Skip to main content
Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry logoLink to Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
. 2014 Sep 18;23(3):207–217.

Cognitive Rehabilitation for Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Promises and Problems

Diana Tajik-Parvinchi 1,, Leah Wright 1, Russell Schachar 1
PMCID: PMC4197521  PMID: 25320614

Abstract

Objective:

Cognitive training entails the repeated exercise of a specific cognitive process over a period of time to improve performance on the trained task as well as on tasks that were not specifically trained (transfer effect). Cognitive training shows promise in remediating deficits in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) – a disorder believed to stem from deficient cognitive processes – where the focus has been primarily on training working memory and attention. We discuss evidence from studies that have produced broad, limited, or no transfer effects with the goal of identifying factors that may be responsible for this heterogeneity.

Results:

There are several implicit assumptions that appear to drive researchers’ decisions regarding both the selection of cognitive abilities to train as well as the training tasks chosen to target those abilities. We identify these implicit assumptions and their weaknesses. We also draw attention to design limitations that may be contributing to lack of transfer.

Conclusion:

Although the overall pattern of findings from these studies is promising, the methodological and theoretical limitations associated with the literature limit conclusions about the efficacy of cognitive training as a rehabilitation method for ADHD. We hypothesize several suggestions that may improve training effects and summarize the evidence which led to our hypotheses.

Keywords: cognitive training, ADHD, children, rehabilitation

Background

In the past decade cognitive training has received considerable attention as an intervention method. This attention has been partly stimulated by the demand for non-pharmacological interventions for children with childhood onset disorders. Cognitive training entails the repeated exercise of a specific cognitive process (or multiple processes) over an extended period of time typically several weeks after which performance gains are expected on the trained task, but more importantly on untrained tasks and/or behavioural measures (transfer). Performance gains on tasks similar but not identical to the training task are defined as ‘near transfer’ (e.g. training memory and improving on an untrained task measuring memory) whereas performance gains on dissimilar tasks and/or behavioral measures are defined as ‘far transfer’ (e.g. training on a memory task and improving on a mathematical task). This transfer of benefits to other cognitive skills/behaviour is the distinguishing element of cognitive training. However, evidence supporting cognitive training as an intervention that can produce transfer is mixed. The aim of the present review is to summarize and evaluate this heterogeneous evidence with a focus on its application in children with ADHD.

ADHD is a common, persistent and impairing disorder distinguished by developmentally inappropriate restlessness, inattention and impulsiveness (DSM-5). It is characterized by academic, behavioral and emotional problems in childhood and by increased risk for motor vehicle accidents, antisocial behavior and school dropout in adolescence (Barkley et al., 2006; Raggi & Chronis, 2006; Reinhardt & Reinhardt, 2013; Küpper et al., 2012; Wilens, 2004). According to current theories, ADHD is a complex disorder with many genetic contributions, but also with contributory environmental risks (Vaidya & Stollstorff, 2008; Volkow et al., 2002). These underlying risks are not manifest in ADHD symptoms, but rather seem to disturb brain structure and function which in turn affects the higher order functions of the brain called executive functions (Crosbie, Pérusse, Barr, & Schachar, 2008). Executive functions manage other cognitive processes and are a collection of abilities that are related to one another but which are, to a great extent separable (Collette, Hogge, Salmon, & Van der Linden, 2006; Friedman et al., 2006). There are several reasons for the interest in cognitive training as an intervention for ADHD. First, cognitive training claims to directly address the cognitive deficits presumed to underlie ADHD (Castellanos, Sonuga-Barke, Milham, & Tannock, 2006). Second, preliminary evidence suggests that cognitive remediation might be at least partially effective in the treatment of ADHD (Johnstone et al., 2012; Shalev, Tsai, & Mevorach, 2007). And third, if effective, cognitive remediation would offer a non-drug alternative for a disorder that typically, and in very large numbers, involves the use of stimulant medication (Froehlich et al., 2007; MTA Cooperative Group, 1999).

Some studies that have used cognitive raining have reported broad transfer effects (Johnstone et al., 2012; Shalev et al., 2007; Van der Molen, Van Luit, Van der Molen, & Jongmans, 2010), others have reported limited transfer effects (Gibson et al., 2011), and others still have failed to generate any transfer at all (Ackerman, Beier, & Boyle, 2005; Chacko et al., 2014; Healy, Wohldmann, Sutton, & Bourne, 2006; Lee et al., 2012; Owen et al., 2010). At this time, we do not know why some studies succeed in producing transfer and some do not. In the present review, we employ two approaches to unravel and understand this heterogeneity. First, there are several implicit assumptions that appear to guide researchers’ decisions regarding which cognitive abilities to target and which training tasks to use. We identify these assumptions as guiding principles that form the foundation for the current state of the literature on cognitive training and explain their weaknesses. Second, we summarize the studies both within the pediatric ADHD population and those outside of this population that have not produced any transfer effects in an effort to decipher the factors that may be responsible for this lack of transfer effects.

