Abstract
A cross-modal association between somatosensory tactile sensation and parietal and occipital activities during Braille reading was initially discovered in tests with blind subjects, with sighted and blindfolded healthy subjects used as controls. However, the neural background of oral stereognosis remains unclear. In the present study, we investigated whether the parietal and occipital cortices are activated during shape discrimination by the mouth using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Following presentation of the test piece shape, a sham discrimination trial without the test pieces induced posterior parietal lobe (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, BA19), and striate cortex (BA17) activation as compared with the rest session, while shape discrimination of the test pieces markedly activated those areas as compared with the rest session. Furthermore, shape discrimination of the test pieces specifically activated the posterior parietal cortex (precuneus/BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17), as compared with sham sessions without a test piece. We concluded that oral tactile sensation is recognized through tactile/visual cross-modal substrates in the parietal and occipital cortices during shape discrimination by the mouth.
Introduction
A cross-modal association between somatosensory tactile sensation and parietal and occipital cortical activities during Braille reading in blind subjects was initially reported in 1996 by Sadato et al. [1], while multisensory processing of tactile and visual convergence were also later reported in sighted [2]–[7] and blindfolded [2], [8]–[10] healthy control subjects. Regarding cross-modal object recognition, bottom-up and top-down corticocortical mechanisms have been speculated [11]–[12]. Although an individual is not able to visualize the inside of their own oral cavity, it is possible to properly chew a food bolus. However, if that multimodal recognition in the mouth is disrupted, ordinary chewing may not be possible without sustaining an oral injury. In the field of dentistry, oral shape discrimination has been applied to examine oral stereognosis and chewing efficacy in aged edentulous patients [13]–[18], as well as the relationship between oral stereognosis and chewing activity [13]–[15], [19], and the effects of tooth loss and denture wearing [16], [20]–[23].
There are methodological differences among the examinations utilized to examine oral stereognosis with an oral shape discrimination task performance, mostly in terms of presenting [13]–[16] or not presenting [19], [21] the test pieces to the subject prior to the trial, and with [24] and without [13]–[16], [19], [21] subject rehearsal of the procedures preceding the sessions. As a result, no unified procedure for examination of oral stereognosis has been established, and there are no standard evaluations of the differences between presenting and not presenting the test pieces prior to the task.
The neural background involved in oral stereognosis also remains unclear. Several studies have investigated oral shape discrimination and its effects on cortical activities, including Fujii et al. [25], who initially reported prefrontal cortex activation, whereas there are no known reports of parietal and occipital cortical activities during oral shape discrimination. Yamamura et al. [26] reported cortical activation during shape discrimination by the mouth in the prefrontal and visual cortices. Nevertheless, currently available findings are contradictory and inconsistent, especially in regard to activation of the occipital cortex during shape discrimination by the mouth.
In the present study, we investigated whether the occipital cortex is activated during shape discrimination by the mouth using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). This technology is applicable in clinical situations and previous studies have detected visual cortex activation using fNIRS [27]–[29].
In this study, we presented the test pieces to the subjects and also allowed them to rehearse after receiving an explanation prior to all sessions of shape discrimination by the mouth. Our experiments consisted of 3 different sessions; resting prior to the trial (REST), discrimination without a test piece in the mouth (SHAM), and shape discrimination with individual test pieces in the mouth (SHAPE). The results of those sessions were used to determine baseline, control, and experimental values, respectively.
The purpose of this study was threefold. First, we attempted to identify the neural background related to reference imagery by explaining the test pieces and allowing rehearsal before the trials in the SHAM session. We also determined the effects of the association of target imagery with the test pieces in the mouth during the SHAPE session with reference imagery based on explanations of the test pieces and rehearsal preceding the trials during the SHAM session. Finally, the effects of target imagery with test pieces in the mouth on occipital cortex activation were compared between the SHAPE and SHAM sessions. From our findings, related neural substrates in the occipital cortex and their functional significance for oral shape discrimination are discussed.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Eleven healthy, male, right-handed subjects aged 28.4±5.8 years (mean ± SD) were enrolled. All provided written informed consent for participation in the study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee of Nihon University School of Dentistry at Matsudo. All subjects were mentally healthy, as indicated by a score of ≤7 on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale [30].
