Neuropsychological aspects of driving after brain lesion: Simulator study and on-road driving. Applied Neuropsychology. 1997;4(4):220–230. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an0404_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcotte TD, Wolfson T, Rosenthal TJ, Heaton RK, Gonzalez R, Ellis RJ, et al. A multimodal assessment of driving performance in HIV infection. Neurology. 2004;63:1417–1422. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000141920.33580.5d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neale VL, Dingus TA, Klauer SG, Sudweeks J, Goodman M. An overview of the 100-car naturalistic study and findings. 2006 [Google Scholar]
- Rizzo M, Robinson S, Neale V. The brain in the wild: Tracking human behavior in natural and naturalistic settings. In: Parasuraman R, Rizzo M, editors. Neuroergonomics: The brain at work. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 113–128. [Google Scholar]
- Rizzo M, Stierman L, Skaar N, Dawson JD, Anderson SW, Vecera SP. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, No. 1865. 2004. Effects of a controlled auditory-verbal distraction task on older driver vehicle control; pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
- Stutts JC, Gish K, Feaganes J, Mercadante M, Rodgman EA, Staplin L, et al. Distractions in everyday driving. Washington, D.C.: AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety; 2003. http://www.aaafoundation.org/pdf/DistractionsInEverydayDriving.pdf. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trick LM, Enns JT, Mills J, Vavrik J. Paying attention behind the wheel: A framework for studying the role of attention in driving. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science. 2004;5(5):385–424. [Google Scholar]
- Uc EY, Rizzo M. Driving and neurodegenerative diseases. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports. 2008;8:377–383. doi: 10.1007/s11910-008-0059-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uc EY, Rizzo M, Anderson SW, Sparks JD, Rodnitzky RL, Dawson JD. Impaired navigation in drivers with Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2007;130:2433–2440. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verstraeten E. Neurocognitive effects of obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports. 2007;7:161–166. doi: 10.1007/s11910-007-0012-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]