Assumptions in Cognitive Training

Several implicit assumptions drive researchers’ decisions regarding which cognitive abilities to train and which training tasks should be used to exercise those abilities. For example, a particular cognitive process might be targeted because it is presumed to be a higher-order function that predicts or influences a range of other cognitive processes. The rationale is that improving that particular process would lead to improvements of all skills under its influence (i.e. broad transfers). For the purposes of the present paper, we will refer to this as the “higher-order assumption”. Although performance on one measure of executive function might be significantly correlated with performance on a task measuring another executive function, there is little known about the specific nature of this relationship. For example, we know little about whether change in one process would induce change in another. The second implicit assumption is that the targeted ability is a central deficit in a particular disorder such as ADHD. We will refer to this as the “central-deficit assumption”. Even though specific cognitive deficits are found regularly in ADHD, the correlation between performance on cognitive tasks and symptom severity is typically moderate at best (McAuley, Chen, Goos, Schachar, & Crosbie, 2010) and the nature of the relationship is unknown. The third assumption relates to the training task and presumes that the training task will target the selected ability of interest. This assumption will be denoted as the “task-purity assumption”. These assumptions will be discussed in the context of the two most commonly targeted cognitive processes in this literature, working memory (WM) and attention.

Implicit Assumptions and WM

There are various definitions of WM. It has been described as the ability to maintain task relevant information for easy access during a task (storage capacity only) (Goldman-Rakic, 1995); as storage capacity + the processing of that information (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Engle, 2001); and as storage capacity + information retrieval from secondary memory if information-maintenance fails (Gibson, Gondoli, Flies, Dobrzenski, & Unsworth, 2009). Working memory is often the target of cognitive training because of its assumed capacity to influence a range of cognitive processes (Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, & Shah, 2011; Olesen, Westerberg, & Klingberg, 2004) sometimes referred to as predictive capacity. Thus, how strongly performance on a particular task (e.g. WM) can predict performance on a different task (e.g. reading comprehension) captures predictive capacity of that task. Empirical evidence supporting this view has been reported within the context of storage + processing and storage + retrieval (Baddeley, 2003; Unsworth & Engle, 2007a), but not when WM is defined as storage capacity-only. This distinction is important since many cognitive training studies justify targeting WM using the higher-order assumption, but use training tasks that exercised short-term memory (STM). STM is defined as the ability to maintain information for a short period of time and does not appear to have a strong predictive capacity (Engle, Tuholski, Laughlin, & Conway, 1999; Conway, Cowan, Bunting, Therriault, & Minkoff, 2002).

Complex span tasks are often used to measure WM (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Turner & Engle, 1989; Unsworth & Engle, 2007a). For example, in operation span task, participants are instructed to answer a mathematical equation and to remember the word at the end of the equation, such as “Does 2+1=3? (yes or no) HOUSE”. After a set of 2–7 trials, they are asked to reproduce the words presented at the end of the equations in the correct serial order (memory component). Solving the equation (processing) prevents rehearsal strategies from maintaining the list of words in primary memory and increases the probability that the to-be-remembered words will dissipate from primary memory, in which case retrieval from secondary memory will be required (retrieval) to access the list of words from the secondary memory (Unsworth & Engle, 2007a). Using complex span tasks, studies have shown an association between WM and reading comprehension (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980), language acquisition (Baddeley, 2003), fluid intelligence (the ability to reason and problem solve in novel situations) (Conway et al., 2002; Kane et al., 2004; Unsworth & Engle, 2007b), vocabulary learning, note taking, and reasoning (Engle, 2001). This association between performance on complex spans and performance on other cognitive tasks is viewed as the predictive capacity of complex spans. As stated earlier, this evidence is often used as the rationale for training WM (the higher-order assumption). However, many studies which have aimed to train WM used simple span tasks to target it (Beck, Hanson, Puffenberger, Benninger, & Benninger, 2010b; Klingberg, Forssberg, & Westerberg, 2002).

Simple span tasks are considered to be a measure of STM (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Engle, 2001; Unsworth & Engle, 2007b). For example, in forward digit span, participants are presented with a list of digits one at a time and required to repeat the list of digits in the correct order. Simple spans do not have a strong capacity to predict performance on other cognitive tasks (Conway et al., 2002; Daneman & Merikle, 1996; Engle et al., 1999; Kail & Hall, 2001) and are easily influenced by scoring procedures, presentation modality, and trial lengths (Unsworth & Engle, 2007b). This is not always the case with backward span tasks. In backward span tasks the participant must reproduce the list in backward serial order. The backward span is sometimes considered to measure STM (Engle et al., 1999; Swanson, Mink, & Bocian, 1999) and sometimes WM (Ackerman et al., 2005). Overall, empirical evidence depicts simple span tasks as having limited predictive capacity.

The central-deficit assumption pertains to the view that a cognitive process, such as WM or attention, is the key deficit in ADHD (Beck, Hanson, Puffenberger, Benninger, & Benninger, 2010a; Klingberg et al., 2002) and training it would remediate a range of behavioural symptoms associated with ADHD. However, current theories suggest that ADHD is a complex disorder (Brown, 2006). Many executive functions seem to be perturbed in ADHD and each child can present with a distinct profile of deficits, some without working memory impairments (Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga-Barke, 2005; Qian, Shuai, Chan, Qian, & Wang, 2013). Presently, we do not have a clear knowledge of the hierarchical organization of cognitive processes implicated in ADHD. Furthermore, as stated earlier, the association between cognitive performance and symptom severity is not strong (Coghill, Hayward, Rhodes, Grimmer, & Matthews, 2013; McAuley et al., 2010). Thus, the view that remediating WM deficit would alleviate a range of symptoms in ADHD has not yet received careful examination.