Experimental procedures
The experiment was performed in a quiet room with the subject comfortably seated. A screen was positioned around the subject to block extra visual information entering their field of view. The session required the subject to discriminate the shape of test piece made of autopolymer resin (UNIFAST3, GC, JAPAN) that was placed in the mouth (oral shape discrimination; SHAPE). There were 6 test pieces with different shapes; circle, ellipse, square, rectangle, triangle, and semicircle [16] (Fig. 1A). Prior to the session, each subject was shown the 6 test pieces and a schematic diagram, received an explanation of the procedure, and was allowed to rehearse the task [16].
During the pretrial period, the investigator approached the subject from the rear so as not to enter their field of view, then instructed them to open their mouth and placed a test piece on the tongue using tweezers. The subject was instructed to be quiet until given a verbal cue (pretrial and pre-task periods), after which they were instructed to explore the test piece in their mouth without biting (task period, 10 seconds). After 10 seconds, they were given a verbal cue to stop the oral exploration and instructed to be quiet again (post-task period). After 20 seconds, the test piece was removed from the mouth, then the subject was shown a schematic diagram of all 6 test pieces and asked to point to the one they thought had been used. The trials was performed 6 times in 1 session, using a different shaped piece in each trial. In addition, the SHAM and REST sessions were conducted in a similar manner, but with no test piece placed in the mouth. During the SHAM session, the subject was instructed to move their tongue in the oral cavity, then point toward the schematic diagram after the experimenter had imitated removing the test piece. During the REST session, there was no task performance and the subject was instructed to remain quiet. The SHAPE and SHAM sessions were performed in random order.
NIRS measurements
Occipital lobe activity was assessed during the pre-task, task, and post-task periods using a 24-channel NIRS device (ETG-100, Hitachi Medical Co., Chiba, Japan) that utilizes 2 wavelengths of near-infrared light (780 and 830 nm) [31]. The distance between the pairs of detector probes was 3.0 cm and the device was set to measure from points associated with the surface of the cerebral cortices [32]–[33]. The probes were fitted with 4×4 thermoplastic shells and placed at the center of the bottom line of shell, which was positioned on the inion (Iz) according to the international 10–20 system [34] (Fig. 1B). The distance between the tip of the probe and bottom of the shell was 1.0 cm. Functional NIRS was used to determine relative changes in the concentration of oxygenated-hemoglobin ([oxy-Hb]), deoxygenated-hemoglobin ([deoxy-Hb]), and total hemoglobin. Fundamental Hb data were displayed and used as an index of Hb change as a scaled variable. Change in [oxy-Hb] was used as an indicator of change in regional cerebral blood volume, because that is more sensitive than [deoxy-Hb] as a parameter for measuring blood flow changes associated with brain activation [35], and has a stronger correlation with blood-oxygenation-level-dependent signals measured by fMRI [36]. The sampling interval was 0.1 seconds. During the measurements, the subject was instructed to open their eyes and gaze at a point forward. Each trial was repeated 6 times and data obtained were averaged using the ‘integral mode’ of the ETG-100 software for the SHAPE, SHAM, and REST sessions. The moving average method (moving average window, 5 seconds) was used to exclude short-term motion artifacts. The baselines for measurements were corrected using linear fitting [37], which was performed by connecting the pre-task baseline (mean of the last 20 seconds of the pre-task period) to the post-task baseline (mean of the last 20 seconds of the post-task period).
Anatomical identification of NIRS channels
The coordinates of all probe positions and anatomical landmark positions (Nz, Iz, A1, A2, Cz) were obtained using a 3-dimensional digitizer (3SPACE ISOTRAK2, Polhemus, US) and transcribed into Montreal Neurological Institute standard brain space [38]–[39] using probabilistic registration [40]–[41]. The probe positions were then projected onto the cortical surface. Next, the anatomical localization corresponding to the probe coordinates was identified using Platform for Optical Topography Analysis Tools (POTATo, Hitachi, Japan) with reference to Automated Anatomical Labeling [42].
Statistical analysis
The value for [oxy-Hb] was calculated every 1 second and compared between the SHAM and REST, SHAPE and REST, and SHAPE and SHAM sessions using paired t-tests implemented with a plug-in-based analysis platform that runs on MATLAB (The MathWorks Inc.), and values showing p<0.05 were considered to be significantly different. A topographical representation of significant (p<0.05) channels every 1 second was projected onto the occipital cortical surface of a Montreal Neurological Institute standard brain space [43]–[44] using a 3-dimensional composite display unit (version 2.41, Hitachi Medical Co. Chiba Japan) [45].
