Abstract
Whereas fear memories are rapidly acquired and enduring over time, extinction memories are slow to form and are susceptible to disruption. Consequently, behavioral therapies that involve extinction learning (e.g., exposure therapy) often produce only temporary suppression of fear and anxiety. This review focuses on the factors that are known to influence the relapse of extinguished fear. Several phenomena associated with the return of fear after extinction are discussed, including renewal, spontaneous recovery, reacquisition, and reinstatement. Additionally, this review describes recent work, which has focused on the role of psychological stress in the relapse of extinguished fear. Recent developments in behavioral and pharmacological research are examined in light of treatment of pathological fear in humans.
Keywords: anxiety, exposure therapy, extinction, fear, PTSD, relapse, resilience, stress
Introduction
More than ever, research in animal models is playing a fundamental role in the development of novel therapies for anxiety. Brain imaging in humans and neuroanatomical studies in animals continue to reveal substantial overlap in the neurobiological systems underlying emotional memories, including conditioned fear memories (Delgado et al. 2008; Herry et al. 2008; Knapska et al. 2012; Kong et al. 2014; Lissek 2012; VanElzakker et al. 2013). Erasing fear memories without disrupting other memory systems remains a heavily coveted end point of behavioral interventions for anxiety disorders (Kindt et al. 2009; Maren 2011; Monfils et al. 2009; Quirk et al. 2010; Schiller et al. 2010). Indeed, behavioral therapies, such as prolonged exposure therapy, typically suppress rather than erase fear memories (Bouton 1988; Maren 2005). The form of learning thought to underlie these therapies—extinction learning—has been found to be rather labile (Bouton 2000; Hermans et al. 2006). As a consequence, fear can readily overpower extinction, and extinguished fear may return under a variety of conditions (Boschen et al. 2009; Bouton 2002; Ji and Maren 2007; Rachman 1979; Rachman 1989; Vervliet et al. 2012). Relapse of extinguished fear poses a considerable challenge to behavioral therapies for fear and anxiety disorders (Boschen et al. 2009; Kindt et al. 2009; Vervliet et al. 2012).
Over the last several decades, Pavlovian fear conditioning has become the gold standard for studying emotional learning and memory in the laboratory (Goswami et al. 2013; LeDoux 2000; Maren 2008; Mineka and Oehlberg 2008; Rasmusson and Charney 1997). Fear conditioning is observed in both humans and animals and is highly amenable to experimental control and investigation. Fear is highly adaptive; it is essential in motivating defensive behavior in the face of threat (Cantor 2009; Ellis 1982; Giske et al. 2013; Öhman and Mineka 2001; Seymour et al. 2004). However, increased conditioned fear is observed in individuals with anxiety disorders, and fear circuits in the brain are thought to mediate and modulate anxiety (Davis 1992; Fanselow and Gale 2003; LeDoux 2012; Zantvoord et al. 2013). In both animals and humans, conditioned fear results from the repeated pairing of a neutral, yet detectable, conditioned stimulus (CS) with an aversive, biologically significant unconditioned stimulus (US) (Delgado et al. 2006; Gunther et al. 1997; Maren 2001). In rodent models, stimuli such as brief tones or lights often serve as CSs, and mild to moderate footshocks most commonly serve as USs. Although footshocks themselves produce an unconditioned response (i.e., a circa-strike “activity burst,” including vocalizations; Fanselow 1994), they ultimately engender a conditioned fear state that is associated with a host of fear responses, including freezing (i.e., immobility). In rodents, freezing is a defensive response to an inescapable threat (Bolles 1970; Fanselow 1994; Nissen 1946; Riess 1945) and is a highly reliable index of conditioned fear to the CS (or context; see below). Conditioned fear responses (CRs) also consist of changes in autonomic reactivity, including the release of stress hormones and endogenous opioids (Antov et al. 2013; Davis 1979; Fanselow and Bolles 1979; Fanselow et al. 1989; Kull et al. 2012; Merz et al. 2013a; Przewłocka 1990; Soeter and Kindt 2011). As will be discussed later, pharmacological interventions for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and anxiety often target autonomic responding (Bailey et al. 2013; Cain et al. 2012). Fear acquisition in rodents is remarkably similar to that of the general human population (Galatzer-Levy et al. 2013), allowing Pavlovian fear conditioning to serve as a translational model (Milad and Quirk 2012; VanElzakker et al. 2013).
During fear conditioning, animals encode not only an association between the CS and US but also an association between the context in which they occur and the US (i.e., identified as context fear) (Fanselow 1980; Maren et al. 2013). Importantly, conditioned fear to the CS generalizes across contexts, unlike that of extinction (see below). Contexts include the physical environment surrounding the animal (exteroceptive contexts), as well as the animal's internal states of being (interoceptive contexts) (Maren et al. 2013). In rodent models, exteroceptive contexts are created with distinct cage odors, changes in the texture of the testing platform, and alterations in background lighting and noise. Interoceptive contexts are inherently subjective to the animal and may consist of unconscious components. Interoceptive contexts include (but are not limited to) states of arousal, drug states, states of deprivation, and temporal states (Bouton 1993; Bouton 2002; Bouton et al. 1990; Cunningham 1979; Davidson 1993; Järbe et al. 1981; Richardson et al. 1986; Servatius and Beck 2005). Context fear is elicited by the exteroceptive context in which the aversive US occurs and is particularly strong with unsignaled USs (i.e., contextual fear conditioning) (Fanselow and Bolles 1979; Waddell et al. 2006).
In contrast to conditioning, extinction is a procedure in which the contingency between the CS and US is degraded by presenting the CS alone many times without the aversive footshock. Context fear associated with the US can also be extinguished by placing the subject in the conditioned context in the absence of any aversive US (refer to Maren et al. 2013). As a result, animals learn that the CS (or context) no longer predicts the aversive US (Bouton 2004; Chang et al. 2009; Hermans et al. 2006; Lolordo and Rescorla 1966; Pavlov 1927), thereby reducing conditioned fear. Extinction has been shown to engage distinct neural circuits that act on and interact with the neural circuits involved in conditioning (Courtin et al. 2014; Herry et al. 2010; Maren 2011; Milad et al. 2006b; Myers and Davis 2002; Orsini et al. 2013). Interestingly, these neural circuits are active during the suppression of fear in humans (Delamater and Westbrook 2014; Milad and Quirk 2012; Milad et al. 2006b). As such, fear extinction in rodents has been argued to model exposure therapy in humans (Bouton 1988; Hofmann 2007; Milad and Quirk 2012). Exposure therapy is used to treat a variety of anxiety disorders, including PTSD (Cahill et al. 2006; Foa 2011; McLean and Foa 2011; Motraghi et al. 2014; Powers et al. 2010; Rauch et al. 2012; Rothbaum and Swartz 2002). Certain cues are thought to be more readily associated with the aversive US, and subsequently, these cues may be resistant to extinction under certain conditions (though this has been met with controversy; Mineka and Öhman 2002; Lueken et al. 2011). Overall, fear-relevant cues in humans (e.g., a picture of a spider or snake) have been shown to be difficult to extinguish (McNally 1986; Öhman et al. 1975a; Öhman et al. 1975b).
Although extinction-based therapies such as exposure therapy are effective at suppressing fear, the long-term efficacy of these treatments is challenged by the propensity of extinguished fear to relapse (Bouton 1988; Rachman 1979; Rachman 1989; Rodriguez et al. 1999; Vervliet et al. 2012). Understanding the nature and causes of fear relapse is essential to developing effective therapeutic interventions in patients with anxiety disorders. In this review, we will focus on the behavioral mechanisms involved in relapse of extinguished fear, taking care to translate work in animal models to humans. Additionally, this review will highlight strategies that are known to enhance the retention of extinction.
Extinction Retention: A Vulnerable Process
Pavlov (1927) was the first to note that extinction procedures produce only a temporary loss of conditioned responding. For example, he observed that presenting a novel stimulus after extinction caused a reemergence of the CR (external disinhibition); moreover, the mere passage of time after extinction resulted in a return of the CR (spontaneous recovery). In the context of aversive conditioning, these and other phenomena indicate that extinction procedures do not erase fear memories; rather, they lead to a new memory that inhibits the representation of the US (thereby reducing conditioned responding) (Bouton 1993; Konorski 1967; Maren 2011; Quirk 2002). The reemergence of fear is clearly inimical to the aims of therapy and is unpleasant for the patient (Vervliet et al. 2012; Vervliet et al. 2013); unrelenting and unmanaged anxiety is certainly not without health risks (Baganz and Blakely 2013; Hou and Baldwin 2012; Kemp and Quintana 2013). In the following sections, we review four fundamental fear relapse phenomena: renewal, spontaneous recovery, reacquisition, and reinstatement. Although each phenomenon is discussed in light of animal research, these forms of fear relapse have also been identified in humans (Hermans et al. 2005; Vervliet et al. 2012; Vervliet et al. 2013). In later sections, we will examine how particular stressors may modulate extinguished fear and how stress factors may relate to relapse of fear in general.
Renewal
A fundamental observation concerning extinction is that it is context-specific (Bouton and Bolles 1979; Bouton and Nelson 1994). That is, when an extinguished CS is encountered outside of the extinction context, renewal of conditional responding occurs (Bouton 2004; Bouton and King 1983; Bouton and Ricker 1994; Neumann and Longbottom 2008; Polack et al. 2013; Vervliet et al. 2013). Renewal of fear to an extinguished CS can occur when the CS is presented outside either the exteroceptive or interoceptive context of extinction (Maren et al. 2013; Maren 2014). Renewal of fear is ordinarily strongest when the extinguished CS is presented back in the conditioning context (extinction and conditioning often occur in separate contexts; Maren 2014). Hence, although fear memories readily generalize across contexts, extinction learning is characteristically limited to the context in which the extinction memory was formed (Bouton 2004; Maren 2014; Rosas et al. 2013). Thus, renewal of fear is a major challenge for clinicians. Suppression of fear in a therapist's office may not readily translate to other environments in the patient's life (Mystkowski et al. 2002; Rodriguez et al. 1999). Further compounding the issue, renewal is not limited to a single change in context; renewal of fear can occur across multiple extinction sessions in several distinct contexts (Bouton et al. 2006). Renewal of fear can also occur when the animal experiences the CS in a novel environment (Neumann and Kitlertsirivatana 2010; Maren 2014) or in a familiar environment where the animal has never experienced the CS (Polack et al. 2013). In both cases, renewal appears to be mediated by an unexpected occurrence of the CS in any given context. Renewal of fear has received considerable attention over the last decade, and several important brain structures have been identified in the regulation of renewal (Lissek et al. 2013; Maren 2011; Maren et al. 2013; Maren 2014; Zelikowsky et al. 2013a). Given the importance of contextual information in fear responding, renewal is thought to interact with other known forms of fear relapse.