Lastly, there is an assumption that a selected training task will target the abilitiy of interest. It is difficult to measure a specific cognitive process with laboratory tasks because of the task impurity problem (Burgess, 1997). Any given task of executive function will involve a range of abilities since executive functions by definition regulate other cognitive processes. Although it is not necessary to have a pure task for training purposes, it is important to know what a training task measures in order to correctly target and train the ability of interest. For example, if we intend to train WM, but use a task that predominately targets STM, then we will most likely not train WM. With these issues highlighted, the next section will summarize the findings in this field.

Training Working Memory

Many of the studies that have trained working memory have used the Cogmed Working Memory Training (2006) software (Table 1). Cogmed consists of verbal and visuospacial forward and backward simple span tasks. Studies which have used Cogmed (Table 1) have been criticized for design limitations that did not control for placebo effects. These limitations make it difficult to attribute the reported transfers to the training program unambiguously rather than to simple practice effects or the passage of time (Shipstead, Redick, & Engle, 2012b). Given that methodological weaknesses have been addressed in previous review papers (Shipstead et al., 2012b), we will only focus on theoretical weaknesses in our review.

Table 1.

A Review of cognitive training studies for children, adolescent, and young adults

Author Year Subject Group (age) Training method (n=?) Training Schedule (sessions) Sess Dur (min) Control Group (n=?) Results
Working Memory Response Inhibition Behaviour Planning/Organization/Reasoning Attention Scholastic Abilities Intelligence/Fluid Intelligence
Beck et al. 2010 ADHD (7–17) Cogmed (n=27) 25 30–40 Waitlist control (n=24) P-BRIEF CPRS DSM-IV P-BRIEF
T-BRIEF CTRS T-BRIEF
Chacko et. al 2014 ADHD (7–11) Cogmed (n=44) 25 30–45 Non-adaptive (n=41) AWMA: Dot Matrix; AWMA: Digit Recall, DBD A-X CPT WRAAT 4-PMV
AWMA: Spatial Recall
AWMA: Listening Recall
Gibson et al. 2011 ADHD (11–16) Cogmed (n=44) 20–25 No controls IFR Tasks DuPaul ADHD-RS (parent and teacher) DuPaul ADHD-RS (parent and teacher)
Gray et al. 2012 LD+ADHD (12–17) Cogmed (n=32) 20–25 45 Math Training (n=20) WISC-DSF SWAN D2 TA WRAT-4PM
WISC-DSB
SS
IOWA-CRS
CANTAB SWM
WMRS
Green et al. 2012 ADHD (7–14) Cogmed (n=12) 25 40 Non-adaptive (n=14) WISC-WMI RAST CPRS*
Mezzacappa & Buckner 2010 Healthy Cogmed (n=9) 25 40–45 WISC-DSB
WRAML-FW
ADHD-RS-IV
Dahlin et al. 2011 Special needs (9–12) Cogmed (n=42) 25 30–40 Controls from Klingberg et al. (2005) WISC-DSF WISC-DSB Stroop RC RAVEN
WAIS-SPF
WAIS-SBB
WD
OV
Olesen et al. (exp1) 2004 Healthy (20–23) WM (n=3) 20–30 35–45 No training control (n=11) WAIS-SB Stroop RAVEN
Olesen et al. (exp2) 2004 Healthy (27–31) Cogmed (n=8) 15–21 No training control (n=11) WAIS-DS WAIS-SB Stroop
Holmes et al. 2009 Healthy (8–11) Cogmed (n= 22) 20 35 Non-adaptive (n=20) AWMA WOND WORD WASI
Loosli et al. 2012 Healthy (9–11) WM training (n=20) 10 12 No training (n=40) SLT TONI
Alloway 2012 Learning difficulties (11–15) Jungle Memory (n=8) 24 30 Targeted edu support (n=7) AWMA WOND WORD WASI
Van der Molen et al. 2010 Learning disabilities Odd Yellow (n=41) Non-adaptive and a no WM training version (n=52) BDR LR SS Stroop AT SR RAVEN
Hoekzema et al. 2010 ADHD (8–14) WM, Attention, cognitive flexibility, planning, problem solving (n=9) 10 45 Social training (n=10) GNG SAT
Johnstone et al. 2010 ADHD (7–13) WM, RI (n=18) 25 20 Low intensity (n=18) GNG CPRS CBCL ADHD-symptoms checklist
Johnstone et al. 2012 ADHD (7–13) IC, WM (n=60) 25 15–20 Healthy (n=68) DS CS GNG CPRS BRS
Oddball Flanker
Rabiner et al. 2010 ADHD (6–7) Attention, reading, math (n=52) 28 75 Waitlist (n=25) CTRS WJ-III
APRS
Steiner et al. 2011 ADHD (11–14) Attention, WM (n=13) 24 45 Waitlist (n=15) CPRS BASC-P BRIEF-P CPT
CTRS, BASC-T, BRIEF-T
Kerns et al. 1999 ADHD (7–14) Attention (n=14) 16 30 Other edu. games Digit Span Coding MFFT ADDES Mazes (WISC-III) ACT UT Math Work-sheets
Stroop VOT(WISC-III) CCPT  
Shalev et al. 2007 ADHD (6–13) Attention (n=20) Other video games (n=16) PRS-Inattention Passage copying, reading
PRS-Hyperactivity Math
Kotwal et al. 1996 ADHD (13) Captain’s Log (n=1) Single-case study No statistical Tests: CPRS hyperactivity & impulsivity CTRS on-task behaviour Coding, Block Design, Vocabulary, Symbol-Search (from WISC_III)
CTRS
Rabiner et al. 2010 ADHD (6–7) Captain’s Log (n=25) 28 75 Wait-list (n=) CTRS inattention WJ-III
CTRS Hyperactive-Impulsive, Oppositional, Social Problems, Anxious/Shy APRS
Slate et al. 1998 ADHD+severly emotionally disturbed (7–11) Captain’s Log (n=4) 64 30 No Control No statistical Tests: CPRS (4/4 children) impulse control, hyperactivity TRF (3/4 children)–attention No statistical Tests: TMT–B (3/4 children) No statistical Tests: WRAT=3–math (3/4 children)
CBCL   No statistical Tests: PPVT-R (3/4 children) receptive vocabulary
CTRS    
No statistical Tests: Behavioral Point System (2/4 children)    