It has been shown that as the number of items being examined increases, so does the risk of type 1 errors [46]. Thus, in order to avoid such errors, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was applied and multiple comparisons using Tukey's method for the time course of averaged data of [oxy-Hb] every 1 second between the task periods of the SHAM and REST, SHAPE and REST, and SHAPE and SHAM sessions were performed. P-values less than 0.05 were considered to indicate significance.
Results
1. Anatomical identification of NIRS channels
Anatomical identification of the NIRS channels is shown in Figure 2. Those channels were localized in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17).
2. Grand averaged waveforms and topographical maps
The grand averaged waveforms for changes in [oxy-Hb] and [deoxy-Hb] during the REST, SHAM, and SHAPE sessions are shown in Figures 3A, B, and C, respectively. Figure 3 shows the topography of change in [oxy-Hb] during the pre-task, task, and post-task periods in the REST, SHAM, and SHAPE sessions. In the REST sessions, the change in [oxy-Hb] was scant throughout the session and there was no apparent change in [oxy-Hb] in the occipital lobe (Fig. 3A). In the SHAM session, there was a slight increase in [oxy-Hb] in the extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19) and striate cortex (BA17) during the task period (Fig. 3B), whereas there were marked increases in [oxy-Hb] in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and primary visual cortex (BA17) during the task and post-task periods in the SHAPE session (Fig. 3C).
3. Cross-sectional statistical topographies
3-1. Comparison between SHAM and REST sessions
The values for [oxy-Hb] in the task and post-task periods of the SHAM session were slightly but significantly increased (p<0.05, paired t-test) as compared to those of the REST session in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and primary visual cortex (BA17) (Fig. 4A).
3-2. Comparison between SHAPE and REST sessions
In the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17), the values for [oxy-Hb] in the task and post-task periods of the SHAPE session were significantly increased (p<0.05, paired t-test) as compared to the REST session (Fig. 4B).
4. Cross-sectional statistical topographies of [oxy-Hb] in the SHAPE and SHAM sessions
In the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17), the values for [oxy-Hb] in the task and the post-task periods of the SHAPE session were significantly increased (p<0.05, paired t-test) as compared to the SHAM session (Fig. 5).
5. Temporal changes in [oxy-Hb] in REST, SHAM, and SHAPE sessions
Following the statistical comparison using paired t-tests between values obtained in the SHAPE and REST sessions (Fig. 5), we performed two-way repeated-measure ANOVA of data obtained from the channels, which showed a significant increase in [oxy-Hb] presented in Fig. 6. Our findings showed a significant correlation between time and task (p<0.05). In addition, post-hoc tests indicated statistically significant differences between the SHAPE and SHAM, SHAPE and REST, and SHAM and REST sessions (p<0.05, Tukey's method for multiple comparisons).
Significant differences regarding changes in [oxy-Hb] during the task periods were seen in channels CH2 (posterior parietal cortex/precuneus, BA7) (Fig. 6A), CH5 (posterior parietal cortex/exstrastriate cortex/BA7, 19) (Fig. 6B), CH14 (extrastriate cortex, BA18, 19) (Fig. 6C), and CH17 (striate cortex/BA17) (Fig. 6D) between the SHAPE and REST, and SHAPE and SHAM sessions (p<0.05, Tukey's method for multiple comparisons). Furthermore, there was a significant difference noted in channel CH17 (BA17), which presented a more long-lasting activation of [oxy-Hb], between the SHAPE and SHAM, and SHAM and REST sessions (Fig. 6D) (p<0.05, Tukey's method for multiple comparisons).
Discussion
In the present study, we obtained the following notable results. Activation was seen in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, BA19), and striate cortex (BA17) during the SHAM control session without test pieces as compared to the REST session. We also noted activation in the PPC (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19) and striate cortex (BA17) in the SHAPE session with test pieces as compared to the REST session. Furthermore, specific activation was seen in the PPC (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19) and striate cortex (BA17) in the SHAPE session as compared to the SHAM session.
Based on our results, we will first discuss neural substrates related to reference imagery such as shape memory and imagery, which were possibly activated by our explanation of the test pieces and rehearsal preceding the sessions in the present subjects. In addition, we discuss verification of target imagery arising from manipulation of the test piece in the mouth. Finally, we speculate that the effects of target imagery facilitate parietal and occipital cortex activities.