Spontaneous Recovery
An extinguished response to a CS also returns merely with the passage of time, a phenomenon termed spontaneous recovery (Bouton 1993, Rescorla 1997; Leung and Westbrook 2010; Rescorla 2004; Pavlov 1927). In this case, a change in temporal context (i.e., a form of interoceptive context) has been suggested to account for the return of fear (Bouton 1993). By this view, recent events may be more strongly associated with one another than with temporally distant ones (Bouton 1988; Bouton 1993; Rescorla 2004), suggesting that renewal processes may interact with spontaneous recovery (Bouton 2002; Bouton 2004). Indeed, presentation of a reminder cue of the extinction context prior to testing attenuates relapse of fear in animal models of renewal or spontaneous recovery (Brooks and Bouton 1993; Brooks and Bouton 1994). Clearly spontaneous recovery is a major obstacle in treatment of pathological fear, as routine and ongoing therapeutic interventions may not be practical or feasible for the patient. If implemented with respect to time, other strategies that are known to reduce or prevent renewal may be useful in curbing spontaneous recovery.
Reacquisition
Another phenomenon that restores conditioned responding is administering additional conditioning trials after extinction (Bouton 2002; Kehoe and Macrae 1997; Napier et al. 1992; Rescorla 2001), a phenomenon known as reacquisition. After extinction, pairing the CS and US once again rapidly restores conditional responding under the majority of circumstances (Bouton 2002). However, in some cases, the reacquisition of fear to an extinguished CS is slow (Bouton 1986). This is particularly true when reacquisition trials occur in a unique extinction context. For this reason, Bouton (2002) has argued that rate of reacquisition may depend upon the presence of contextual cues associated with conditioning or extinction, noting that when cues for extinction are removed (and replaced with cues for the conditioning context), reacquisition of CR is far more rapid. Thus, similar to spontaneous recovery, reacquisition interacts with contextual information and also reflects the context-dependence of extinction memories. Interestingly, intermittent CS-US pairings alongside CS-alone presentations can, if implemented correctly, deepen extinction and weaken the possibility of reacquisition of fear in the wake of the extinction procedure (Bouton 2002; also see Ricker and Bouton 1996). Through this design, postextinction presentation of a CS-US pairing may call on memories of extinction, rather than of conditioning, and thereby may result in a weakening of reacquisition. Even with this paradigm, however, some reacquisition of fear is likely to occur (Bouton 2002).
Reinstatement
Encountering the US in absence of the CS after extinction has been shown to reinstate fear responding to the CS (Bouton and Bolles 1979; Rescorla and Heth 1975; Westbrook et al. 2002). Reinstatement can occur with either a strong or weak US (i.e., a footshock of a smaller amplitude than that utilized in conditioning). Additionally, the reinstating US is often unsignaled (unlike reacquisition), but reinstatement can occur with a signaled US (i.e., presentation of a cue other than the CS with the US; Bouton and Bolles 1979). According to one view, presentation of the US might reinstate fear by serving as a retrieval cue for the conditioning memory. Alternatively, context-US associations might summate with fear to the CS to promote conditional responding (Bouton 1993; Bouton 2002; Bouton and King 1983). Evidence that reinstatement occurs only in the context in which unsignaled USs are delivered is consistent with this view (Bouton 1984; Bouton 1988; Bouton 1993; Bouton and Bolles 1979; Bouton and King 1983). However, Westbrook and colleagues (2002) argue that reinstatement is not always context specific. In this study, Westbrook and colleagues (2002) extinguished two distinct CSs (termed CS1 and CS2) in two separate contexts (conditioning occurred in context A, CS1 was extinguished in context B, and CS2 was extinguished in context C; letters correspond to unique contexts). Later, rats received a footshock (US) reminder in context B (but not C) and were subsequently tested for retention of extinction in a separate neutral context (context D). Rats exhibited more fear to CS1 than to CS2 when tested in D, suggesting that reinstatement is not context specific. This stimulus-specific reinstatement of fear across contexts appears to be independent of renewal of fear (Westbrook et al. 2002; see also Holland 1990). The notion that reinstatement can be observed outside of the context in which the US reminder is presented will become important for other reinstatement studies described in this manuscript (e.g., Morris et al. 2005a; Morris et al. 2005b).
In some respects, reinstatement may be determined by the mere aversiveness of the US, such that the fear state induced by an unsignaled US reminds the animal of the state of fear at the time of conditioning, thereby facilitating fear responding. As such, other aversive fear-inducing stimuli might yield reinstatement to the CS. Indeed, the concept of reinstatement in recent years has grown to include other “aversive triggers” of fear responding to the CS. For example, presentation of an unextinguished CS (which induces fear) reinstates fear to a different, extinguished CS (Halladay et al. 2012). Moreover, recent exposure to a conditioned context (i.e., a “dangerous” context) has been shown to reinstate fear to an extinguished CS in a separate context (Morris et al. 2005a; Morris et al. 2005b). Low levels of context fear prior to CS presentation in the testing context do not negate this effect. This work by Morris and colleagues (2005a) suggests, in contrast to Bouton (2002), that reinstatement can occur in a context different from the context in which fear was induced (e.g., via exposure to separate dangerous context) (also refer to Westbrook et al. 2002). Conceivably, the mechanisms of these two forms of reinstatement are different insofar as US-induced reinstatement appears to be context dependent, whereas the reinstatement that follows fear induction is not. That said, both footshocks and dangerous contexts are stressful, and stress may therefore play a role in reinstatement of fear (Jacobs and Nadel 1985). Stress clearly confers susceptibility to anxiety disorders (Callaghan et al. 2013; Cohen et al. 2013; Green et al. 2011; Timmermans et al. 2013), though less is known about the role of stress in relapse. Moreover, stress systems in the brain are known to overlap and interact with fear circuitry (Asan et al. 2013; Tye et al. 2011). As such, examining the role that stress might play in the relapse of fear is of fundamental importance in understanding and preventing relapse.
Stress and Fear Relapse
Stress is precipitated by a variety of stimuli, can exert a multitude of physiological effects, and may exhibit both facilitatory and inhibitory effects on fear memory depending on the nature of the stressor and the task at hand (Akirav and Maroun 2013; Baratta et al. 2007; Kim and Diamond 2002; Roth et al. 2012; Sanders et al. 2010; Sapolsky 2003; Trammell and Clore 2013; Wideman et al. 2013). Stress can even alter the memory systems involved in solving the task (Goodman et al. 2012; Packard and Goodman 2012). Moreover, the extent to which an animal has control over the stressor ultimately affects the consequence of stress on behavioral performance (Baratta et al. 2007; Christianson et al. 2013; Kubala et al. 2012; Maier et al. 2006). Recently, Hartley and colleagues (2013) demonstrated in humans that inescapable stress impairs extinction, while controllable stress actually reduces spontaneous recovery. As such, understanding the consequences of stress on fear relapse requires an appreciation of the type and time-course of the stress (i.e., physical vs. psychological, acute vs. chronic vs. intermittent, controllable vs. uncontrollable, etc.), as well as its physiological effect on the animal.
Physical stressors are commonly used in rodents (Heinrichs and Koob 2006) and include footshock, restraint stress, forced swim, nutrient deprivation, and loud noise. Certainly, physical stressors are not without psychological implications; physical stressors are merely differentiated from psychological stressors based on the source of the stress. In contrast, psychological stressors do not place physical strain on the animal's body and include predator odor exposure, social isolation, prolonged elevation, dangerous context exposure, and distress vocalizations (Deschaux et al. 2012; Fleshner et al. 2004; Hu et al. 2014; McNeal et al. 2014; Morris et al. 2005a; Wallace and Rosen 2000). Psychological stressors may also include exposure to novel stimuli and contexts, as well as associative unexpectancy (i.e., an unexpected occurrence of an extinguished CS in a familiar context; Maren 2014). Introduction or elimination of conspecifics can yield psychosocial stress (Huhman 2006). Psychosocial stressors include social defeat, chronic subordinate colony housing, and maternal deprivation (Fraga et al. 2014; Papciak et al. 2013; Uschold-Schmidt et al. 2013). Although all of the aforementioned stressors are specific to rodent models, analogs of these stressors are implemented in human research, particularly psychological and psychosocial stressors (Björkqvist 2001; Campbell and Ehlert 2012; Hartley et al. 2013; Maner et al. 2008; Schultheiss et al. 2005). Worth noting is that stress responding can also be induced pharmacologically or via electrical stimulation of anxiogenic regions of the brain (Kellet and Kokkinidis 2004; Morris et al. 2005b).
Psychological stressors in rodents have garnered considerable attention in recent years, driven in part by the important role played by psychological stress in the psychopathology of human anxiety disorders. Several studies now highlight the potential risk of fear relapse in the wake of psychological stress. Deschaux and colleagues (2012) demonstrated a return of fear to an extinguished CS after a 30-minute exposure of rats to an elevated platform. The relapse of fear in these rats was blocked by chronic administration of the antidepressant, fluoxetine (administration of fluoxetine occurred over 20 consecutive days; refer to Deschaux et al. 2012). As discussed earlier, Morris and colleagues (2005a) induced relapse of extinguished fear in rats simply by briefly exposing the animals to a dangerous context. This effect was blocked with the beta-adrenergic antagonist propranolol (Morris et al. 2005b). Additionally, Morris and colleagues (2005b) demonstrated that artificial induction of adrenergic activity with acute systemic administration of epinephrine replicated the effects of exposure to the dangerous context. Thus, reinstatement of fear to an extinguished CS may be related to the stress engendered by other unsignaled footshocks or fear in general (Halladay et al. 2012; McCarty and Kopin 1978; Morris et al. 2005b). By this view, any aversive experience might result in the reinstatement of extinguished fear. Whether stress-induced relapse and reinstatement are mediated by overlapping neural structures has yet to be fully explored. If there is overlap, behavioral and pharmacological strategies that curb reinstatement may also attenuate stress-induced relapse. Likewise, stress reduction techniques may be key to reducing vulnerabilities to fear relapse. The effect of physical stress on fear responding in the aftermath of extinction has gone largely unexplored, though a similar pattern is expected as with psychological stressors. Nonassociative mechanisms that accompany stress should also be explored with regards to relapse.
