Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Autism Dev Disord. 2014 Nov;44(11):2769–2777. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2137-3

A Randomized Clinical Trial Comparison Between Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) and Structured Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) Intervention for Children with Autism

Fereshteh Mohammadzaheri 1, Lynn Kern Koegel 2, Mohammad Rezaee 3, Seyed Majid Rafiee 4
PMCID: PMC4194254  NIHMSID: NIHMS597091  PMID: 24840596

Abstract

Accumulating studies are documenting specific motivational variables that, when combined into a naturalistic teaching paradigm, can positively influence the effectiveness of interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The purpose of this study was to compare two ABA intervention procedures, a naturalistic approach, Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) with a structured ABA approach in a school setting. A Randomized Clinical Trial design using two groups of children, matched according to age, sex and mean length of utterance was used to compare the interventions. The data showed that the PRT approach was significantly more effective in improving targeted and untargeted areas after three months of intervention. The results are discussed in terms of variables that produce more rapid improvements in communication for children with ASD.

Keywords: Pragmatic skills, naturalistic approach, analog approach, autism, pivotal response treatment, ABA

Introduction

Milestones in language and social communication play a major role at almost every point in development. However, for children autism spectrum disorder (ASD) a defining characteristic of the disability is difficulty with social communication across contexts (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In fact, most parents of children with ASD first become concerned about their child's development because of early delays or regressions in the acquisition of verbal communication (Locke, Ishijima, Kasari, & London, 2010; Short & Schopler, 1998). Children with ASD may experience delays in the onset of verbal expressive language, and some may remain nonverbal throughout life (Prizant, Wetherby, Rubin, & Laurent, 2003; Koegel & Koegel, 2006). For those who do learn to use expressive verbal communication, many have difficulty using communication effectively to accomplish social interactive goals (Donno, Parker, Gilmour, & Skuse, 2010).

In addition to social goals, interventions for communication delays are critical, as a myriad of other challenges are correlated with language difficulties (Landa, 2007), including increased disruptive behaviors (Carr & Durand, 1985), academic difficulties (Catts, 1996), reduced levels of play (Ungerer & Sigman, 1984), and so on. In contrast, functional language use by school age has been shown to relate to better long-term outcomes in individuals with autism (DeMyer, Hingtgen, & Jackson, 1981; Lovaas, 1987). Thus, the need for effective and efficient interventions that address communication is essential.

A variety of treatment approaches have been developed to address the social communication of children with (ASD). The most commonly used treatment options for ASD are derived from the field of behavior analysis (ABA) based on theories of learning and operant conditioning (Lovaas, 1987), as they are evidence-based (National Research Council, 2001; National Standard Project, 2009; Simpson, 2005). Structured ABA approaches define discrete intervention targets, which are addressed through massed trials of antecedent-behavior-consequence chains. They use adult-selected materials that are presented repeatedly to promote success. Tight control over the antecedent stimuli, prompt hierarchy, and consequences are maintained and tokens or edibles paired with verbal praise are provided contingent upon correct responses (see detailed description in Landa, 2007).

The increase in the prevalence of ASD (Blumburg, Bramlett, Kogan, Schieve, Jones, & Lu, 2013) along with the documented effectiveness of ABA has led to the development of several comprehensive treatment programs and curricula (Leaf & McEachin, 1999; Green & Luce, 1996). Many programs that utilize discrete trial training teach individual skills one at a time through drill-based repetition of learning trials (e.g., Lovaas 1987). These approaches require as many as 40 hours per week, but boast success rates of almost half of the children being indistinguishable from their peers following intensive early intervention (Lovaas, 1987).

While the structured ABA procedures are very effective in producing behavioral changes in a wide variety of areas, the literature has discussed three major difficulties encountered with the intervention: (a) gains are extremely slow (often requiring many thousands of trials to teach a single word); (b) when gains occur they often do not generalize; and (c) the children typically are often unmotivated to be involved in the teaching sessions, frequently exhibiting escape-motivated disruptive behaviors (Koegel, R.L., Camarata, Koegel, L.K., Ben-Tall, & Smith, 1998). Consequently, the ABA approaches to intervention often require massive numbers of trials presented repeatedly in an analog teaching paradigm for the children to show success. This can be extremely time-consuming for all involved.