Effects of SHAM session as compared to REST
We found activation in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA 18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17) during the SHAM discrimination sessions without test pieces as compared to the REST session. As noted above, preceding performance of the REST, SHAM, and SHAPE sessions, the participants were presented the test pieces and rehearsed shape discrimination without a test piece in the mouth. Therefore, even with no test pieces to manipulate, shape memory and imagery may have activated the parietal and occipital cortices during the SHAM session.
Mayer et al. [47] reported a common neural substrate for visual working memory and attention in the prefrontal cortex, visual, and parietal areas. Also, Kosslyn et al. [48] and Stokes et al. [49] noted the involvement of primary visual cortex activation in visual imagery, while Cavanna et al. [50] reported visual memory induced in the PPC, and Todd et al. [51], Sathian et al. [52], and Parra et al. [53] found that was activated in the PPC and extrastriate cortex during working memory activation. Also, Ven et al. [54] applied TMS to examine visual cortex disruption resulting in memory interference. Furthermore, Jonides et al. [55] reported that rehearsal of information entering working memory from the visual external world is processed by structures in the parietal and temporal lobes specialized for perceptual processing, and these same structures remain active when the stimulus is removed for a brief duration. From these and our findings, it is speculated that activations of the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA 18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17) during the SHAM control session could be elicited by shape imagery and/or shape memory introduced by the instructions and rehearsals preceding the sessions.
Effects of SHAPE session as compared to REST
Activation in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17) in the SHAPE session as compared with the REST session was observed in our study.
Ptito et al. [2] reported that tactile stimulation of the tongue evoked posterior parietal cortex and extrastriate cortex activities, while James et al. [3] and Sathian et al. [56] found exstrastriate cortex activation during shape discrimination by fingers trials. Nakashita et al. [57] also reported tactile-visual integration in the posterior parietal cortex and Pasalar et al. [58] noted that application of TMS to the posterior parietal cortex disrupted shape discrimination. Furthermore, Hannula et al. [59] and Zangaladze et al. [60] applied TMS stimulation to the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and confirmed disruption in tactile temporal discrimination.
When considering tactile shape discrimination from the neural substrate in bottom-up, top-down, and cross-modal tactile/visual association, it is also speculated that bottom-up oral tactile sensation may undergo a top-down processing from posterior parietal cortex (BA7) toward the extrastriate and striate cortices [11]–[12].
Furthermore, in consideration of the aspect of behavior during the SHAPE session, we observed that the subjects likely rotated the test piece with the tongue and lips in order to understand the angular gradient. This manipulation was sufficient for mental rotation by the test piece in the mouth, which induced activation of the posterior parietal cortex and extrastriate cortex. Keeher et al. [61] reported that the superior parietal cortex exhibited a positive linear relationship hemodynamic response and degree of rotation, while Cohen et al. [62] also found activation of the posterior parietal cortex during mental rotation of shape. Taken together, target imagery including bottom-up oral tactile sensation during shape discrimination by the mouth may be associated with the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA 18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17), and these neural substrates in parietal and occipital lobes also may play a role in mental rotation by the test piece in the mouth.
Parietal and occipital cortex activation related to presence or absence of test piece in mouth during shape discrimination
In the present trials, we noted significant activation in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and visual cortex (BA17), based on the statistically significant differences between the SHAPE and SHAM sessions. Furthermore, the activation was identical with that seen in the SHAM session. These results suggest that target imagery induced by the test piece in the mouth may have activated the identical cortical regions in the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19) and striate cortex (BA17) as seen in the SHAM session. Thus, we consider that target imagery including bottom-up oral sensations facilitates shape imagery and shape memory related to cortical activation in those 3 areas. Our findings also revealed long-lasting activation of [oxy-Hb] during the post-task periods in the SHAPE and SHAM, and SHAM and REST sessions (Fig. 6D). Visual cortex activation in the post-task period is thought to occur as storage of shape memory during recognition by shape discrimination.
Ptito et al. [2] reported activation in the inferior parietal, precuneus, and prefrontal cortices, but not cuneus by tongue tactile stimulation in healthy control subjects using a tongue display unit (TDU). Considering the differences in data between that study obtained by passive stimulation to the tongue and the present study obtained by shape discrimination in the mouth, the latter method is thought to be more effective for verification of the test pieces, as well as shape and memory imagery [48], [54]–[55] and mental rotation [61]–[62]. This is the first known study to report parietal and occipital cortex activation during shape discrimination by the mouth.