Stress prior to conditioning has been shown to facilitate fear learning in rodents and to make fear responding more resistant to attenuation under certain conditions (Corley et al. 2012; Long and Fanselow 2012; Maren and Chang 2006; Rau et al. 2005; Rau and Fanselow 2009; Rodrigues et al. 2009). Additionally, stress prior to extinction (but after conditioning) can also impair the acquisition of extinction (Adamec et al. 2006; Maren and Chang 2006; Maren 2013). In rodents, the experience of aversive stress prior to conditioning can impair the retention of extinction memories in retrieval tests. For example, single prolonged stress prior to conditioning has been shown to enhance the renewal of fear in extinguished rats (Knox et al. 2012). Additionally, extinguished context fear is poorly retained in rats that have undergone a single prolonged stress procedure prior to the context conditioning procedure (Yamamoto et al. 2008). Similarly, Goswami and colleagues (2010) showed that exposure to predator threat prior to fear conditioning weakened the acquisition and recall of extinction in a cohort of Lewis rats (described as “PTSD-like”). These PTSD-like rats were shown to exhibit low levels of exploratory behavior on an elevated plus maze. Interestingly, Goswami and colleagues (2010) demonstrated that this impairment in extinction retention was not observed in rats that had previously exhibited high levels of exploratory behavior on the elevated plus maze (i.e., “resilient” rats). In humans, a similar pattern exists in both the facilitation of conditioning and the weakening of extinction through stress (Milad et al. 2008; Peri et al. 2000; Robinson et al. 2013; VanElzakker et al. 2013).
Early life stressors long before fear conditioning may also contribute to the susceptibility of fear relapse. Acute maternal deprivation has been shown to foster a propensity for relapse of fear in young rat pups (Cowan et al. 2013). Interestingly, infant pups under standard rearing conditions exhibit a degree of resistance to relapse; fear fails to reinstate or renew in these rats (but see Revillo et al. 2013). However, the lifelong implications of acute maternal deprivation on fear responding remain unclear. That said, prenatal stress (i.e., stress in pregnant rats) has even been found to reduce the retention of extinction of offspring trained later in life (Green et al. 2011).
Individual Differences and Susceptibility to Relapse
Anxiety disorders exist throughout the world, but certain groups are preferentially affected. For example, women have an increased risk of clinical anxiety and PTSD compared with men (Foa and Street 2001; Kobayashi et al. 2012). Sex differences are observed in animal models of fear conditioning and extinction, as well as in stress responding (Farrell et al. 2013; Gupta et al. 2001; Lebron-Milad et al. 2013; Lynch et al. 2013; Maren et al. 1994; Merz et al. 2013a; Merz et al. 2013b; Milad et al. 2009). Humans exhibit a similar pattern, albeit with fewer consistencies across studies (Milad et al. 2006a). Nevertheless, it stands to reason that susceptibility of relapse may differ between sexes. Indeed, female rats are more likely to exhibit renewal of fear (Baker-Andresen et al. 2013). Chronic stress prior to conditioning preferentially impairs the recall of extinction memories in male rats, but not female rats; however, unstressed female rats appear to not extinguish as robustly as males (Baran et al. 2009).
The developmental stage of the animal can also have a profound impact on the nature of fear acquisition and expression of extinguished fear (Callaghan and Richardson 2013; Campbell and Ampuero 1985; Kim and Richardson 2007a; Kim and Richardson 2007b; Kim and Richardson 2010; Mactutus et al. 1982; Sanders 2011). Stress responding can vary widely across development in both humans and animals, suggesting that the occurrence of relapse may interact with developmental stages (Green and McCormick 2013; Revillo et al. 2013; Takahashi et al. 1991; Wright et al. 2012). For example, postweanling rats as young as a few weeks old are capable of renewal, spontaneous recovery, and reinstatement, whereas preweanling rats are known to exhibit resistance to relapse of conditioned fear (Kim and Richardson 2010; but see Revillo et al. 2013). Adolescent rats are particularly susceptible to fear relapse when compared with preadolescent and adult rats (Baker et al. 2013). To combat this susceptibility, Baker and colleagues (2013) have shown that a CS-alone presentation (i.e., a retrieval trial) prior to or following extinction training reduced the risk of subsequent renewal in the adolescent rats. Interestingly, Sanders (2011) demonstrated a deficit in renewal of extinguished fear in aged mice (17 months). This impairment in renewal may be related to the weakening of contextual gating systems in the aging mouse brain.
These studies suggest that strategies of relapse prevention should be targeted to susceptible groups accordingly. Drug treatments in humans must certainly be mindful of developmental stages in children and adolescents (Huemer et al. 2010). Furthermore, future work should ascertain how sex differences interact with developmental stages across the lifespan. Several genes are associated with an increased risk for PTSD and other anxiety disorders (Almli et al. 2014; El-Kordi et al. 2013; Erhardt and Spoormaker 2013; Felmingham et al. 2013; Norrholm et al. 2013; Wilker et al. 2013), and these genes may also predispose individuals to relapse. In addition, epigenetic mechanisms of stress and psychopathology have received considerable attention in recent years (Maddox et al. 2013; Norrholm et al. 2013; Zovkic et al. 2013), and these factors might also confer individual differences in fear extinction and relapse. Indeed, some individuals extinguish fear rapidly and exhibit resilience in the face of stress (Franklin et al. 2012; Jovanovic and Ressler 2010; Galatzer-Levy et al. 2013). Insight into what contributes to resiliency, both in terms of behavior and brain function, may offer a means to reduce susceptibility of fear relapse in others.
Strategies to Prevent Relapse
To this point, we have discussed factors that influence the return of extinguished fear. We will now describe classic and contemporary strategies that may combat relapse of fear.
Optimizing Behavioral Therapy
Several studies indicate that the magnitude and duration of fear reduction after extinction relates to the amount of extinction training (Cain et al. 2003; Denniston et al. 2003; Orinstein et al. 2010). Extinction learning is slowly acquired, and limited training leads to rapid fear relapse. Thus, animals undergoing massed extinction trials often experience several hundred consecutive CS-alone presentations (Laborda and Miller 2013; Urcelay et al. 2009). As is often the case in procedures for massed extinction, the intertrial interval may be of short duration, which may in actuality weaken the extinction memory (Li and Westbrook 2008). Spaced trials may offer greater protection from fear relapse than with massed trials (Li and Westbrook 2008; Urcelay et al. 2009). As mentioned earlier, spaced trial procedures across multiple points in time may buffer against spontaneous recovery by supporting multiple temporal contexts with which extinction training is associated (Bouton 2002; Tsao and Craske 2000; Urcelay et al. 2009). The greater efficacy of spaced versus massed extinction trials continues to be explored (Li and Westbrook 2008; also see Fitzgerald et al. 2013).
Extinction in multiple contexts has been argued to facilitate suppression of fear in future encounters with the CS (Balooch et al. 2013; Chelonis et al. 1999; Gunther et al. 1998). This strategy has been argued to be particularly effective at reducing renewal. This has come with mixed results, however (Fitzgerald et al. 2013). Recent experiments suggest that extinction in multiple contexts is not always successful in preventing renewal (Bouton et al. 2006). Pairing massed extinction with extinction in multiple contexts may help foster greater fear suppression than with either strategy alone (Laborda and Miller 2013; Vervliet et al. 2013). Interestingly, by extinguishing renewed fear as a result of an extinguished CS presentation in a novel context, rats exhibit resistance to subsequent renewal of fear when the CS is presented in the conditioning context (Holmes and Westbrook 2013). Although contexts are distinct for experimental animals, several features and cues in the contexts are often shared among different testing chambers and contexts. During times of high risk of relapse, presentation of retrieval (reminder) cues of the extinction experience may foster resilience against renewal and spontaneous recovery or against relapse in general (Bouton 2000; Brooks and Bouton 1993; Brooks and Bouton 1994; Culver et al. 2011; Dibbets et al. 2008; Dibbets et al. 2013). As a preventative measure, it has been proposed that having patients consciously identify similarities between the extinction context and other environments may prove helpful in preventing relapse (Bouton 2002).
Enriched environments are known to offer protection against the negative effects of stress (Baldini et al. 2013; Mitra and Sapolsky 2009). In turn, supportive and enriched environments may offer protection against stress-induced relapse. Additionally, voluntary exercise has been shown to foster resilience in animals (Fox et al. 2008; Salem et al. 2009; but see Hare et al. 2012). In rodents, voluntary exercise most often comes in the form of wheel running. With more time spent on an exercise wheel, uncontrollable stress exerts less of a negative impact on the animal (Greenwood et al. 2005). Thus, voluntary exercise may help buffer stress-induced fear relapse.
Time of day effects can also play a role in the acquisition and retention of extinction. Recent work in humans highlighted a deepening of extinction simply through training subjects in the morning as opposed to evening sessions (Pace-Schott et al. 2013). In rodent behavioral work, experimenters should be mindful of renewal effects that may occur as a result of irregularity in the time of day at testing. Although patients may be reluctant to entertain the idea, habituation to the US has also been argued to potentially reduce the risk of fear relapse to CS (Rauhut et al. 2001). Other work in humans suggests that merely observing others undergoing extinction can provide some protective effects (Golkar et al. 2013). Interestingly, postexposure sleep appears to enhance long-term fear reduction (Kleim et al. 2013). In another sleep study, Wixted (2013) described a means by which fear can be extinguished during sleep by exposing subjects to odor cues during sleep. In combination with other extinction strategies, these and other novel procedures may offer greater protection against relapse; however, that remains to be demonstrated.