In response to the slow acquisition of target behaviors and high levels of disruptive behaviors, a body of research has focused on variables that increase the child's responsiveness to the task. An outgrowth of these structured ABA approaches are more child-directed naturalistic behavioral methods (Hancock & Kaiser, 2002; Koegel, L.K., Koegel, J.K., Harrower, & Carter, 1999a; Koegel, L.K., et. al. 1999b) and social-pragmatic interventions that use developmental theory as a guide (Dawson, et. al., 2010; Greenspan, Wieder, & Simons, 1998; Greenspan & Weider, 1999; Mahoney & Perales, 2003; Prizant, et. al., 2003; Salt, et. al., 2002). Naturalistic models, such as Pivotal Response Treatment (Ingersoll & Schreibman, 2006; Koegel, L.K., et. al., 1999a) target specific skills as well as core pivotal areas (e.g., motivation) which result in widespread gains in untargeted areas, such as joint attention (Matson, Benavidez, Compton, Paclawyskyj, & Baglio, 1996), affect (Koegel, Bimbela, Schreibman, 1996), and decreases in untreated disruptive behavior (Koegel, Koegel, & Surratt, 1992). Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) relies on operant teaching principles and has been used to target a wide range of deficits, including social skills and communication (Handleman & Harris, 2001).

To be specific, PRT is an intervention approach based on behavioral principles of ABA that focuses on incorporating variables known to improve responsiveness, rate of responding, and positive affect. These variables include child choice (Koegel, Dyer, & Bell, 1987), task variation (Dunlap, 1984), interspersing maintenance and acquisition trials (Dunlap, 1984), reinforcing attempts (Koegel, O’Dell, & Dunlap, 1988), and using direct natural consequences (Koegel & Williams, 1980; Williams, Koegel, & Egel 1981). As a package, these variables have been shown to be extremely effective, when compared to structured ABA approaches. Most of the studies comparing the two procedures have used single subject designs (Koegel, et. al., 1987). Thus, there is a need for Randomized Clinical Trial (RCT) designs that compare PRT with structured ABA (which is currently the standard of care in many clinics and schools) as a treatment as usual control. Therefore, we conducted the current study to examine the effectiveness PRT compared to structured ABA to improve communication deficits in 6–11 years old children with autism using a RCT design. The specific questions asked in this study were: (1) Would PRT or structured ABA result in greater gains in targeted language areas (mean length of utterance); and (2) Would PRT or structured ABA result in greater generalized gains in untreated areas as measured by a standardized communication checklist (the Children’s Communication Checklist, Bishop, 2006) completed by each participant’s teacher and parent.

Methods

Participants

Thirty children, 18 boys & 12 girls, ranging in age from 6 to 11 years, participated in this study. Each child: (1) was diagnosed with autism by an child psychiatrist according to the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and was referred to the Hamedan University of Medical Sciences and Health Services, Iran, for intervention. In addition, the first author screened each child for symptoms of ASD prior to the start of the study. As well, each child was screened by the public school system and placed in special education classrooms for children with autism spectrum disorder; (2) used expressive verbal communication with a mean length of utterance (MLU) of at least two words; (3) had no vision or hearing loss; (5) had no other co-morbid psychiatric disorders; (6) was not bilingual; and (7) had an Intelligence Quotient (IQ) of at least 50 indicating that they exhibited mild to moderate intellectual impairments. To be specific, in the traditional ABA group thirteen of the fifteen children had a reported IQ between 50–60 and two children had a reported IQ of 60–70. In the PRT group twelve of the fifteen children had a reported IQ between 50–60 and three children had a reported IQ between 60–70. All of the children attended self-contained special education classrooms, but The present study was implemented during the summer when the typical full school day was not in session, however the students spent 2 hours per day, four days per week, for a total of eight hours per week in school. Two of the eight hours were spent participating in the present study and the students spent the remaining six hours with their teacher previewing academic material that would be presented during the upcoming school year. An outside treatment record indicated that none of the children received any other type of intervention during the entirety of this study. Participant information is listed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Participant Characteristics

Characteristics ABA group PRT group
n % n %
Gender
Male 9 60 9 60
Female 6 40 6 40

Ethnicity
Iranian 100 100

ABA group PRT group
M SD M SD p

Age (in months) 110.47 18.62 110.67 18.71 ns
MLU 2.77 .49 2.76 .50 ns

Randomization. This study was interested in a subpopulation of verbal children with autism. Therefore, prior to the start of intervention, teachers were asked to nominate students that fit a predetermined list of criteria that was necessary for inclusion in the study. Based on this list, a total of 15 dyads were conveniently selected who were matched by age, sex, and MLU. Each participant in each dyad was then randomly assigned to one of two treatment groups. This resulted in 15 participants being randomly assigned to an ABA treatment group and their matched counterparts being randomly assigned to a PRT treatment group. None of the participants (teachers or parents) completing the assessments were aware of the intervention to which they were assigned, nor did they have access to the randomization list.