Clinical implications
Oral examinations using shape discrimination have been conducted in dental research, especially in the field of prosthodontics, in order to elucidate chewing ability and oral sensitivity, and the relationships between chewing ability and oral sensitivity. However, as noted above, the procedures used for oral shape discrimination in past investigations are inconsistent in regard to presentation of the test pieces preceding the shape discrimination task. A standard method for oral discrimination is necessary to provide reliable findings from investigations of both healthy controls and patients.
In the present study, parietal and occipital cortical activations during shape discrimination by the mouth were observed. In contrast, Fujii et al. [25] reported activation in the prefrontal cortex but not the occipital cortex. According to Lacey and Sathian [11], haptic perception of a familiar shape uses visual object imagery via top-down paths from the prefrontal and parietal areas, whereas haptic perception of an unfamiliar shape may use special imagery processes and involve bottom-up pathways from the somatosensory cortex. Therefore, we speculate that reference imagery with explanation of the test pieces may produce familiarity with the pieces, which may facilitate top-down processing from the prefrontal and parietal cortices to parietal and occipital cortical activities. Because of familiarity with the test pieces, our data may be inconsistent with prior research results presented by Fujii et al. [25].
Additional investigations are necessary for better understanding of the effects of reference imagery in order to evaluate oral sensory perception ability in healthy subjects, as well as aged patients who are edentulous, and patients with movement disorders or psychiatric issues. Our results warrant further study regarding how reference imagery modulates the activation of neural substrates induced by target imagery during shape discrimination by the mouth, and how deprivation of intraoral tactile sensation modulates the neural substrates in the parietal and occipital lobes.
Summary and conclusions
The following results were obtained in this study. After recognition of the shape of the test piece, shape discrimination without the piece in the mouth activated the extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19) and striate cortex (BA17). Furthermore, shape discrimination with the test piece activated the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19), and striate cortex (BA17), and specifically activated the posterior parietal cortex (BA7), extrastriate cortex (BA18, 19) and striate cortex (BA17), as compared to the SHAM session without the test pieces. We concluded that parietal and occipital cortical areas may play important roles in intraoral somatosensory shape discrimination.
Funding Statement
The authors have no support or funding to report.
References
- 1. Sadato N, Pascual-Leone A, Grafman J, Ibañez V, Deiber MP, et al. (1996) Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature 380: 526–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Ptito M, Matteau I, Zhi Wang A, Paulson OB, Siebner HR, et al. (2012) Crossmodal recruitment of the ventral visual stream in congenital blindness. Neural Plasticity 2012: 304045 10.1155/2012/304045). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. James TW, Humphrey GK, Gati JS, Servos P, Menon RS, et al. (2002) Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas. Neuropsychologia 40: 1706–1714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Amedi A, Jacobson G, Hendler T, Malach R, Zohary E (2001) Convergence of visual and tactile shape processing in the human lateral occipital complex zohary. Cerebral Cortex 12: 1202–1212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Zhang M, Weisser VD, Stilla R, Prather SC, Sathian K (2004) Multisensory cortical processing of object shape and its relation to mental imagery. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 4: 251–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Stoesz MR, Zhang M, Weisser VD, Prather SC, Mao H, et al. (2003) Neural networks active during tactile form perception: common and differential activity during macrospatial and microspatial tasks. International Journal of Psychophysiology 50: 41–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Prather SC, Votaw JR, Sathian K (2004) Task-specific recruitment of dorsal and ventral visual areas during tactile perception. Neuropsychologia 42: 1079–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Sathian K (2005) Visual cortical activity during tactile perception in the sighted and the visually deprived. Dev Psychobiol 46: 279–286 10.1002/dev.20056). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Kauffman T, Théoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2002) Braille character discrimination in blindfolded human subjects. Neuroreport 13: 571–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Weisser V, Stilla R, Peltier S, Hu X, Sathian K (2005) Short-term visual deprivation alters neural processing of tactile form. Exp Brain Res 166: 572–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lacey S, Sathian K (2012) Representation of Object Form in Vision and Touch. In: Murray MM, Wallace MT, editors. The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press Chapter 10. [PubMed]