Inline graphic not examined;

Inline graphic = the cognitive process was examined but training related improvements were not found;

Inline graphic = the cognitive process was examined and training related improvements were reported either at post-training session or at the follow-up session subsequent to training; ACT = Attentional Capacity Test; ADHD-RS-IV = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Rating Scale-IV; APRS = Academic Performance Rating Scale; AT = Arithmetic Test; AWMA = Alloway Working Memory Assessment; A-X CPT = A-X Continuous Performance Test; BDR = Backwards Digit Recall; BRIEF = Behavioural Rating Inventory of Executive Functions; BRS = Behaviour Rating Scale (self-designed); CANTAB SWM = Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing Automated Battery Spatial Working Memory; CBCL = Child Behavior Check List; CCPT = Children’s Continuous Performance Task; CPRS = Conners Parent Rating Scale; CRS = Conners Rating Scales; Behaviour Assessment Scales for Children; CS = Counting Span; DBD = Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale; CTRS = Conners Teacher Rating Scale; D2 TA = D2 Test of Attention; DS = Digit Span; GNG = Go/No-Go; IC = Inhibitory Control; IFR = Immediate Free Recall; KB-IQ = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; IOWA-CRS = IOWA-Conners Rating Scale; LR = Listening Recall; MFFT = Matching Familiar Faces Test; PRS = Parent Rating Scales; RAST = Restricted Academic Situations Task; RAVEN = Raven’s Progressive Matrices; RD = Reading Comprehension; RT = Reading Test; SA = Selective Attention Task; SLT = Salzburger Lesetest; SR = Story Recall; SS = Spatial Span; SWAN = the Strengths and Weakness of ADHD-symptoms and Normal-behavior scale; TMT = Trail Making Test; TONI = Test of Nonverbal Intelligence; OV = Orthographic Verification; WAIS-DSF = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Digit Span Forwards; WAIS-DS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Digit Span; WAIS-DSB = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Digit Span Backwards; WISC-DSB = Wechsler Intelligence Scales-Digit Span Backwards; WISC-DSF = Wechsler Intelligence Scales-Digit Span Backwards; WAIS-SB = Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale-Span Board; WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale; WJ-III = Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Achievement; WD = Word Decoding; WISC-WMI = Wechsler Intelligence Scales-Working Memory Index; WISCIII VOT = WISC-III Hooper Visual Organization Test; WMRS = Working Memory Rating Scale; WOND = Wechsler Objective Number Dimensions; WORD = Wechsler Objective; WRAT-4PMV = Wide-Range Achievement Test-4-Progress Monitoring Version

Studies on Cogmed have reported transfer to tasks that measured the same ability (storage capacity) and some transfer to attention and organizational behaviour based on parent and teacher ratings. There are three well-controlled studies which have used Cogmed as a training program (Chacko et al., 2014; Gray et al., 2012; Green & King, 1998), two of which did not produce transfer effects and will be discussed in a later section. Green et al. (2012) was a well-controlled and double-blind study. They reported significant reductions in “off-task” behaviour (far transfer), albeit with a small effect size (Rapport, Orban, Kofler, & Friedman, 2013) and significant improvements on storage capacity (near transfer) in their training group. However, they did not find any significant differences in parent reports of problem behavior. In other words, they were not able to improve problem behaviour associated with ADHD. A meta-analytic review (Melby-Lervåg & Hulme, 2013) as well as a systematic review (Shipstead, Hicks, & Engle, 2012a) of studies which have used Cogmed training program and other WM training programs (Melby-Lervåg & Hulme, 2013) have revealed short-term near transfer effects only. These findings show limited transfer to other cognitive skills and limited effects on behavior, highlighting the weakness of higher-order and central-deficit assumptions. The limited transfer effects generated by Cogmed working memory training may stem from the failure of simple span tasks to target WM (task-purity assumption) and/or the limited capacity to influence other cognitive processes (predictive capacity). Empirical evidence appears to point to the former notion demonstrating that Cogmed targets short-term memory and not WM (Rapport et al., 2013).

Several studies have used complex span tasks to train WM in children and adolescents and have been able to produce broader transfer effects (Alloway & Alloway, 2008; Loosli, Buschkuehl, Perrig, & Jaeggi, 2012; Van der Molen et al., 2010). One commercially available program which includes complex span tasks is Jungle Memory designed by Alloway and Alloway (2008). Alloway (2012) used Jungle Memory to train WM in adolescents with learning difficulties and reported improvements in working memory capacity (near transfer), vocabulary (far transfer) and mathematical abilities (far transfer). Other researchers who have used complex span tasks to train WM have reported transfer effects to reading ability in healthy children (Loosli et al., 2012) and scholastic abilities in adolescents with mild to borderline intellectual disability (Van der Molen et al., 2010). These findings suggest broader transfers than those produced with simple span tasks corroborating the view that complex spans have greater predictive capacity (Daneman & Carpenter, 1980; Engle et al., 1999; Unsworth & Engle, 2007b) and are perhaps better suited to exercise WM and/or produce transfer effects. However, a meta-analysis of WM training programs which included Jungle memory showed that these training programs were only successful in producing short-term near transfer effects (Melby-Lervåg & Hulme, 2013). Thus further research is required to investigate the effectiveness of complex span tasks as training tasks before conclusive statements can be construed.

Training Attention

Attention is broadly defined as the ability to concentrate on a selected aspect of the environment. The type of attention targeted by cognitive training researchers is varied. Four attentional functions have been described, each associated with a separate neural correlates (Posner & Petersen, 1990; Tsal, Shalev, & Mevorach, 2005). These include orienting of attention (directing attention to a specific stimulus in the sensory environment), selective attention (selection of the relevant information from sensory input), sustained attention (the ability to sustain attention over time) and executive attention (the ability to divide or to alternate/shift attention between two tasks).

Studies which have trained attention appear to have based their selection on the central-deficit assumption with the approach that inattentiveness is the key impairment in ADHD and remediating it would alleviate cognitive and behavioral difficulties associated with ADHD. There are two commercially available programs (“Captain’s Log” and “Pay Attention”) that train various attentional networks in addition to other cognitive processes. Several studies have used Captain’s Log as a training program for children with ADHD and have reported improvements on the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) and transfer to parent/teacher rating scales (Table 1). Slate et al. (Slate, Meyer, Burns, & Montgomery, 1998) reported additional transfer effects to math and vocabulary but they did not run formal statistical tests, it is therefore difficult to consider their findings to be significant training-induced improvements.

“Pay Attention” is a paper and pencil program which targets sustained attention, selective attention and alternating/ divided attention. Kerns et al. (Kerns, Eso, & Thomson, 1999) used “Pay Attention” to train children with ADHD (7–14 years) and reported broad transfer effects (e.g. improved problem solving and mathematical ability). However, parent reports of inattention-impulsivity and hyperactivity were not significantly different from those described in the pre-training phase. In contrast, Shalev et al. (2007) used neuropsychological tasks to train sustained attention, selective attention, orienting attention and executive attention in children 6–13 years with ADHD and reported improvements in reading comprehension, passage copying and a reduction of parent reported inattentiveness. Overall, these findings demonstrate improvements on the targeted ability and limited transfer to other cognitive skills. Although these results are promising, transfer still remains narrow in most studies. This highlights the limitations of the central-deficit assumption.

Lack of Transfer

Studies which have not produced any transfer effects are more prevalent in adults than in children. This may be due to publication bias or it may be that cognitive training is more successful in children. The only published studies that have not shown any transfer effects in children are the ones that examined the efficacy of Cogmed. Chacko et al. (2014) examined effectiveness of Cogmed in children 7–11 years of age with ADHD. They found that the active training group improved on WM storage tasks but not on WM tasks measuring storage + processing/manipulation. Similarly, they did not observe transfer to any other measures assessing academic ability, attention, or parent/teacher rating scales (Table 1). Gray et al. (2012) used Cogmed to train adolescents 12–17 years of age with learning disability and ADHD. They reported gains on two tasks measuring WM but not on other WM tasks or any of their transfer tasks (for details see Table 1). These findings emphasize the limitations of the central and higher-order assumptions and the view that simple span tasks are not qualified to produce broad transfers. A series of work by Gibson et al. (Gibson et al., 2011; Gibson et al., 2013; Gibson et al., 2009) indicate that a modification of the adaptive algorithm of simple span tasks may increase their effectiveness as training tasks. Gibson and colleagues describe a dual-component model of WM where task relevant information is stored in the primary memory (PM); but upon loss of information from PM, attempts are made for the relevant information to be recalled from secondary memory (SM). Gibson et al. (2009) demonstrate that actively maintaining information in PM is not impaired in ADHD; whereas retrieval of task relevant information from SM is impaired. In their later work Gibson et al. (2011) reveal that Cogmed enhances the maintenance of information in PM, a component of WM which is not affected in ADHD. Gibson and colleagues suggest that the adaptive algorithm of Cogmed for advancing to the next difficulty level is designed in such way that the tasks currently tax the resources of the PM. They propose a modification to this algorithm to shift the demand to SM component of WM and confirm (Gibson et al., 2013) that by changing this adaptive algorithm, task demand does get shifted to the SM. Although it is plausible that such a modification would improve effectiveness of Cogmed training program or simple span tasks in general, this hypothesis must be examined and verified. In other words, it remains to be determined whether such an alteration would produce broader transfer effects in general or whether it would mitigate symptoms of ADHD more effectively.