Implementer Training and Setting

All sessions were conducted in a small (10’ by 15’) treatment room at the public school site using a one-to-one teacher-child format. The treatment rooms contained a table, chairs, and stimulus materials relevant to each intervention. One of the treatment providers for each intervention was a speech/language specialist who held a master’s degree. Additionally, five advanced graduate students in speech and hearing sciences assisted with the intervention (three for the ABA group and two for the PRT group). In total, four individuals implemented the sessions for the structured ABA group and three different individuals implemented the sessions for the PRT group. The treatment providers had previous experience teaching children with autism using structured ABA procedures, which was the standard of care when this study was implemented. Prior to the start of the study, the implementers met with individuals with experience in the interventions and were provided with specific methodologies for their respective intervention. Specifically, the clinicians in the PRT group read How to teach pivotal behaviors to children with autism: A training manual (1989) and the clinicians in the structured ABA group read the Lovaas ebook (1981). Both groups emailed consultants in California with expertise in each methodology, who provided input on the use of the procedures with regard to specific students, approximately once weekly throughout the study. Since structured ABA was being used with the students, treatment continued as usual but procedures were discussed to assure that Fidelity of Implementation would be met. For the PRT session, the treatment providers were taught to incorporate motivational strategies into the ABA intervention. Each child was observed during at least four separate sessions and fidelity of implementation was scored for correct/incorrect implementation of the procedures by the first author. Observations were either in-vivo or by videotaped and scored following the session. For each observation a total of ten minutes was scored in one-minute intervals, and each of the 7 points were scored as correct (+) or incorrect (-) according to each of the variables outlined in Table 2 and in accordance with previous publications (Bryson, Koegel, Koegel, Openden, Smith, & Nefdt, 2007; Koegel, O’Dell, & Dunlap, 1988). Specifically, in both interventions the teacher had to obtain the child’s attention, provide a clear opportunity, and provide contingent consequences. Four areas differentiated the interventions (see below). Fidelity of Implementation averaged 85% (range 80%–90%) and never fell below the required minimum 80% level throughout the study (Bryson, Koegel, Koegel, Openden, Smith, & Nefdt, 2007).

Table 2.

Definitions for Fidelity of Implementation. An asterisk (*) indicates differences between the two conditions.

Child Attending
The interventionist must have the child’s attention prior to presenting an opportunity.
Clear Opportunity
The question/instruction/opportunity (SD) to respond must be clearand appropriate to the
task.
Child Choice *
For PRT groups the interventionist should follow the child’s choice with tasks and
activities. However, the interventionist must always assume control should the child
engage in hazardous (i.e. self-injury) or inappropriate (i.e. self-stimulation) activities. If
child is not showing interest in the current task, interventionist should attempt to change
the activity.
For the ABA group the interventionist should choose the materials or activity that is
relevant to the target behavior.
Maintenance Tasks *
For the PRT intervention, the interventionist should be interspersing tasks the child can
already perform with acquisition (new) tasks. For ABA groups the target behavior
should be worked on exclusively.
Contingent
Reinforcement must be contingent upon child’s behavior. The interventionist’s response
(i.e. giving the child a reinforcer) must be dependent upon the child’s response (i.e. saying “little toy”).
Natural *
For the PRT intervention reinforcement should be natural or directly related to the
desired behavior. For ABA intervention reinforcement should included food items,
activities, or items (stickers) that the child enjoys but are unrelated to the intervention.
Contingent on Attempts *
For PRT any goal-directed attempt to respond to questions, instructions, or opportunities
should be reinforced. Although an attempt does not necessarily need to be correct, it has
to be reasonable. For ABA intervention a strict shaping paradigm must be used wherein
each rewards are provided upon correct responses or responses that are at least as good or
better than the previous response.

Materials and Target Behavior

All sessions focused on improving verbal expressive communication by expanding the child’s Mean Length of Utterance (MLU). For children that participated in the structured ABA intervention, task materials included commercially purchased picture cards to evoke the target response. Each child’s favorite foods, toys, and other desired activities were provided for rewards. For the children that participated in the PRT intervention, a variety of child-chosen foods, toys, and activities were provided for rewards.