Behavioral interventions for PTSD in humans are often implemented immediately following trauma (Agorastos et al. 2011; Kearns et al. 2012; Roberts et al. 2009). In recent years, the efficacy of immediate extinction procedures has come under question. In some instances, immediate extinction has been found to be robust and effective in preventing relapse (Myers et al. 2006); there's even some evidence that these procedures are capable of erasing the original fear memory (Maren 2011; Monfils et al. 2009). On the other hand, immediate extinction after conditioning is often ineffective in long-term suppression of fear in rodents (Archbold et al. 2010; Chang and Maren 2009; Maren 2011; Maren 2013; Maren and Chang 2006). In general, delayed extinction has been shown to be far more effective in humans in preventing relapse in renewal and spontaneous recovery paradigms (Huff et al. 2009). Similarly, Archbold and colleagues (2013) found spontaneous recovery in rats to be more pronounced shortly after extinction training (i.e., between 1 and 4 hours postextinction) when compared with later time points (i.e., 8 to 24 hours postextinction). Long-term consolidation of the extinction memories may account for the reduction in spontaneous recovery at the later time points (Archbold et al. 2013). Distributing extinction training across a wide array of time points may deepen extinction (Gershman et al. 2013), and this notion is supported by clinical work in humans (Rowe and Craske 1998; Tsao and Craske 2000).
Pharmacotherapeutic Interventions
Pharmacological therapies often coincide with behavioral interventions of anxiety disorders (Choi et al. 2010; Davis et al. 2006; de Kleine et al. 2013; Dunlop et al. 2012; Hetrick et al. 2010). Several pharmacological strategies have been identified that are known to facilitate extinction learning in animals (for a recent review, see Fitzgerald et al. 2013). In combination with behavioral therapies, these strategies may offer greater protection from relapse (Foa et al. 2002). Recently, a single L-dopa administration was shown to be effective in preventing renewal of fear in rats (Haaker et al. 2013). As mentioned, acute propranolol administration in the wake of a stressful exposure to a dangerous context has been shown to prevent relapse of fear (Morris et al. 2005b). In modulating glucocorticoid signaling, cannabinoids may offer protection in the form of enhanced extinction learning (de Bitencourt et al. 2013; Ganon-Elazar and Akirav 2012; Rabinak and Phan 2013). Systemic administration of the cholinergic antagonist scopolamine has been found to reduce renewal of fear in low doses, perhaps through disruption of both hippocampal activity and normal context-dependent encoding of extinction (Zelikowsky et al. 2013b). d-Cycloserine has also shown some efficacy in reducing anxiety (Bouton et al. 2008), although its effects on extinction are mixed (Fitzgerald et al. 2013). In patients with generalized anxiety disorder, chronic administration of the melatonergic antidepressant, agomelatine, has been found to be efficacious in reducing relapse (Stein et al. 2012). In the future, more uncommon molecular targets might pave the way for future relapse therapies. For example, global increases of magnesium levels in the brain have been shown to improve extinction retention (Abumaria et al. 2011). Pharmacologically enhanced brain-derived neurotrophic factor signaling in the brains of female mice attenuates renewal of fear (Baker-Andresen et al. 2013). In another study in rats, systemic injection of fibroblast growth factor-2 has been shown to also offer protection against renewal (Graham and Richardson 2010).
Deep Brain Stimulation
Specific regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in rodents and humans have been implicated in fear suppression and resilience (Courtin et al. 2014; Maier et al. 2006), and the PFC has an important role in extinction (Chang and Maren 2011; Herry 2010; Likhtik et al. 2005; Likhtik et al. 2014; Maren 2011; Maroun 2013; Milad et al. 2007; Sotres-Bavon et al. 2012). Stress is thought to perturb these areas of the PFC (Akirav and Maroun 2007; Maier and Watkins 2010; McEwen and Morrison 2013), and one strategy for facilitating extinction involves functional activation of the PFC. By artificially stimulating these regions, resilience in the face of stress might be encouraged, possibly offering a means of relapse prevention. In most cases, artificial stimulation in the brain has been examined only in light of facilitating the extinction process, rather than observing whether its use results in a more resilient form of extinction. That said, others have begun to elucidate the significance of cortical stimulation in relapse prevention. High-frequency stimulation in the PFC of rats has been shown to be effective in preventing reemergence of extinguished fear as a result of a subconditioning procedure (Zheng et al. 2013). Subconditioning involves pairing the US once again with the CS after conditioning; however, the US is dramatically weaker. In turn, the postextinction subconditioning procedure will not initiate reconditioning of fear to the CS but will impair the retention of extinction in future tests. Unlike chronic fluoxetine administration in a separate experiment from the same laboratory, Zheng and colleagues (2013) demonstrated that high-frequency stimulation of the PFC did not buffer against acute stress-induced relapse elicited by exposure to an elevated platform (Deschaux et al. 2012). Cortical stimulation may not be entirely effective alone, but combinative strategies (i.e., behavioral training in conjunction with stimulation) may prove fruitful. Moreover, high-frequency stimulation is not the only method by which resilience may be produced. In a trace-conditioning paradigm, pairing extinction training with low-frequency stimulation in anterior cingulate cortex (a technique that is known to reduce neural excitability and ultimately induce long-term depression) reduced spontaneous recovery in primates (Klavir et al. 2012). Lastly, there is even evidence that peripheral vagus nerve stimulation coinciding with extinction training can facilitate rapid fear suppression (Peña et al. 2013).
Conclusions
The relapse of fear after exposure therapy is a major challenge for clinical interventions for fear and anxiety (Vervliet et al. 2012). Although we have focused on fear recovery phenomena in isolation of one another, it is important to consider that the conditions that precipitate these phenomena might be concurrently experienced to produce additive or super-additive effects on relapse. For example, experiencing both a passage of time with a change in context after extinction (i.e., a paradigm including both spontaneous recovery and renewal components) might result in a particularly strong relapse of fear (Laborda and Miller 2013). Consistent with these ideas, emerging evidence suggests that relapse phenomena (such as spontaneous recovery or reinstatement, for example) may be mediated by separate neural mechanisms (Ma et al. 2012) and in turn may require alternative modes of intervention. Critically, our understanding of these relapse phenomena has been notably advanced by the study of Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction in rodents, which provide a unique and unparalleled opportunity for concurrent behavioral and neural analyses. Nonetheless, more work is clearly needed to understand the myriad of factors that cause fear to return in anxious brains.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01MH065961).
References
- Abumaria N, Yin B, Zhang L, Li XY, Chen T, Descalzi G, Zhao L, Ahn M, Luo L, Ran C, Zhuo M, Liu G. Effects of elevation of brain magnesium on fear conditioning, fear extinction, and synaptic plasticity in the infralimbic prefrontal cortex and lateral amygdala. J Neurosci. 2011;31:14871–14881. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3782-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adamec R, Head D, Blundell J, Burton P, Berton O. Lasting anxiogenic effects of feline predator stress in mice: sex differences in vulnerability to stress and predicting severity of anxiogenic response from the stress experience. Physiol Behav. 2006;88:12–29. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agorastos A, Marmar CR, Otte C. Immediate and early behavioral interventions for the prevention of acute and posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2011;24:526–532. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834cdde2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akirav I, Maroun M. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex-amygdala circuit in stress effects on the extinction of fear. Neural Plast. 2007;2007:30873. doi: 10.1155/2007/30873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akirav I, Maroun M. Stress modulation of reconsolidation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013;226:747–761. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2887-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Almli LM, Fani N, Smith AK, Ressler KJ. Genetic approaches to understanding post-traumatic stress disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;17:355–370. doi: 10.1017/S1461145713001090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antov MI, Wölk C, Stockhorst U. Differential impact of the first and second wave of a stress response on subsequent fear conditioning in healthy men. Biol Psychol. 2013;94:456–468. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Archbold GE, Bouton ME, Nader K. Evidence for the persistence of contextual fear memories following immediate extinction. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:1303–1311. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07161.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Archbold GE, Dobbek N, Nader K. Temporal dynamics of recovery from extinction shortly after extinction acquisition. Learn Mem. 2013;20:395–398. doi: 10.1101/lm.028225.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asan E, Steinke M, Lesch KP. Serotonergic innervation of the amygdala: targets, receptors, and implications for stress and anxiety. Histochem Cell Biol. 2013;139:785–813. doi: 10.1007/s00418-013-1081-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baganz NL, Blakely RD. A dialogue between the immune system and brain, spoken in the language of serotonin. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2013;4:48–63. doi: 10.1021/cn300186b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey CR, Cordell E, Sobin SM, Neumeister A. Recent progress in understanding the pathophysiology of post-traumatic stress disorder: implications for targeted pharmacological treatment. CNS Drugs. 2013;27:221–232. doi: 10.1007/s40263-013-0051-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker KD, McNally GP, Richardson R. Memory retrieval before or after extinction reduces recovery of fear in adolescent rats. Learn Mem. 2013;20:467–473. doi: 10.1101/lm.031989.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker-Andresen D, Flavell CR, Li X, Bredy TW. Activation of BDNF signaling prevents the return of fear in female mice. Learn Mem. 2013;20:237–240. doi: 10.1101/lm.029520.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldini S, Restani L, Baroncelli L, Coltelli M, Franco R, Cenni MC, Maffei L, Berardi N. Enriched early life experiences reduce anxiety-like behavior in rats: a role for insulin-like growth factor 1. J Neurosci. 2013;33:11715–117123. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3541-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balooch SB, Neumann DL, Boschen MJ. Extinction treatment in multiple contexts attenuates ABC renewal in humans. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50:604–609. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baran SE, Armstrong CE, Niren DC, Hanna JJ, Conrad CD. Chronic stress and sex differences on the recall of fear conditioning and extinction. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;91:323–332. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baratta MV, Christianson JP, Gomez DM, Zarza CM, Amat J, Masini CV, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Controllable versus uncontrollable stressors bi-directionally modulate conditioned but not innate fear. Neuroscience. 2007;146:1495–1503. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.03.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Björkqvist K. Social defeat as a stressor in humans. Physiol Behav. 2001;73:435–442. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00490-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolles RC. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychol Rev. 1970;77:32–34. [Google Scholar]