The other studies that have not been successful in producing transfer are in adult samples. Lee et al. (2012) trained healthy young adults on a complex video-game (Space Fortress; Donchin, 1989). Space Fortress engages cognitive processes such as working memory, resource management and manual control. It requires players to navigate their ship and destroy a Space Fortress (for a full description of the game see Donchin, 1989). These investigators did not find transfer to untrained tasks measuring memory, attention, visual processing, motor control, reasoning ability and dual-tasking ability. Healy et al. (2006) had similar findings using a different training apparatus. They trained healthy young adults on a perceptual-motor task and aimed to improve inhibitory control. Their rational was that since in several of their conditions, participants had to inhibit prepotent responses (having a pre-existing probability of occurring) transfer should take place to conditions that included the same inhibitory demand. The investigators did not find transfer to other conditions.

One plausible explanation for lack of transfer in the above two studies may be that manual control and visuo-spatial skills can improve over time making it easier to perform the tasks and potentially decrease the cognitive load. When a given training task allows for task specific strategies to develop, it may lose its novelty and performance may become automatic (for further details see Morrison & Chein, 2011). Imaging findings support this view as well (Olesen et al., 2004). It is generally believed that an increase in neural activiation is observed at first when the participant is required to carry out novel tasks. However as training continues, performance on the task becomes more automated and does not require as many cognitive resources leading to a decrease in neural activation (see Buschkuehl et al., 2012 for a review). In veiw of these findings, it may be wise to minimize development of task-specific strategies and strive to maintain high cogntive loads when designing training tasks.

Owen et al. (2010) trained healthy adults 18–60 years of age for six weeks on an online brain training program. Participants were required to train for at least ten minutes a day, three times a week for 23–28 training sessions. They randomly assigned participants to two experimental groups and a control group. The training tasks consisted of different combinations of tasks targeting reasoning, planning, problem-solving, STM, attention, visuospatial processing and mathematics. The control group answered abstract questions from six different categories using online resources. Owen et al. found no evidence of transfer and no significant differences between their groups. Ackerman et al. (Ackerman, Kanfer, & Calderwood, 2010) trained healthy adults 50–71 years of age on the Nintendo Wii Brain Academy software and reading exercises. Ackerman et al. measured transfer to tasks measuring fluid intelligence (the ability to reason and problem solve in novel situations independent of previously acquired knowledge), crystallized intellegence (acquired knowledge), and perceptual speed (the time required to correctly identify a stimulus). They did did not find any training-related transfers.

One explanation for the lack of transfer in these two studies may be the wide age range of their samples (18–60 years and 50–70 years). It is possible that learning processes follow a different pattern in older adults. For example Ackerman et al. (2010) explained that the nature of learning in older adults may be narrower than younger people. Consistent with this position, Dahlin et al. (2008) was successful in producing transfer in younger adults (21–26 years), but failed to produce the same effect in older adults (66–70 years) using the same paradigm. These findings imply that the plastic capacity of the brain may decrease as the brain ages. This view however does not mean that cognitive training has no chance of producing transfer in older adults since other studies have been successful in producing transfer in older adults (Schmiedek, Lovden, & Lindenberger, 2010). Perhaps different training guidelines should be adopted when training older adults. Another explanation for the lack of transfer in the Owen et al. study may be the short training session of only ten minutes which encompassed exercising multiple cognitive processes within that span of time. It is plausible that each cognitive process was not sufficiently exercised in a single session to become significantly enhanced.

Discussion and Conclusion

The theoretical weaknesses associated with this field cluster around the implicit assumptions summarized earlier. These assumptions require consideration and discussion. Empirical evidence thus far does not appear to support the central-deficit and the higher-order assumptions. In fact, research in the neuroscience and mental health fields suggest a range of cognitive deficits associated with ADHD (Nigg, Blaskey, Stawicki, & Sachek, 2004; Nigg et al., 2005) and these cognitive deficits may be similar to those of other childhood onset disorders (Lipszyc & Schachar, 2010). Research is detaching itself from a central-deficit approach and shifting towards multiple-deficit models to capture the heterogeneity of ADHD (Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Castellanos et al., 2006; Nigg et al., 2005). However, for researchers/ clinicians interested in training WM, they may consider complex span tasks or use Gibson et al. (2013) algorithm for adaptation purposes in order to target WM and produce broader transfers.

The task-purity assumption will likely be an issue to tackle when selecting the training tasks and it becomes of greater concern when the training task is shown to measure different processes (e.g. STM vs WM). One approach to bypass this problem may be the same one preferred by many studies examining latent variables associated with a set of executive functions. A latent variable is an ability that is correlated with performance on a set of cognitive tasks. Perhaps in order to successfully target an ability of interest, a collection of tasks known to measure that ability should be selected to train it.

Taken as a whole, research in this field has demonstrated that cognitive processes which were once believed to be hard-wired are in fact trainable and modifiable. Future research aimed at identifying moderator factors that influence transfer effects as well the mechanism underlying transfer will be vital to this field. This line of research together with the objective of unraveling the plastic capacity of the human brain will have far-reaching implications.