Procedures

Baseline

Prior to the implementation of treatment, each child was given a series of six pictures that they were asked to describe. Language samples were collected on the children’s responses and later analyzed for MLU. Utterance segmentation was based on pause or change of topic (Miller, 1981). For each child, the total number of words emitted for the six cards was divided by the total number of utterances to yield an MLU. Following the determination of MLU, each child was matched according to age, gender, and MLU and randomly assigned to one of two groups: (1) a control (treatment as usual) group consisting of 15 children that received structured ABA intervention, or (2) an experimental group consisting of 15 children that received PRT intervention. In addition, prior to the start of intervention, each child was given the (CCC) by a speech-language specialist (see below).

Intervention

Following baseline assessments, one of the treatments (structured ABA or PRT) was implemented. Treatment sessions were conducted twice weekly for 60 minutes per session over a three month period. Thus, each child received a total of 24 hours of intervention. Parents and teachers were informed that their children/students would receive speech and language services, but were naïve to the specific target behavior (expanding MLU) and the treatment group to which their child was assigned. None of the parents or teachers was present during the intervention sessions. For all children, the target behaviors were the same and involved expanding the children’s MLU using recast procedures (Nelson, Camarata, Welsh, Butkovsky, & Camarata, 1996). All sessions followed the prescribed interventions. During both treatments the speech-language specialists required the child to be attending, provided a clear opportunity for the target behavior, and provided contingent consequences. Four procedures distinguished the two interventions, as follows. First, the materials in the structured ABA session consisted of teacher chosen materials (pre printed picture cards) and the materials in the PRT intervention consisted of child-chosen items and activities. Second, in the structured ABA sessions target behaviors were worked on exclusively, while in the PRT sessions target behaviors were interspersed with previously mastered (maintenance) tasks. Third, in the structured ABA sessions favorite foods and toys were used as rewards along with verbal praise, independent of whether they were related to the target behavior. In the PRT intervention natural rewards that were connected to the target behavior were provided. For example, if the child requested a stuffed animal, the natural reward of being given the stuffed animal was provided contingent upon the longer utterance (or attempt). Fourth, in the structured ABA sessions reinforcement was provided based on a shaping paradigm and in the PRT sessions all attempts were rewarded. Thus, children were rewarded for successively longer utterances in the structured ABA intervention and the length of their responses had to be at least as long as the previous response to be provided with a reinforcer. In the PRT intervention the child was rewarded for both longer utterances in addition to shorter utterances. Specific definitions are listed in Table 2. In order to assess for fidelity of implementation (FoI) supervision was conducted at least once weekly. If a teacher did not meet FoI in a particular area feedback was provided before the subsequent session. However, the overall FoI was achieved at or above the required 80% for both interventions.

Structured ABA (Treatment as Usual) Intervention

The structured ABA intervention was based on the procedures described in Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel (1987). During the structured ABA sessions stimulus materials were chosen by the clinician and consisted of a variety of printed commercial cards depicting various age-appropriate vocabulary items. Trials consisted of attempting to evoke responses through the use of successive trials, with each item presented serially by the clinician. Correct responses or successive approximations were reinforced. Edible reinforcers paired with social reinforcers were provided contingent upon a correct response or successive approximation. These procedures were the standard of care provided to children with autism in the Iranian special education classrooms.

PRT intervention

The PRT intervention was based on the published manual, Pivotal Response Treatment: Using Motivation as a Pivotal Response (Koegel, 2011). In this intervention instead of the clinician arbitrarily selecting a stimulus item, items were selected according to the child's preference for any given item for any given trial. The task was varied so that the reward was provided both for responses that had previously been mastered (in this case, shorter utterances) interspersed with rewards for acquisition tasks (in this case, longer utterances). The reinforcement contingency was broadened so that if the child imitated either the exact correct response or a successive approximation, or made any clear verbal attempt to respond, the child was reinforced. Instead of the child being reinforced with edibles and praise, the child was reinforced with the opportunity to play with the instructional stimulus, paired with verbal praise. An example of the intervention procedure is presented in Table 3.

Table 3.

Examples of the differentiation between structured ABA and PRT intervention sessions.