- 12. Qin W, Yu C (2013) Neural pathways conveying novisual information to the visual cortex. Neural Plast 2013: 864920 10.1155/2013/864920). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Kumamoto Y, Kaiba Y, Imamura S, Minakuchi S (2010) Influence of palatal coverage on oral function - Oral stereognostic ability and masticatory efficiency. J Prosthodont Res 54: 92–96 10.1016/j.jpor.2009.11.006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Hirano K, Hirano S, Hayakawa I (2004) The role of oral sensorimotor function in masticatory ability. J Oral Rehabil 31: 199–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Garrett NR, Kapur KK, Jochen DG (1994) Oral Stereognostic Ability and Masticatory Performance in Denture Wearers. Int J Prosthodont 7: 567–573. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Kaiba Y, Hirano S, Hayakawa I (2006) Palatal Coverage Disturbance in Masticatory Function. J Med Dent Sci 53: 1–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Ikebe K, Amemiya M, Morii K, Matsuda K, Furuya M, et al. (2007) Association between oral stereognostic ability and masticatory performance in aged complete denture wearers. Int J Prosthodont 20: 245–250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Jacobs R, Serhal CB, Steenberghe D (1998) Oral stereognosis: a review of the literature. Clin Oral Invest 2: 3–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Engelen L, van der Bilt A, Bosman F (2004) Relationship between oral sensitivity and masticatory performance. J Dent Res 83: 388–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Ikebe K, Amemiya M, Morii K, Matsuda K, Furuya YM, et al. (2007) Comparison of oral stereognosis in relation to age and the use of complete dentures. J Oral Rehabil 34: 345–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Amarasena J, Jayasinghe V, Amarasena N, Yamada Y (2010) Oral Stereognostic Ability during Adaptation to New Dentures in Experienced and Non-experienced Complete Denture Wearers. J. Oral Biosci 52: 181–186. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Leung KC, Pow EH, McMillan AS, Wong MC, Li LS, et al. (2002) Oral perception and oral motor ability in edentulous patients with stroke and Parkinson's disease. Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 29: 497–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Pow EH, Leung KC, McMillan AS, Wong MC, Li LS, et al. (2001) Oral stereognosis in stroke and Parkinson's disease a comparison of partially dentate and edentulous individuals. Clin Oral Investig 5: 112–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Kawagishi S, Kou F, Yoshino K, Tanaka T, Masumi S (2009) Decrease in stereognostic ability of the tongue with age. J Oral Rehabil 36: 872–879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Fujii R, Takahashi T, Toyomaru A, Miyamoto T, Ueno T, et al. (2011) Comparison of cerebral activation involved in oral and manual stereognosis. J Clin Neurosci 18: 1520–1523 10.1016/j.jocn.2011.03.005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Yamamura K, Kurose M, Narita N, Yamada Y (2008) Activity of the Cerebral Cortex during Intraoral Form-discrimination Task. IADR 86th General Session & Exhibition abstract 3505.
- 27. Takahashi K, Ogata S, Atsumi Y, Yamamoto R, Shiotsuka S, et al. (2000) Activation of the visual cortex imaged by 24-channel near-infrared spectroscopy. J Biomed Opt 5: 93–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Kojima H, Suzuki T (2010) Hemodynamic change in occipital lobe during visual search: visual attention allocation measured with NIRS. Neuropsychologia 48: 349–352 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.028). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Maehara G, Taya S, Kojima H (2007) Changes in hemoglobin concentration in the lateral occipital regions during shape recognition: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. J Biomed Opt 12: 062109 10.1117/1.2815720). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP (1983) The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 67: 361–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Narita N, Kamiya K, Yamamura K, Kawasaki S, Matsumoto T, et al. (2009) Chewing-related prefrontal cortex activation while wearing partial denture prosthesis: pilot study. J Prosthodont Res 53: 126–135 10.1016/j.jpor.2009.02.005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Okada E, Firbank M, Schweiger M, SArridge SR, Cope M, et al. (1997) Theoretical and experimental investigation of near- infrared light propagation in a model of the adult head. Appl Opt 36: 21–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Tamura M, Hoshi Y, Okada F (1997) Localized near-infrared spectroscopy and functional optical imaging of brain activity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 352: 737–742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Jasper HH (1958) The ten twenty electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 10: 371–375. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Hoshi Y, Kobayashi N, Tamura M (2001) Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J Appl Physiol 90: 1657–1662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Strangman G, Culver JP, Thompson JH, Boas DA (2002) A Quantitative Comparison of Simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS Recordings during Functional Brain Activation. Neuroimage 17: 719–731. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Sawa M, Yamashita H, Fujimori K, Okada G, Takahashi T, et al. (2012) Depressive symptoms and apathy are associated with psychomotor slowness and frontal activation. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 262: 493–499 10.1007/s00406-012-0296-9). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Brett M, Johnsrude IS, Owen AM (2002) The problem of functional localization in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 243–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC (1994) Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr 18: 192–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Okamoto M, Dan I (2005) Automated cortical projection of head-surface locations for transcranial functional brain mapping. Neuroimage 26: 18–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Singh AK, Okamoto M, Dan H, Jurcak V, Dan I (2005) Spatial registration of multichannel multi-subject fNIRS data to MNI space without MRI. Neuroimage 27: 842–851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, et al. (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15: 273–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Tian F, Liu H (2014) Depth-compensated diffuse optical tomography enhanced by general linear model analysis and an anatomical atlas of human head. Neuroimage 15: 166–180 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Okamoto M, Dan H, Sakamoto K, Takeo K, Shimizu K, et al. (2004) Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. Neuroimage 21: 99–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Ichikawa N, Kaga M, Fujiwara M, Kawasaki S, Kawaguchi F (2001) Development of Optical Topography System ETG-100. MEDIX 34: 47–52. [Google Scholar]
- 46. Marumo K, Takizawa R, Kinou M, Kawasaki S, Kawakubo Y, et al. (2014) Functional abnormalities in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during a semantic fluency task, and their association with thought disorder in patients with schizophrenia. Neuroimage 15: 518–526 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.050). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Mayer JS, Bittner RA, Nikolić D, Bledowski C, Goebel R, et al. (2007) Common neural substrates for visual working memory and attention. Neuroimage 36: 441–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Kosslyn SM, Pascual-Leone A, Felician O, Camposano S, Keenan JP, et al. (1999) The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science 284: 167–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Stokes M, Saraiva A, Rohenkohl G, Nobre AC (2011) Imagery for shapes activates position-invariant representations in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 56: 1540–1545 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.071). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129: 564–583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Todd JJ, Marois R (2005) Posterior parietal cortex activity predicts individual differences in visual short-term memory capacity. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 5: 144–155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Sathian K, Zangaladze A, Hoffman JM, Grafton ST (1997) Feeling with the mind's eye. Neuroreport 8: 3877–3881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Parra MA, Della Sala S, Logie RH, Morcom AM (2014) Neural correlates of shape-color binding in visual working memory. Neuropsychologia 52: 27–36 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.036). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. van de Ven V, Sack AT (2013) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of visual cortex in memory: cortical state, interference and reactivation of visual content in memory. Behav Brain Res 236: 67–77 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.08.001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Jonides J, Lacey S, Nee DE (2005) Processes of working memory in mind and brain. Curr Dir Psychol Sc 14: 2–5. [Google Scholar]
- 56. Sathian K, Zangaladze A (2002) Feeling with the mind's eye: contribution of visual cortex to tactile perception. Behav Brain Res 20 135: 127–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Nakashita S, Saito DN, Kochiyama T, Honda M, Tanabe HC, et al. (2008) Tactile-visual integration in the posterior parietal cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Res Bull 75: 513–525 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.09.004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Pasalar S, Ro T, Beauchamp MS (2010) TMS of posterior parietal cortex disrupts visual tactile multisensory integration. Eur J Neurosci 31: 1783–1790 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07193.x). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Hannula H, Neuvonen T, Savolainen P, Tukiainen T, Salonen O, et al. (2008) Navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation of the primary somatosensory cortex impairs perceptual processing of tactile temporal discrimination. Neurosci Lett 437: 144–147 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.03.093). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Zangaladze A, Epstein CM, Grafton ST, Sathian K (1999) Involvement of visual cortex in tactile discrimination of orientation. Nature 401: 587–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Keehner M, Guerin SA, Miller MB, Turk DJ, Hegarty M (2006) Modulation of neural activity by angle of rotation during imagined spatial transformations. Neuroimage 33: 391–398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Cohen MS, Kosslyn SM, Breiter HC, DiGirolamo GJ, Thompson WL, et al. (1996) Changes in cortical activity during mental rotation. A mapping study using functional MRI. Brain 119: 89–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]