Acknowledgments/Conflicts of Interest

Dr. Russell Schachar is a consultant to and has equity in a company that develops cognitive rehabilitation software. Research reported in this paper was supported by the Ontario Brain Institute (OBI-FedDev) and Behavioural Neurological and Applied Solutions (BNAS). The funders had no role in review, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  1. Ackerman PL, Beier ME, Boyle MO. Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131:30. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ackerman PL, Kanfer R, Calderwood C. Use it or lose it? Wii brain exercise practice and reading for domain knowledge. Psychology and aging. 2010;25:753. doi: 10.1037/a0019277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alloway T. Can interactive working memory training improving learning? Journal of Interactive Learning Research. 2012;23:197–207. [Google Scholar]
  4. Alloway T, Alloway R. Jungle memory training program. Edinburgh, United Kingdom: Memosyne; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baddeley A. Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2003;36:189–208. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00019-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baddeley A, Hitch G. Working memory. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1974. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. Young adult outcome of hyperactive children: Adaptive functioning in major life activities. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45(2):192–202. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000189134.97436.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beck SJ, Hanson CA, Puffenberger SS, Benninger KL, Benninger WB. A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2010a;39:825–836. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beck SJ, Hanson CA, Puffenberger SS, Benninger KL, Benninger WB. A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2010b;39:825–836. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown TE. Executive functions and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Implications of two conflicting views. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education. 2006;53:35–46. [Google Scholar]
  11. Burgess PW. Theory and methodology in executive function research. In: Rabbitt P, editor. Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function. 1997. pp. 81–116. [Google Scholar]
  12. Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM, Jonides J. Neuronal effects following working memory training. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2012;2:S167–S179. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Castellanos FX, Tannock R. Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3:617–628. doi: 10.1038/nrn896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chacko A, Bedard A, Marks D, Feirsen N, Uderman J, Chimiklis A, …Zwilling A. A randomized clinical trial of Cogmed Working Memory Training in school - age children with ADHD: A replication in a diverse sample using a control condition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;55(3):247–255. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Coghill D, Hayward D, Rhodes S, Grimmer C, Matthews K. A longitudinal examination of neuropsychological and clinical functioning in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Improvements in executive functioning do not explain clinical improvement. Psychological Medicine. 2014;14(5):1087–1099. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Collette F, Hogge M, Salmon E, Van Der Linden M. Exploration of the neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging. Neuroscience. 2006;139:209–221. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Conway AR, Cowan N, Bunting MF, Therriault DJ, Minkoff SR. A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence. 2002;30:163–183. [Google Scholar]
  19. Crosbie J, Pérusse D, Barr CL, Schachar RJ. Validating psychiatric endophenotypes: Inhibitory control and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:40–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dahlin E, Neely AS, Larsson A, Bäckman L, Nyberg L. Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science. 2008;320(5882):1510–1512. doi: 10.1126/science.1155466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Daneman M, Carpenter PA. Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1980;19:450–466. [Google Scholar]
  22. Daneman M, Merikle PM. Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 1996;3:422–433. doi: 10.3758/BF03214546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Donchin E. The learning strategies project: Introductory remarks. Acta Psychologica. 1989;71:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  24. Engle RW. What is working memory capacity? In: Roediger HL III, Nairne JS, editors. The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G Crowder. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2001. pp. 297–314. [Google Scholar]
  25. Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin JE, Conway AR. Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology General. 1999;128:309–331. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.128.3.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Friedman NP, Miyake A, Corley RP, Young SE, Defries JC, Hewitt JK. Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science. 2006;17:172–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN, Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Kahn RS. Prevalence, recognition, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a national sample of US children. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2007;161:857–864. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.9.857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gibson BS, Gondoli DM, Flies AC, Dobrzenski BA, Unsworth N. Application of the dual-component model of working memory to ADHD. Child Neuropsychology. 2009;16:60–79. doi: 10.1080/09297040903146958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gibson BS, Gondoli DM, Johnson AC, Steeger CM, Dobrzenski BA, Morrissey RA. Component analysis of verbal versus spatial working memory training in adolescents with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Child Neuropsychology : A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence. 2011;17:546–563. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.551186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gibson BS, Gondoli DM, Kronenberger WG, Johnson AC, Steeger CM, Morrissey RA. Exploration of an adaptive training regimen that can target the secondary memory component of working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition. 2013;41:726–737. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0295-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Goldman-Rakic P. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14:477–485. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gray S, Chaban P, Martinussen R, Goldberg R, Gotlieb H, Kronitz R, …Tannock R. Effects of a computerized working memory training program on working memory, attention, and academics in adolescents with severe LD and comorbid ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53:1277–1284. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02592.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Green CT, Long DL, Green D, Iosif AM, Dixon JF, Miller MR, Schweitzer JB. Will working memory training generalize to improve off-task behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9(3):639–648. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0124-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Green JF, King DJ. The effects of chlorpromazine and lorazepam on abnormal antisaccade and no-saccade distractibility. Biological Psychiatry. 1998;44:709–715. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00452-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Healy AF, Wohldmann EL, Sutton EM, Bourne LE., Jr Specificity effects in training and transfer of speeded responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2006;32:534. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Shah P. Short-and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:10081–10086. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103228108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Johnstone SJ, Roodenrys S, Blackman R, Johnston E, Loveday K, Mantz S, Barratt MF. Neurocognitive training for children with and without AD/HD. Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders. 2012;4:11–23. doi: 10.1007/s12402-011-0069-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kail R, Hall LK. Distinguishing short-term memory from working memory. Memory & Cognition. 2001;29:1–9. doi: 10.3758/bf03195735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, Engle RW. The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133:189. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kerns KA, Eso K, Thomson J. Investigation of a direct intervention for improving attention in young children with ADHD. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1999;16:273–295. [Google Scholar]