Structured ABA PRT
Stimulus Materials Commercial flashcards Child-preferred toys
Instruction “What’s that?” “What’s that?” (when child reaches for a toy)
Child Response “Car” “Car”
Recast “Red Car” “Red Car”
Child Response to recast “Red car” “Red car” or “Red Ca”
Consequence to play with
  1. Child given treat or sticker to play with

  2. Shaping paradigm used

  1. Child given the red car to play with

  2. Child rewarded for attempts to use 2 word combinations

Task Variation Flash cards presented
repeatedly/serially
Toys varied during session

Dependent Measures and Data Collection

Data were collected on two measures. In order to assess each child’s gain on the behavior that was targeted (MLU) during the intervention sessions, language samples were collected both prior and following intervention using the same procedures (described above). Each response to the pictures was written down. Then, the total number of words the child emitted was divided by the total number of utterances, to yield an MLU.

In order to assess any generalized gains, prior to intervention and following the completion of the three-month intervention, each child’s parent and teacher (who were naïve to the experimental hypothesis, the target behavior, and the child’s treatment type) were given the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC). The CCC is norm-referenced and is recommended for children age 4;0 to 16;11 years of age. It consists of 70 items that are grouped to 9 subscales. The first two include the structural characteristics of a verbal interaction; a) speech; and b) syntax. The pragmatic domain includes five subscales: c) inappropriate initiation, d) coherence, e) stereotyped language, f) use of context, and g) rapport. The sum of these scales is called the "pragmatic composite". The last two scales are h) social relationship; and i) interests that present the child's nonverbal skills in everyday situations. The scale is especially sensitive to children with ASD as it detects deficits not identified by other communication assessments, as it identifies pragmatic language deficits that are not assessed by language tests that focus exclusively on language fundamentals (Bishop, 2006; Volkmar, Lord, Bailey, Schultz, & Klin, 2004). Reliability. Two separate reliability measures were calculated. First, twelve of the 30 protocols (six from the PRT group and 6 from the ABA group) were scored by an independent observer who was naïve to the experimental hypothesis. This consisted of having a total of 60 subtests scored by the reliability observer. Reliability was considered to occur when the subtest score was identical for both recorders. For individual subtests, reliability was 99% for the PRT group and 99% for the structured ABA subtests. Specifically, the scores differed for one of the 30 subtests for the PRT group (a one point difference) and two of the 30 subtests for the ABA group (a one and two point difference). A second reliability measure was calculated wherein a naïve observer averaged the parent and teacher scores for 25% of the tests. Reliability was considered to occur if the averaged score was identical for both recorders. Reliability was 100% on this measure.

Results

An analyses of demographic and outcome data were conducted to assess for possible differences between the ABA and PRT groups. An independent samples test indicated no significant difference between the two groups in regard to age (t(28) = 0.03, p = .97) and MLU (t(28) = 0.02, p = .98) prior to the start of intervention. Each group consisted of 60% boys and 40% girls. Mean scores were computed for both the teacher and the parent for each measure of the CCC, and then averaged to yield a single composite score. There were no significant differences between the groups on the CCC prior to the start of intervention (t(28) = 0.05, p = 0.96).

In regard to targeted behavior (MLU), pre-intervention analysis showed no significant differences between the two groups prior to the start of intervention, p > .05. Specifically, the MLU score was 2.77 for the children in the structured ABA group and 2.76 for the children in the PRT group. Following the three-month intervention, an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) yielded significant differences between the two groups. Specifically, the MLU of the structured ABA group improved slightly following intervention, but non-significantly, to 2.79. On the other hand, significant improvements were seen in the PRT group, with a mean of 3.20 F (1, 27) = 6.97, p= .01. Thus, the PRT group showed significantly greater gains in MLU following the three-month intervention.

Furthermore, the ANCOVA also revealed significant differences between baseline and post-treatment measures of the CCC between the two groups, F (1, 26) = 6.38, p = .01, with the PRT group showing greater overall gains on this measure as well. Prior to intervention, the average score on the CCC was 118.83 for the structured ABA group and 118.96 for the PRT group, p > .05. However, following intervention the structured ABA group improved slightly, with an average score of 120.53 while the PRT group made larger gains with an average score of 133.70 on the CCC. In summary, the PRT groups showed significantly greater general (non-treatment) improvements following intervention.

Discussion

The results of this study showed that the PRT intervention was more effective at improving social communication skills for children with autism than the structured ABA treatment using an RCT research design. The children who participated in this study demonstrated greater gains both the targeted area (MLU) as well as overall gains in pragmatic skills, including inappropriate initiation, coherence, stereotyped language, use of context, and rapport, as measured by the CCC. Thus, the motivational components of PRT were more effective in producing improvements in social communication (Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987).