- Boschen MJ, Neumann DL, Waters AM. Relapse of successfully treated anxiety and fear: theoretical issues and recommendations for clinical practice. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009;43:89–100. doi: 10.1080/00048670802607154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Differential control by context in the inflation and reinstatement paradigms. J Exper Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1984;10:56–74. [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Slow reacquisition following the extinction of conditioned suppression. Learn Motiv. 1986;17:1–15. [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Context and ambiguity in the extinction of emotional learning: implications for exposure therapy. Behav Res Ther. 1988;26:137–149. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(88)90113-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol Bull. 1993;114:80–99. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. A learning theory perspective on lapse, relapse, and the maintenance of behavior change. Health Psychol. 2000;19:57–63. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.19.suppl1.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:976–986. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01546-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn Mem. 2004;11:485–494. doi: 10.1101/lm.78804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Role of conditioned contextual stimuli in reinstatement of extinguished fear. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1979;5:368–378. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.5.4.368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, García-Gutiérrez A, Zilski J, Moody EW. Extinction in multiple contexts does not necessarily make extinction less vulnerable to relapse. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44:983–994. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Kenney FA, Rosengard C. State-dependent fear extinction with two benzodiazepine tranquilizers. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:44–55. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.104.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, King DA. Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear: tests for the associative value of the context. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1983;9:248–265. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Nelson JB. Context-specificity of target versus feature inhibition in a feature-negative discrimination. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1994;20:51–65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Ricker ST. Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context. Anim Learn Behav. 1994;22:317–324. [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Vurbic D, Woods AM. D-cycloserine facilitates context-specific fear extinction learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;90:504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Westbrook RF, Corcoran KA, Maren S. Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:352–360. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks DC, Bouton ME. A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates spontaneous recovery. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1993;19:77–89. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.19.1.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks DC, Bouton ME. A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates response recovery (renewal) caused by a return to the conditioning context. J of Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1994;20:366–379. [Google Scholar]
- Cahill SP, Foa EB, Hembree EA, Marshall RD, Nacash N. Dissemination of exposure therapy in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress. 2006;19:597–610. doi: 10.1002/jts.20173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain CK, Blouin AM, Barad M. Temporally massed CS presentations generate more fear extinction than spaced presentations. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2003;29:323–333. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.29.4.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain CK, Maynard GD, Kehne JH. Targeting memory processes with drugs to prevent or cure PTSD. Expert Opin Investig Drugs. 2012;21:1323–1350. doi: 10.1517/13543784.2012.704020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaghan BL, Graham BM, Li S, Richardson R. From resilience to vulnerability: mechanistic insights into the effects of stress on transitions in critical period plasticity. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:90. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Early experiences and the development of emotional learning systems in rats. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2013;3:8. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell BA, Ampuero MX. Dissociation of autonomic and behavioral components of conditioned fear during development in the rat. Behav Neurosci. 1985;99:1089–1102. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.99.6.1089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell J, Ehlert U. Acute psychological stress: does the emotional stress response correspond with physiological responses? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:1111–1134. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cantor C. Post-traumatic stress disorder: evolutionary perspectives. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009;43:1038–1048. doi: 10.3109/00048670903270407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang CH, Knapska E, Orsini CA, Rabinak CA, Zimmerman JM, Maren S. Fear extinction in rodents. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter. 2009;8:23. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0823s47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang CH, Maren S. Early extinction after fear conditioning yields a context-independent and short-term suppression of conditional freezing in rats. Learn Mem. 2009;16:62–68. doi: 10.1101/lm.1085009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang CH, Maren S. Medial prefrontal cortex activation facilitates re-extinction of fear in rats. Learn Mem. 2011;18:221–225. doi: 10.1101/lm.2070111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chelonis JJ, Calton JL, Hart JA, Schachtman TR. Attenuation of the renewal effect by extinction in multiple contexts. Learn Motiv. 1999;30:1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Choi DC, Rothbaum BO, Gerardi M, Ressler KJ. Pharmacological enhancement of behavioral therapy: focus on posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;2:279–299. doi: 10.1007/7854_2009_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christianson JP, Drugan RC, Flyer JG, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Anxiogenic effects of brief swim stress are sensitive to stress history. Progress Neuro-Psychopharm Biol Psychiatry. 2013;44:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MM, Tottenham N, Casey BJ. Translational developmental studies of stress on brain and behavior: implications for adolescent mental health and illness? Neuroscience. 2013;249:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corley MJ, Caruso MJ, Takahashi LK. Stress-induced enhancement of fear conditioning and sensitization facilitates extinction resistant and habituation-resistant fear behaviors in a novel animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Physiol Behav. 2012;105:408–416. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courtin J, Chaudun F, Rozeske RR, Karalis N, Gonzalez-Campo C, Wurtz H, Abdi A, Beaufreton J, Bienvenu TC, Herry C. Prefrontal parvalbumin interneurons shape neuronal activity to drive fear expression. Nature. 2014;505:92–96. doi: 10.1038/nature12755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan CS, Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Acute early-life stress results in premature emergence of adult-like fear retention and extinction relapse in infant rats. Behav Neurosci. 2013;217:703–711. doi: 10.1037/a0034118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Culver NC, Stoyanova M, Craske MG. Clinical relevance of retrieval cues for attenuating context renewal of fear. J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25:284–292. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham CL. Alcohol as a cue for extinction: state dependency produced by conditioned inhibition. Anim Learn Behav. 1979;7:45–52. [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. Morphine and naloxone: effects on conditioned fear as measured with the potentiated startle paradigm. Eur J Pharmacol. 1979;54:341–347. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(79)90063-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1992;15:353–375. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M, Myers KM, Chhatwai J, Ressler KJ. Pharmacological treatments that facilitate extinction of fear: relevance to psychotherapy. NeuroRx. 2006;3:82–96. doi: 10.1016/j.nurx.2005.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson TL. The nature and function of interoceptive signals to feed: toward integration of physiological and learning perspectives. Psychol Rev. 1993;100:640–657. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.100.4.640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bitencourt RM, Pamplona FA, Takahashi RN. A current overview of cannabinoids and glucocorticoids in facilitating extinction of aversive memories: potential extinction enhancers. Neuropharmacology. 2013;64:389–395. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.05.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Kleine RA, Rothbaum BO, van Minnen A. Pharmacological enhancement of exposure-based treatment in PTSD: a qualitative review. Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2013;4 doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.21626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delamater AR, Westbrook RF. Psychological and neural mechanisms of experimental extinction: a selective review. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;108C:38–51. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delgado MR, Nearing KI, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction. Neuron. 2008;59:829–838. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delgado MR, Olsson A, Phelps EA. Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans. Biol Psychol. 2006;73:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denniston JC, Chang RC, Miller RR. Massive extinction treatment attenuates the renewal effect. Learn Motiv. 2003;34:68–86. [Google Scholar]
- Deschaux O, Zheng X, Lavigne J, Nachon O, Cleren C, Moreau J, Garcia R. Post-extinction fluoxetine treatment prevents stress-induced reemergence of extinguished fear. Psychopharm. 2012;225:209–216. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2806-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dibbets P, Havermans R, Arntz A. All we need is a cue to remember: The effect of an extinction cue on renewal. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46:1070–1077. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dibbets P, Moor C, Voncken MJ. The effect of a retrieval cue on the return of spider fear. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2013;44:361–367. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunlop BW, Mansson E, Gerardi M. Pharmacological innovations for posttraumatic stress disorder and medication-enhanced psychotherapy. Curr Pharm Des. 2012;18:5645–5658. doi: 10.2174/138161212803530899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Kordi A, Kästner A, Grube S, Klugmann M, Begemann M, Sperling S, Hammerschmidt K, Hammer C, Stepniak B, Patzig J, de Monasterio-Schrader P, Strenzke N, Flügge G, Werner HB, Pawlak R, Nave KA, Ehrenreich H. A single gene defect causing claustrophobia. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e254. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis ME. Evolution of aversive information processing: a temporal trade-off hypothesis. Brain Behav Evol. 1982;21:151–160. doi: 10.1159/000121623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erhardt A, Spoormaker VI. Translational approaches to anxiety: focus on genetics, fear extinction and brain imaging. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15:417. doi: 10.1007/s11920-013-0417-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS. Conditioned and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. Pavlov J Biol Sci. 1980;15:177–182. doi: 10.1007/BF03001163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS. Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychon Bull Rev. 1994;1:429–438. doi: 10.3758/BF03210947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS, Bolles RC. Naloxone and shock-elicited freezing in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1979;93:736–744. doi: 10.1037/h0077609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS, Calcagnetti DJ, Helmstetter FJ. Role of mu and kappa opioid receptors in conditional fear-induced analgesia: the antagonistic actions of nor-binaltrophimine and the cyclic somatostatin octapeptide, Cys2Tyr3Orn5Pen7-amide. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1989;250:825–830. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS, Gale GD. The amygdala, fear, and memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;985:125–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07077.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrell MR, Sengelaub DR, Wellman CL. Sex differences and chronic stress effects on the neural circuitry underlying fear conditioning and extinction. Physiol Behav. 2013;122:208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felmingham KL, Dobson-Stone C, Schofield PR, Quirk GJ, Bryant RA. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism predicts response to exposure therapy in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73:1059–1063. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald PJ, Seemann JR, Maren S. Can fear extinction be enhanced? A review of pharmacological and behavioral findings. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.12.007. Brain Res Bull [Epub ahead of print Dec 25] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleshner M, Campisi J, Amiri L, Diamond DM. Cat exposure induces both intra- and extracellular Hsp72: the role of adrenal hormones. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29:1142–1152. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB. Prolonged exposure therapy: past, present, and future. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28:1043–1047. doi: 10.1002/da.20907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, Franklin ME, Moser J. Context in the clinic: How well do cognitive-behavioral therapies and medications work in combination? Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:987–997. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01552-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, Street GP. Women and traumatic events. J Clin Psychiatry. 2001;62(Suppl) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox JH, Hammack SE, Falls WA. Exercise is associated with reduction in the anxiogenic effect of mCPP on acoustic startle. Behav Neurosci. 2008;122:943–948. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.4.943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraga MC, de Moura EG, da Silva Lima N, Lisboa PC, de Oliveira E, Silva JO, Claudio-Neto S, Filgueiras CC, Abreu-Villaça Y, Manhães AC. Anxiety-like, novelty-seeking and memory/learning behavioral traits in male Wistar rats submitted to early weaning. Physiol Behav. 2014;124:100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin TB, Saab BJ, Mansuy IM. Neural mechanisms of stress resilience and vulnerability. Neuron. 2012;75:747–761. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galatzer-Levy IR, Bonanno GA, Bush DE, LeDoux JE. Heterogeneity in threat extinction learning: substantive and methodological considerations for identifying individual difference in response to stress. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:55. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganon-Elazar E, Akirav I. Cannabinoids prevent the development of behavioral and endocrine alterations in a rat model of intense stress. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:456–466. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershman SJ, Jones CE, Norman KA, Monfils MH, Niv Y. Gradual extinction prevents the return of fear: Implications for the discovery of state. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:164. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giske J, Eliassen S, Fiksen O, Jakobsen PJ, Aksnes DL, Jørgensen C, Mangel M. Effects of the emotion system on adaptive behavior. Am Nat. 2013;182:689–703. doi: 10.1086/673533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golkar A, Selbing I, Flygare O, Öhman A, Olsson A. Other people as a means to a safe end: Vicarious extinction blocks the return of learned fear. Psychol Sci. 2013;24:2182–2190. doi: 10.1177/0956797613489890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman J, Leong KC, Packard MG. Emotional modulation of multiple memory systems: implications for the neurobiology of post-traumatic stress disorder. Rev Neurosci. 2012;23:627–643. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami S, Cascardi M, Rodríguez-Sierra OE, Duvarci S, Paré D. Impact of predatory threat on fear extinction in Lewis rats. Learn Mem. 2010;17:494–501. doi: 10.1101/lm.1948910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goswami S, Rodriguez-Sierra O, Cascardi M, Paré D. Animal models of post-traumatic stress disorder: Face validity. Front Neurosci. 2013;7:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham BM, Richardson R. Fibroblast growth factor-2 enhances extinction and reduces renewal of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MK, Rani CS, Joshi A, Soto-Piña AE, Martinez PA, Frazer A, Strong R, Morilak DA. Prenatal stress induces long term stress vulnerability, compromising stress response systems in the brain and impairing extinction of conditioned fear after adult stress. Neuroscience. 2011;192:438–451. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.06.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MR, McCormick CM. Effects of stressors in adolescence on learning and memory in rodent models. Horm Behav. 2013;64:364–379. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwood BN, Foley TE, Burhans D, Maier SF, Fleshner M. The consequences of uncontrollable stress are sensitive to duration of prior wheel running. Brain Res. 2005;1033:164–178. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.11.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunther LM, Denniston JC, Miller RR. Conducting exposure treatment in multiple contexts can prevent relapse. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36:75–91. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(97)10019-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunther LM, Miller RR, Matute H. CSs and USs: What's the difference? J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1997;23:15–30. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.23.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gupta RR, Sen S, Diepenhorst LL, Rudick CN, Maren S. Estrogen modulates sexually dimorphic contextual fear conditioning and hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) in rats. Brain Res. 2001;888:356–365. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)03116-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haaker J, Gaburro S, Sah A, Gartmann N, Lonsdorf TB, Meier K, Singewald N, Pape HC, Morellini F, Kalisch R. Single dose of L-dopa makes extinction memories context-independent and prevents the return of fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:E2428–E2436. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1303061110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halladay LR, Zellikowsky M, Blair HT, Fanselow MS. Reinstatement of extinguished fear by an unextinguished conditional stimulus. Front Behav Neurosci. 2012;6:1–7. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hare BD, D'Onfro KC, Hammack SE, Falls WA. Prior stress interferes with the anxiolytic effect of exercise in C57BL/6J mice. Behav Neurosci. 2012;126:850–856. doi: 10.1037/a0030617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartley CA, Gorun A, Reddan MC, Ramirez F, Phelps EA. Stressor controllability modulates fear extinction in humans. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.12.003. Neurobiol Learn Mem [Epub ahead of print Dec 11] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs SC, Koob GF. Application of experimental stressors in laboratory rodents. Curr Protoc Neurosci Chapter. 2006;8:Unit8.4. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0804s34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermans D, Craske MG, Mineka S, Lovibond PF. Extinction in human fear conditioning. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:361–368. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermans D, Dirikx T, Vansteenwegen D, Baeyens F, Van den Bergh O, Eelen P. Reinstatement of fear responses in human aversive conditioning. Behav Res Ther. 2005;43:533–551. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herry C, Ciocchi S, Senn V, Demmou L, Müller C, Lüthi A. Switching on and off fear by distinct neuronal circuits. Nature. 2008;454:600–606. doi: 10.1038/nature07166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herry C, Ferraguti F, Singewald N, Letzkus JJ, Ehrlich I, Lüthi A. Neuronal circuits of fear extinction. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:599–612. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07101.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hetrick SE, Purcell R, Garner B, Parslow R. Combined pharmacotherapy and psychological therapies for post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) 2010 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007316.pub2. Cochrane Database Syst Rev:CD007316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofmann SG. Enhancing exposure-based therapy from a translational research perspective. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:1987–2001. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC. Event representation in Pavlovian conditioning: image and action. Cognition. 1990;37:105–131. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90020-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes NM, Westbrook RF. Extinction of reinstated or ABC renewed fear responses renders them resistant to subsequent ABA renewal. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2013;39:208–220. doi: 10.1037/a0031986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hou R, Baldwin DS. A neuroimmunological perspective on anxiety disorders. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2012;27:6–14. doi: 10.1002/hup.1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Yang J, Song T, Hou N, Liu Y, Zhao X, Zhang D, Wang L, Wang T, Huang C. A new stress model, a scream sound, alters learning and monoamine levels in rat brain. Physiol Behav. 2014;123:105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huemer J, Erhart F, Steiner H. Posttraumatic stress disorder in children and adolescents: A review of psychopharmacological treatment. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2010;41:624–640. doi: 10.1007/s10578-010-0192-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huff NC, Hernandez JA, Blanding NQ, LaBar KS. Delayed extinction attenuates conditioned fear renewal and spontaneous recovery in humans. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123:834–843. doi: 10.1037/a0016511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huhman KL. Social conflict models: Can they inform us about human psychopathology. Horm Behav. 2006;50:640–646. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs WJ, Nadel L. Stress-induced recovery of fears and phobias. Psychol Rev. 1985;92:512–531. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Järbe TU, Sterner U, Hjerpe C. Conditioning of an interoceptive drug stimulus to different exteroceptive contexts. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1981;73:23–26. doi: 10.1007/BF00431094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ji J, Maren S. Hippocampal involvement in contextual modulation of fear extinction. Hippocampus. 2007;17:749–758. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovanovic T, Ressler KJ. How the neurocircuitry and genetics of fear inhibition may inform our understanding of PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:648–662. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09071074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kearns MC, Ressler KJ, Zatzick D, Rothbaum BO. Early interventions for PTSD: A review. Depress Anxiety. 2012;29:833–842. doi: 10.1002/da.21997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kehoe EJ, Macrae M. Savings in animal learning: implications for relapse and maintenance after therapy. Behav Ther. 1997;28:141–155. [Google Scholar]
- Kellet J, Kokkinidis L. Extinction deficit and fear reinstatement after electrical stimulation of the amygdala: Implications for kindling-associated fear and anxiety. Neuroscience. 2004;127:227–287. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp AH, Quintana DS. The relationship between mental and physical health: Insights from the study of heart rate variability. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;89:288–296. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Richardson R. A developmental dissociation in reinstatement of an extinguished fear response in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007a;88:48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Richardson R. A developmental dissociation of context and GABA effects on extinguished fear in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2007b;121:131–139. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.1.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Richardson R. New findings on extinction of conditioned fear early in development: Theoretical and clinical implications. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:453–462. doi: 10.1038/nrn849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kindt M, Soeter M, Vervliet B. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:256–258. doi: 10.1038/nn.2271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klavir O, Genud-Gabai R, Paz R. Low-frequency stimulation depresses the primate anterior-cingulate-cortex and prevents spontaneous recovery of aversive memories. J Neurosci. 2012;32:8589–8597. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6481-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleim B, Wilhelm FH, Temp L, Margraf J, Wiederhold BK, Rasch B. Sleep enhances exposure therapy. Psychol Med. 2013;10:1–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knapska E, Macias M, Mikosz M, Nowak A, Owczarek D, Wawrzyniak M, Pieprzyk M, Cymerman IA, Werka T, Sheng M, Maren S, Jaworski J, Kaczmarek L. Functional anatomy of neural circuits regulating fear and extinction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:17093–17098. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202087109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knox D, George SA, Fitzpatrick CJ, Rabinak CA, Maren S, Liberzon L. Single prolonged stress disrupts retention of extinguished fear in rats. Learn Mem. 2012;19:43–49. doi: 10.1101/lm.024356.111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobayashi I, Cowdin N, Mellman TA. One's sex, sleep, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Sex Differ. 2012;3:29. doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-3-29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong E, Monje FJ, Hirsch J, Pollak DD. Learning not to fear: Neural correlates of learned safety. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:515–527. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konorski J. Integrative Activity of the Brain: An Interdisciplinary Approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Kubala KH, Christianson JP, Kaufman RD, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Short- and long-term consequences of stressor controllability in adolescent rats. Behav Brain Res. 2012;234:278–284. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kull S, Müller BH, Blechert J, Wilhelm FH, Michael T. Reinstatement of fear in humans: autonomic and experiential responses in a differential conditioning paradigm. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2012;140:43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laborda MA, Miller RR. Preventing return of fear in an animal model of anxiety: additive effects of massive extinction and extinction in multiple contexts. Behav Ther. 2013;44:249–261. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebron-Milad K, Tsareva A, Ahmed N, Milad MR. Sex differences and estrous cycle in female rats interact with the effects of fluoxetine treatment on fear extinction. Behav Brain Res. 2013;253:217–222. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeDoux JE. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron. 2012;73:653–676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung HT, Westbrook RF. Increased spontaneous recovery with increases in conditioned stimulus alone exposures. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2010;36:354–367. doi: 10.1037/a0017882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li SH, Westbrook RF. Massed extinction trials produce better short-term but worse long-term loss of context conditioned fear responses than spaced trials. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2008;34:336–351. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.34.3.336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Likhtik E, Pelletier JG, Paz R, Paré D. Prefrontal control of the amygdala. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7429–7437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2314-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Likhtik E, Stujenske JM, A Topiwala M, Harris AZ, Gordon JA. Prefrontal entrainment of amygdala activity signals safety in learned fear and innate anxiety. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:106–113. doi: 10.1038/nn.3582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lissek S. Toward an account of clinical anxiety predicated on basic, neurally mapped mechanisms of Pavlovian fear learning: the case for conditioned overgeneralization. Depress Anxiety. 