  41. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2002;24:781–791. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.6.781.8395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kotwal DB, Burns WJ, Montgomery DD. Computer-assisted cognitive training for ADHD a case study. Behavior Modification. 1996;20:85–96. [Google Scholar]
  43. Küpper T, Haavik J, Drexler H, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Wermelskirchen D, Prutz C, Schauble B. The negative impact of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder on occupational health in adults and adolescents. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health. 2012;85(8):837–847. doi: 10.1007/s00420-012-0794-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lee H, Boot WR, Basak C, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Neider M, …Gratton G. Performance gains from directed training do not transfer to untrained tasks. Acta Psychologica. 2012;139:146–158. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lipszyc J, Schachar R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2010;16:1064. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Loosli SV, Buschkuehl M, Perrig WJ, Jaeggi SM. Working memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology. 2012;18:62–78. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.575772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. McAuley T, Chen S, Goos L, Schachar R, Crosbie J. Is the behavior rating inventory of executive function more strongly associated with measures of impairment or executive function? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2010;16:495–505. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:270. doi: 10.1037/a0028228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Morrison AB, Chein JM. Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2011;18:46–60. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0034-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. MTA Cooperative Group A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Multimodal Treatment Study of Children with ADHD. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:1073–1086. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nigg JT, Blaskey LG, Stawicki JA, Sachek J. Evaluating the endophenotype model of ADHD neuropsychological deficit: Results for parents and siblings of children with ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:614–625. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nigg JT, Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1224–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:75–79. doi: 10.1038/nn1165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Owen AM, Hampshire A, Grahn JA, Stenton R, Dajani S, Burns AS, …Ballard CG. Putting brain training to the test. Nature. 2010;465:775–778. doi: 10.1038/nature09042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1990;13:25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Qian Y, Shuai L, Chan RC, Qian QJ, Wang Y. The developmental trajectories of executive function of children and adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Research In Developmental Disabilities. 2013;34:1434–1445. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rabiner DL, Murray DW, Skinner AT, Malone PS. A randomized trial of two promising computer-based interventions for students with attention difficulties. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2010;38:131–142. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9353-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Raggi VL, Chronis AM. Interventions to address the academic impairment of children and adolescents with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2006;9(2):85–111. doi: 10.1007/s10567-006-0006-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rapport MD, Orban SA, Kofler MJ, Friedman LM. Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review. 2013;33:1237–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Reinhardt MC, Reinhardt CA. Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder, comorbidities, and risk situations. Jornal de Pediatria (Versao en Portugues) 2013;89(2):124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jped.2013.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schmiedek F, Lovden M, Lindenberger U. Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: Findings from the COGITO Study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2010;2 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Shalev L, Tsal Y, Mevorach C. Computerized progressive attentional training (CPAT) program: Effective direct intervention for children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence. 2007;13:382–388. doi: 10.1080/09297040600770787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shipstead Z, Hicks KL, Engle RW. Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence support the claims? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 2012a;1:185–193. [Google Scholar]
  64. Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Engle RW. Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin. 2012b;138:628. doi: 10.1037/a0027473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Slate SE, Meyer TL, Burns WJ, Montgomery DD. Computerized cognitive training for severely emotionally disturbed children with ADHD. Behavior Modification. 1998;22:415–437. doi: 10.1177/01454455980223012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Swanson HL, Mink J, Bocian KM. Cognitive processing deficits in poor readers with symptoms of reading disabilities and ADHD: More alike than different? Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999;91:321. [Google Scholar]
  67. Tsal Y, Shalev L, Mevorach C. The diversity of attention deficits in ADHD the prevalence of four cognitive factors in ADHD versus controls. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2005;38:142–157. doi: 10.1177/00222194050380020401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Turner ML, Engle RW. Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language. 1989;28:127–154. [Google Scholar]
  69. Unsworth N, Engle RW. The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review. 2007a;114:104. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Unsworth N, Engle RW. On the division of short-term and working memory: An examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychological Bulletin. 2007b;133:1038–1066. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Vaidya CJ, Stollstorff M. Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Current status and working hypotheses. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2008;14:261–267. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Van der Molen MJ, Van Luit JE, Van der Molen MW, Jongmans MJ. Everyday memory and working memory in adolescents with mild intellectual disability. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2010;115:207–217. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.3.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M, Franceschi D, …Ding YS. “Nonhedonic” food motivation in humans involves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate amplifies this effect. Synapse. 2002;44:175–180. doi: 10.1002/syn.10075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wilens TE. Impact of ADHD and its treatment on substance abuse in adults. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004;65:38–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

RESOURCES