There are several potential reasons why the PRT intervention may have been more effective than the structured ABA intervention. First, the use of stimulus items such as preferred toys and activity rather than artificial stimuli such as picture cards used in the analog intervention, and the presentation of the teaching within the context of natural (play) interactions during the PRT sessions, may have created more interest in the teaching sessions, thereby resulting in greater improvements in communication skills. Previous single subject design (multiple baseline) studies have suggested that responsiveness and affect improve when variables such as child choice are considered (Yoder, Kaiser, Alpert, & Fischer, 1993). Second, the literature suggests that children with ASD demonstrate lower levels of off-task and disruptive behavior when motivational components are incorporated (Koegel, et. al., 1992). While the current study did not measure disruptive behavior, it is possible that the participants exhibited more avoidance and escape behavior during the structured ABA intervention, thereby receiving a less effective intervention. Next, research suggests that there is greater generalization using PRT when compared to structured ABA intervention (Koegel, et. al., 1984). While all children appeared to be making progress on their target goals in the clinic sessions, the parents and teachers may have noticed greater gains that occurred in the children’s natural settings as a result of a more widespread generalization when the motivational components were incorporated, which is consistent with other research (Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987). Finally, because PRT focuses on pivotal behaviors, rather than individual target behaviors, a more widespread effect may have occurred (Koegel & Koegel, 2011).

The present study did not incorporate the parents. While this is not a desired or standard procedure, it was helpful to assess the effect of the intervention. That is, the parents were not informed of the target behaviors, and therefore they were unlikely to directly provide intervention or reinforcement in that area, which could have interfered with the study outcome. Further, the study was conducted in a country where few services are available for children with autism. While RCTs are often difficult because of multiple treatment interference when many interventions are available to families (Koegel, Koegel, Ashbaugh, & Bradshaw, 2014), outside treatment records indicated that no other interventions for ASD were taking place while the present study was implemented. Therefore, it was likely that the effect was produced by the study’s intervention. That is, the possibility of other variables, such as co-occurring treatments was minimized in the present study. Further, the children spent very few hours in school because of summer break while our interventions were implemented, which also reduced the possibility of multiple treatment interference. Therefore, we are fairly confident that the intervention gains were the result of the study conditions, and not other confounding variables, such as multiple treatment interference.

Future research may wish to focus on the relative weight of each variable of PRT rather than assessing the effectiveness of the package. Second, it may be interesting to look at disruptive and off-task behaviors, correct vs. incorrect behaviors, and responsiveness, to assess areas that may have resulted differences between the groups. Because the groups were well matched, it would be interesting to make direct comparisons between them, such as analyzing the different domains of the CCC in future research. Also, it would be interesting to understand the children’s trajectories by assessing their skill development at various points in time before the start of intervention as well as to evaluate the long-term follow-up of the interventions. Additionally, the children in this study received relatively few hours of intervention. Assessing outcomes with more intensive doses of the intervention would be interesting. Finally, this study focused on one target behavior (improving MLU). There have been studies showing that other areas are improved when motivational components are used, such as expressive vocabulary (Koegel, O’Dell, & Koegel, 1987), language (Koegel, Koegel, Green-Hopkins, & Barnes, 2010) and academics (Koegel, Singh, & Koegel, 2010), however all of these interventions have been conducted in multiple baseline design studies. Additional research using RCTs that address other target areas should be helpful.

These findings contribute to the overall body of literature supporting PRT, and other naturalistic interventions, as an effective intervention for remediating core symptoms of autism (e.g., language). Structured ABA interventions are widely used as a standard of care, as was the case in this study, despite the fact that incorporating motivational components may result in faster gains of targeted behaviors as well as generalized gains in untreated areas. This finding is consistent with previous studies, which have demonstrated the effectiveness of PRT on other areas, including conversation (Boettcher, 2004), and social initiations (Koegel, L.K., et. al., 1999b). In summary, the present study suggests that incorporating the motivational variables of PRT is more effective for improving MLU and pragmatic skills in children with autism when compared to structured ABA intervention. With large numbers of children being diagnosed with autism, intervention procedures that are more efficient are both time and cost effective. As well, procedures that speed up the habilitation process are important for children with ASD, particularly if they produce widespread gains beyond the specific treatment goals.

Table 4.