2012;29:257–263. doi: 10.1002/da.21922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lissek S, Glaubitz B, Uengoer M, Tegenthoff M. Hippocampal activation during extinction learning predicts occurrence of the renewal effect in extinction recall. Neuroimage. 2013;81:131–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lolordo VM, Rescorla RA. Protection of the fear-eliciting capacity of a stimulus from extinction. Acta Biol Exp (Warsz) 1966;26:251–258. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long VA, Fanselow MS. Stress-enhanced fear learning in rats is resistant to the effects of immediate massed extinction. Stress. 2012;15:627–636. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.650251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lueken U, Kruschwitz JD, Muehlhan M, Siegert J, Hoyer J, Wittchen HU. How specific is specific phobia? Different neural response patterns in two subtypes of specific phobia. Neuroimage. 2011;56:363–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch J, 3rd, Cullen PK, Jasnow AM, Riccio DC. Sex differences in the generalization of fear as a function of retention intervals. Learn Mem. 2013;20:628–632. doi: 10.1101/lm.032011.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma X, Ma YY, Yu LC. Distinct mechanisms mediate the reinstatement and spontaneous recovery of extinguished fear in rats. Neurosci Lett. 2012;510:34–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mactutus CF, Concannon JT, Riccio DC. Nonmonotonic age changes in susceptibility to hypothermia-induced retrograde amnesia in rats. Physiol Behav. 1982;28:939–943. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(82)90218-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maddox SA, Schafe GE, Ressler KJ. Exploring epigenetic regulation of fear memory and biomarkers associated with post-traumatic stress disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:62. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier SF, Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Watkins LR. Behavioral control, the medial prefrontal cortex, and resilience. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2006;8:397–406. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/smaier. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier SF, Watkins LR. Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in coping and resilience. Brain Res. 2010;1355:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maner JK, Miller SL, Schmidt NB, Eckel LA. Submitting to defeat: social anxiety, dominance threat, and decrements in testosterone. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:764–768. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02154.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:897–931. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S. Building and burying fear memories in the brain. Neuroscientist. 2005;11:89–99. doi: 10.1177/1073858404269232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S. Pavlovian fear conditioning as a behavioral assay for hippocampus and amygdala function: cautions and caveats. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28:1661–1666. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06485.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S. Seeking a spotless mind: extinction, deconsolidation, and erasure of fear memory. Neuron. 2011;70:830–845. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S. Nature and causes of the immediate extinction deficit: a brief review. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.012. Neurobiol Learn Mem [Epub ahead of print Oct 29] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S. Fear of the unexpected: Hippocampus mediates novelty-induced return of extinguished fear in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;108:88–95. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S, Chang CH. Recent fear is resistant to extinction. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:18020–18025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608398103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S, De Oca B, Fanselow MS. Sex differences in hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats: positive correlation between LTP and contextual learning. Brain Res. 1994;661:25–34. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91176-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S, Phan KL, Liberzon I. The contextual brain: implications for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14:417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn3492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maroun M. Medial prefrontal cortex: Multiple roles in fear and extinction. Neuroscientist. 2013;19:370–383. doi: 10.1177/1073858412464527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCarty R, Kopin IJ. Sympatho-adrenal medullary activity and behavior during exposure to footshock stress: A comparison of seven rat strains. Physiol Behav. 1978;21:567–572. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(78)90132-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS, Morrison JH. The brain on stress: vulnerability and plasticity of the prefrontal cortex over the life course. Neuron. 2013;79:16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLean CP, Foa EB. Prolonged exposure therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder: A review of evidence and dissemination. Expert Rev Neurother. 2011;11:1151–1163. doi: 10.1586/ern.11.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNally RJ. Pavlovian conditioning and preparedness: Effects of initial fear level. Behav Res Ther. 1986;24:27–33. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90146-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNeal N, Scotti MA, Wardell J, Chandler DL, Bates SL, Larocca M, Trahanas DM, Grippo AJ. Disruption of social bonds induces behavioral and physiological dysregulation in male and female prairie voles. Auton Neurosci. 2014;180:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merz CJ, Hermann A, Stark R, Wolf OT. Cortisol modifies extinction learning of recently acquired fear in men. 2013a doi: 10.1093/scan/nst137. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci [Epub ahead of print Sep 11] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merz CJ, Wolf OT, Schweckendiek J, Klucken T, Vaitl D, Stark R. Stress differentially affects fear conditioning in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013b;38:2529–2541. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Goldstein JM, Orr SP, Wedig MM, Klibanski A, Pitman RK, Rauch SL. Fear conditioning and extinction: Influence of sex and menstrual cycle in healthy humans. Behav Neurosci. 2006a;120:1196–1203. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Igoe SA, Lebron-Milad K, Novales JE. Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience. 2009;164:887–895. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Orr SP, Lasko NB, Chang Y, Rauch SL, Pitman RK. Presence and acquired origin of reduced recall for fear extinction in PTSD: Results of a twin study. J Psychiatr Res. 2008;42:515–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: Ten years of progress. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:129–151. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction in rats: Implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biol Psychol. 2006b;73:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Wright CI, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ, Rauch SL. Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:446–454. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mineka S, Oehlberg K. The relevance of recent developments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008;127:567–580. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mineka S, Öhman A. Phobias and preparedness: the selective, autonomic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Bio Psychiatry. 2002;52:927–937. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01669-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitra R, Sapolsky RM. Effects of enrichment predominate over those of chronic stress on fear-related behavior in male rats. Stress. 2009;12:305–312. doi: 10.1080/10253890802379955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monfils MH, Cowansage KK, Klann E, LeDoux JE. Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: Key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science. 2009;324:951–955. doi: 10.1126/science.1167975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RW, Furlong TM, Westbrook RF. Recent exposure to a dangerous context impairs extinction and reinstates lost fear reactions. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2005a;31:40–55. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.31.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RW, Westbrook RF, Killcross AS. Reinstatement of extinguished fear by beta-adrenergic arousal elicited by a conditioned context. Behav Neurosci. 2005b;119:1662–1671. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Motraghi TE, Seim RW, Meyer EC, Morissette SB. Virtual reality exposure therapy for the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder: A methodological review using CONSORT guidelines. J Clin Psychol. 2014;70:197–208. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers KM, Davis M. Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction. Neuron. 2002;36:567–584. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01064-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers KM, Ressler KJ, Davis M. Different mechanisms of fear extinction dependent on length of time since fear acquisition. Learn Mem. 2006;13:216–223. doi: 10.1101/lm.119806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mystkowski JL, Craske MG, Echiverri AM. Treatment context and return of fear in spider phobia. Behav Ther. 2002;33:399–416. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2005.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Napier RM, Macrae M, Kehoe EJ. Rapid reacquisition in conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1992;18:182–192. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.18.2.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neumann DL, Kitlertsirivatana E. Exposure to a novel context after extinction causes a renewal of extinguished conditioned responses: Implications for the treatment of fear. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48:565–567. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neumann DL, Longbottom PL. The renewal of extinguished conditioned fear with fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli by a context change after extinction. Behav Res Ther. 2008;46:188–206. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nissen HW. "Freezing" behavior in rats. Science. 1946;103:27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norrholm SD, Jovanovic T, Smith AK, Binder E, Klengel T, Conneely K, Mercer KB, Davis JS, Kerley K, Winkler J, Gillespie CF, Bradley B, Ressler KJ. Differential genetic and epigenetic regulation of catechol-O-methyltransferase is associated with impaired fear inhibition in posttraumatic stress disorder. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:30. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öhman A, Eriksson A, Olofsson C. One-trial learning and superior resistance to extinction of autonomic responses conditioned to potentially phobic stimuli. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1975a;88:619–627. doi: 10.1037/h0078388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öhman A, Erixon G, Löfberg I. Phobias and preparedness: Phobic versus neutral pictures as conditioned stimuli for human autonomic responses. J Abnorm Psychol. 1975b;84:41–45. doi: 10.1037/h0076255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öhman A, Mineka S. Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev. 2001;108:483–522. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orinstein AJ, Urcelay GP, Miller RR. Expanding the intertrial interval during extinction: Response cessation and recovery. Behav Ther. 2010;41:14–29. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsini CA, Yen C, Maren S. Ensemble coding of context-dependent fear memory in the amygdala. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013;7:199. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pace-Schott EF, Spencer RM, Vijayakumar S, Ahmed NA, Verga PW, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Milad MR. Extinction of conditioned fear is better learned and recalled in the morning than in the evening. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47:1776–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.07.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packard MG, Goodman J. Emotional arousal and multiple memory systems in the mammalian brain. Front Behav Neurosci. 2012;6:14. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papciak J, Popik P, Fuchs E, Rygula R. Chronic psychosocial stress makes rats more ‘pessimistic’ in the ambiguous-cue interpretation paradigm. Behav Brain Res. 2013;256:305–310. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.08.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov IP. Conditioned Reflexes. London: Oxford University Press; 1927. [Google Scholar]
- Peña DF, Engineer ND, McIntyre CK. Rapid remission of conditioned fear expression with extinction training paired with vagus nerve stimulation. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73:1071–1077. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peri T, Ben-Shakhar G, Orr SP, Shaley AY. Psychophysiologic assessment of aversive conditioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:512–519. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00144-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polack CW, Laborda MA, Miller RR. On the differences in degree of renewal produced by the different renewal designs. Behav Processes. 2013;99:112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powers MB, Halpern JM, Ferenschak MP, Gillihan SJ, Foa EB. A meta-analytic review of prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30:635–641. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Przewłocka B, Sumová A, Lasoń W. The influence of conditioned fear-induced stress on the opioid systems in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;37:661–666. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90543-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quirk GJ. Memory for extinction of conditioned fear is long-lasting and persists following spontaneous recovery. Learn Mem. 2002;9:402–407. doi: 10.1101/lm.49602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quirk GJ, Paré D, Richardson R, Herry C, Monfils MH, Schiller D, Vicentic A. Erasing fear memories with extinction training. J Neurosci. 2010;30:14993–14997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4268-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabinak CA, Phan KL. Cannabinoid modulation of fear extinction brain circuits: A novel target to advance anxiety treatment. 2013 doi: 10.2174/13816128113199990437. Curr Pharm Des [Epub ahead of print Jun 14] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman SJ. The return of fear. Behav Res Ther. 1979;17:164–165. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(79)90028-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachman SJ. The return of fear: Review and prospect. Clin Psychol Rev. 1989;9:147–168. [Google Scholar]