MLU and CCC Scores for the structured ABA vs. PRT groups

ABA group PRT group
MLU SD MLU SD p
Pre-Intervention 2.77 .5 2.76 .49 ns
Post-Intervention 2.79 .5 3.20 .78 .01*

CCC
Mean
Score
SD CCC Mean
Score
SD p
Pre-Intervention 118.83 7.53 118.96 6.99 ns
Post-Intervention 120.53 6.99 133.70 5.93 .01*

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This project was supported in part by the Zehne Ziba Rehabilitation Institute in Hamedan, Iran, and University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences in Tehran, Iran, and the National Institute of Health research grant (DC010924). We are grateful to the teachers and parents who completed the measures, and the children who participated in this research. Also, we would like to thank Diane Morovati and Robert L. Koegel for their feedback during the preparation of this manuscript. We would also like to thank the Broad Foundation, the Kind World Foundation, and the Douglas foundation for their support. In addition to being employed at the University of California at Santa Barbara, Dr. Lynn Kern Koegel is also a partner in the private firm, Koegel Autism Consultants.

REFRENCES

  1. American Psychological Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington, D.C: Author; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bishop DVM. Children’s Communication Checklist (2nd ed., U.S. ed.) San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  3. Blumburg SJ, Bramlett MD, Kogan MD, Schieve LA, Jones JR, Lu MC. Changes in prevalence of Parent-reported Autism Spectrum Disorder in school-aged U.S. children: 2007–2011-1012. National Health Statistics Reports. 2013;2013;65 http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/nhsr/nhsr065.pdf. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boettcher MA. Teaching social conversation skills to children with autism through self-management: An analysis of treatment gains and meaningful outcomes. 2004 [Google Scholar]
  5. Carr EG, Durand VM. Reducing behavior problems through functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1985;18(2):111–126. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1985.18-111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Catts HW. The Relationship Between Speech-Language Impairments and Reading Disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1996;36:948–958. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3605.948. 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Center for Disease Control (CDC) Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders- Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 Sites, United States, 2008. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Reports. 2012 Mar 30; Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/pdf/ss/ss6103.pdf. [PubMed]
  8. Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J, Greenson J, Donaldson A, Varley J. Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. Pediatrics. 2010;125(1):e17–e23. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-0958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. DeMyer M, Hingtgen J, Jackson R. Infantile autism reviewed: a decade of research. Schizophrenia Review. 1981;7(3):388–451. doi: 10.1093/schbul/7.3.388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Donno R, Parker G, Gilmour J, Skuse DH. Social communication deficits in disruptive primary-school children. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;196(4):282–289. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.061341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dunlap G. The influence of task variation and maintenance tasks on the learning and affect of autistic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1984;37(1):41–64. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90057-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Greenspan SI, Wieder S. A functional developmental approach to autism spectrum disorders. The Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps. 1999;24(3):147–161. [Google Scholar]
  13. Greenspan SI, Wieder S, Simons R. The child with special needs: Encouraging intellectual and emotional growth. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley Longman; 1998. 1998. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hancock TB, Kaiser AP. The effects of trainer- implemented enhanced milieu teaching on the social communication of children with autism. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. 2002;22(1):39–54. [Google Scholar]
  15. Handleman JS, Harris SL, editors. Preschool education programs for children with autism. Austin, TX: Pro-ed; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  16. Ingersoll B, Schreibman L. Teaching reciprocal imitation skills to young children with autism using a naturalistic behavioral approach: Effects on language, pretend play, and joint attention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(4):487–505. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0089-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Koegel LK. Pivotal Response Treatment: Using Motivation as a Pivotal Response. University of California, Santa Barbara; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  18. Koegel RL, Bimbela A, Schreibman L. Collateral Effects of Parent Training on Family Interactions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1996;26(3):347–359. doi: 10.1007/BF02172479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Koegel RL, Camarata S, Koegel LK, Ben-Tall A, Smith AE. Increasing speech intelligibility in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1998;28(3):241–251. doi: 10.1023/a:1026073522897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Pivotal Response Treatments. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  21. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Ashbaugh K, Bradshaw J. The importance of early identification and intervention for children with or at risk for autism spectrum disorders. International journal of speech-language pathology. 72014;(0):1–7. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2013.861511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Koegel LK, Koegel JK, Harrower JK, Carter CM. Pivotal response intervention I: Overview of approach. The Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps. 1996a;24(3):174–185. [Google Scholar]