- Rasmusson AM, Charney DS. Animal models of relevance to PTSD. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1997;821:332–351. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48290.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rau V, DeCola JP, Fanselow MS. Stress-induced enhancement of fear learning: An animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:1207–1223. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rau V, Fanselow MS. Exposure to a stressor produces a long lasting enhancement of fear learning in rats. Stress. 2009;12:125–133. doi: 10.1080/10253890802137320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauch SA, Eftekhari A, Ruzek JI. Review of exposure therapy: A gold standard for PTSD treatment. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2012;49:679–687. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2011.08.0152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauhut AS, Thomas BL, Ayres JJ. Treatments that weaken Pavlovian conditioned fear and thwart its renewal in rats: implications for treating human phobias. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2001;27:99–114. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. Spontaneous recovery after Pavlovian conditioning with multiple outcomes. Anim Learn Behav. 1997;25:99–107. [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. Retraining of extinguished Pavlovian stimuli. J of Exp Psychol Anim Behav Processes. 2001;27:115–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. Spontaneous recovery. Learn Mem. 2004;11:501–509. doi: 10.1101/lm.77504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, Heth CD. Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1975;1:88–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Revillo DA, Molina JC, Paglini MG, Arias C. A sensory-enhanced context allows renewal of an extinguished fear response in the infant rat. Behav Brain Res. 2013;253:173–177. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richardson R, Riccio DC, Axiotis R. Alleviation of infantile amnesia in rats by internal and external contextual cues. Dev Psychobiol. 1986;19:453–462. doi: 10.1002/dev.420190506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ricker ST, Bouton ME. Reacquisition following extinction in appetitive conditioning. Anim Learn Behav. 1996;24:423–436. [Google Scholar]
- Riess BF. A possible explanation of "freezing" behavior in rats. Science. 1945;102:570. doi: 10.1126/science.102.2657.570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts NP, Kitchiner NJ, Kenardy J, Bisson JI. Systematic review and meta-analysis of multiple-session early interventions following traumatic events. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:293–301. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08040590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson OJ, Overstreet C, Charney DR, Vytal K, Grillon C. Stress increases aversive prediction error signal in the ventral striatum. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:4129–4133. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213923110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues SM, LeDoux JE, Sapolsky RM. The influence of stress hormones on fear circuitry. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:289–313. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez BI, Craske MG, Mineka S, Hladek D. Context-specificity of relapse: effects of therapist and environmental context on return of fear. Behav Res Ther. 1999;37:845–862. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00106-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosas JM, Todd TP, Bouton ME. Context change and associative learning. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2013;4:237–244. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth MK, Bingham B, Shah A, Joshi A, Frazer A, Strong R, Morilak DA. Effects of chronic plus acute prolonged stress on measures of coping style, anxiety, and evoked HPA-axis reactivity. Neuropharmacology. 2012;63:1118–1126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothbaum BO, Schwartz AC. Exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychother. 2002;56:59–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.2002.56.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe MK, Craske MG. Effects of an expanding-spaced vs. massed exposure schedule on fear reduction and return of fear. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36:701–717. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(97)10016-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salam JN, Fox JH, Detroy EM, Guignon MH, Wohl DF, Falls WA. Voluntary exercise in C57 mice is anxiolytic across several measures of anxiety. Behav Brain Res. 2009;197:31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.07.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders MJ. Context processing in aging: Older mice are impaired in renewal of extinguished fear. Exp Aging Res. 2011;37:572–594. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2011.619874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders MJ, Stevens S, Boeh H. Stress enhancement of fear learning in mice is dependent upon stressor type: Effects of sex and ovarian hormones. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2010;94:254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sapolsky RM. Stress and plasticity in the limbic system. Neurochem Res. 2003;28:1735–1742. doi: 10.1023/a:1026021307833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiller D, Monfils MH, Raio CM, Johnson DC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature. 2010;463:49–53. doi: 10.1038/nature08637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultheiss OC, Wirth MM, Torges CM, Pang JS, Villacorta MA, Welsh KM. Effects of implicit power motivation on men's and women's implicit learning and testosterone changes after social victory or defeat. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;88:174–188. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Servatius RJ, Beck KD. Mild interoceptive stressors affect learning and reactivity to contextual cues: Toward understanding the development of unexplained illnesses. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1483–1491. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seymour B, O'Doherty JP, Dayan P, Koltzenburg M, Jones AK, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. Temporal difference models describe higher-order learning in humans. Nature. 2004;429:664–667. doi: 10.1038/nature02581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soeter M, Kindt M. Noradrenergic enhancement of associative fear memory in humans. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2011;96:263–271. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sotres-Bavon F, Sierra-Mercado D, Pardilla-Delgado E, Quirk GJ. Gating of fear in prelimbic cortex by hippocampal and amygdala inputs. Neuron. 2012;76:804–812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein DJ, Ahokas A, Albarran C, Olivier V, Allgulander C. Agomelatine prevents relapse in generalized anxiety disorder: A 6-month randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled discontinuation study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012;73:1002–1008. doi: 10.4088/JCP.11m07493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi LK, Turner JG, Kalin NH. Development of stress-induced responses in preweanling rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1991;24:341–360. doi: 10.1002/dev.420240504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timmermans W, Xiong H, Hoogenraad CC, Krugers HJ. Stress and excitatory synapses: From health to disease. Neuroscience. 2013;248:626–636. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.05.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trammell JP, Clore GL. Does stress enhance or impair memory consolidation? Cogn Emot. 2014;28:361–374. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.822346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao JCI, Craske MG. Timing of treatment and return of fear: Effects of massed uniform- and expanding-spaced exposure schedules. Behav Ther. 2000;31:479–497. [Google Scholar]
- Tye KM, Prakash R, Kim SY, Fenno LE, Grosenick L, Zarabi H, Thompson KR, Gradinaru V, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K. Amygdala circuitry mediating reversible and bidirectional control of anxiety. Nature. 2011;471:358–362. doi: 10.1038/nature09820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urcelay GP, Wheeler DS, Miller RR. Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery. Learn Behav. 2009;37:60–73. doi: 10.3758/LB.37.1.60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uschold-Schmidt N, Peterlik D, Füchsl AM, Reber SO. HPA axis changes during the initial phase of psychosocial stressor exposure in male mice. J Endocrinol. 2013;218:193–203. doi: 10.1530/JOE-13-0027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanElzakker MB, Kathryn Dahlgren M, Caroline Davis F, Dubois S, Shin LM. From Pavlov to PTSD: The extinction of conditioned fear in rodents, humans, and in anxiety disorders. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.11.014. Neurobiol Learn Mem [Epub ahead of print Dec 7] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervliet B, Baevens F, Van den Bergh O, Hermans D. Extinction, generalization, and return of fear: A critical review of renewal research in humans. Biol Psychol. 2013;92:51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vervliet B, Craske MG, Hermans D. Fear extinction and relapse: State of the art. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012;9:215–248. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waddell J, Morris RW, Bouton ME. Effects of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis lesions on conditioned anxiety: Aversive conditioning with long-duration conditional stimuli and reinstatement of extinguished fear. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:324–336. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.2.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace KJ, Rosen JB. Predator odor as an unconditioned fear stimulus in rats: Elicitation of freezing by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114:912–922. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.5.912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westbrook RF, Iordanova M, McNally G, Richardson R, Harris JA. Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus: Two roles for context. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2002;28:97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wideman CH, Cierniak KH, Sweet WE, Moravec C, Murphy HM. An animal model of stress-induced cardiomyopathy utilizing the social defeat paradigm. Physiol Behav. 2013;120:220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilker S, Elbert T, Kolassa IT. The downside of strong emotional memories: How human memory-related genes influence the risk for posttraumatic stress disorder: A selective review. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.08.015. Neurobiol Learn Mem [Epub ahead of print Sep 4] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wixted J. Sleep aromatherapy curbs conditioned fear. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:1510–1512. doi: 10.1038/nn.3556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright LD, Muir KE, Perrot TS. Enhanced stress responses in adolescent versus adult rats exposed to cues of predation threat, and peer interaction as a predictor of adult defensiveness. Dev Psychobiol. 2012;54:47–69. doi: 10.1002/dev.20575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto S, Morinobu S, Fuchikami M, Kurata A, Kozuru T, Yamawaki S. Effects of single prolonged stress and D-cycloserine on contextual fear extinction and hippocampal NMDA receptor expression in a rat model of PTSD. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:2108–2116. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zantvoord JB, Diehle J, Lindauer RJ. Using neurobiological measures to predict and assess treatment outcome of psychotherapy in posttraumatic stress disorder: Systematic review. Psychother Psychosom. 2013;82:142–151. doi: 10.1159/000343258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zelikowsky M, Bissiere S, Hast TA, Bennett RZ, Abdipranoto A, Vissel B, Fanselow MS. Prefrontal microcircuit underlies contextual learning after hippocampal loss. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013a;110:9938–9943. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301691110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zelikowsky M, Hast TA, Bennett RZ, Merjanian M, Nocera NA, Ponnusamy R, Fanselow MS. Cholinergic blockade frees fear extinction from its contextual dependency. Biol Psychiatry. 2013b;73:345–352. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng X, Deschaux O, Lavigne J, Nachon O, Cleren C, Moreau JL, Garcia R. Prefrontal high-frequency stimulation prevents sub-conditioning procedure-provoked, but not acute stress-provoked, reemergence of extinguished fear. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013;101:33–38. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zovkic IB, Meadows JP, Kaas GA, Sweatt JD. Interindividual variability in stress susceptibility: A role for epigenetic mechanisms in PTSD. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:60. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]