  23. Koegel Koegel, Green-Hopkins Barnes. Brief Report: Question-Asking and Collateral Language Acquisition in Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40:509–515. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0896-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Koegel LK, Koegel JK, Harrower JK, Carter CM. Pivotal response intervention I: Overview of approach. The Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps. 1999a;24(3):174–185. [Google Scholar]
  25. Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Shoshan Y, McNerney E. Pivotal response intervention II: Preliminary long-term outcome data. The Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps. 1999b;24(3):186–198. [Google Scholar]
  26. Koegel RL, Koegel LK, Surratt AV. Language intervention and disruptive behavior in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1992;22(2):141–153. doi: 10.1007/BF01058147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Koegel LK, Singh A, Koegel R. Improving Motivation for Academics in Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;Vol 40(9):1057–1066. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0962-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Koegel RL, O’Dell MC, Koegel LK. A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1987;17(2):187–200. doi: 10.1007/BF01495055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Koegel RL, Schreibman L, Good A, Cerniglia L, Murphy C, Koegel LK. How to teach pivotal behaviors to children with autism: A training manual. Santa Barbara, California: University of California; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  30. Koegel RL, Williams JA. Direct versus indirect response-reinforcer relationships in teaching autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1980;8(4):537–547. doi: 10.1007/BF00916505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Landa R. Early communication development and intervention for children with autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13(1):16–25. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20134. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Leaf RB. In: A work in progress: Behavior management strategies and a curriculum for intensive behavioral treatment of autism. McEachin J, editor. New York: Drl Books; 1999. pp. 1417–1418. [Google Scholar]
  33. Locke J, Ishijima EH, Kasari C, London N. Loneliness, friendship quality and the social networks of adolescents with high-functioning autism in an inclusive school setting. Journal of Research in Special Education Needs. 2010;10(3):74–81. [Google Scholar]
  34. Lovaas OI. Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1987;55(1):3–9. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.55.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lovaas OIvar. The me book. Pro-Ed, Inc. Austin, Texas; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  36. Mahoney G, Perales F. Relationship-focused intervention to enhance the social-emotional functioning of young children with autism spectrum disorders. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. 2003;23(2):74–86. [Google Scholar]
  37. Matson JL, Benavidez DA, Compton LS, Paclawskyj T, Baglio CS. Behavioral treatment of autistic persons: A review of research from 1980 to the present. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 1996;17:433–465. doi: 10.1016/s0891-4222(96)00030-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Miller JF. Assessing Language Production in Children: Experimental Procedures. Allyn & Bacon, Boston, NY; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  39. National Autism Center. National standards project: Addressing the need for evidence-based practice guidelines for autism spectrum disorder. Findings and conclusions. 2009 Retrieved from http://www.nationalautismcenter.org/pdf/NAC%20Findings%20&%20Conclusions.pdf.
  40. National Research Council. Educating Children with Autism. Washington, D.C: The National Academy Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  41. Nelson KE, Camarata SM, Welsh J, Butkovsky L, Camarata M. Effects of imitative and conversational recasting treatment on the acquisition of grammar in children with specific language impairment and younger language-normal children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1996;39:850–859. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3904.850. 4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Prizant BM, Wetherby AM, Rubin E, Laurent AC. The SCERTS model: A transactional, family- centered approach to enhancing communication and socioemotional abilities of children with autism spectrum disorder. Infants and Young Children. 2003;16(4):296–316. [Google Scholar]
  43. Salt J, Shemilt J, Sellars V, Boyd S, Coulson T, McCool S. The Scottish centre for autism preschool treatment programme: II. The results of a controlled treatment outcome study. Autism. 2002;6(1):33–46. doi: 10.1177/1362361302006001004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Short A, Schopler E. Factors relating to age of onset in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1988;18:207–16. doi: 10.1007/BF02211947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Simpson RL. Evidence-Based Practices and Students with Autism SpectrumDisorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 2005;20(3):140–149. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ungerer JA, Sigman M. The relation of play and sensorimotor behavior to language in the second year. Child Development. 1984;55:1448–1455. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Williams JA, Koegel RL, Egel AL. Response-reinforcer relationships and improved learning in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1981;14(1):53–60. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1981.14-53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Yoder PJ, Kaiser AP, Alpert C, Fischer R. Following the Child's Lead When Teaching Nouns to Preschoolers With Mental Retardation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1993;36:158–167. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3601.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Volkmar FR, Lord C, Bailey A, Schultz RT, Klin A. Autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(1):135–170. doi: 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00317.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES