Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Child Fam Stud. 2013 Sep 19;23(7):1298–1309. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9830-2

Mindfulness and Self-Compassion: Exploring Pathways to Adolescent Emotional Well-Being

Karen Bluth 1, Priscilla W Blanton 2
PMCID: PMC4198323  NIHMSID: NIHMS526086  PMID: 25328369

Abstract

Adolescents today are confronted with the compounded stressors of life in our high-pressured society and the cognitive, physiological, and emotional changes characteristic of this stage of development. To explore ways to promote well-being in this population, mindfulness, defined as paying attention in the moment in an intentional and purposeful way, was examined in terms of its associations with aspects of emotional well being. It has been reported to have positive effects on emotional well-being in adults, and shows promise for similar results in research with youth. Moreover, the mechanisms through which being mindful may influence positive outcomes have only recently been explored, and have not been investigated with adolescents. Self-compassion, defined by the three components of self-kindness, sensing oneself as part of a common humanity, and maintaining perspective in challenging circumstances, was examined as a potential mediator of the relationship of mindfulness to various outcome measures. Measures assessing mindfulness, self-compassion, and aspects of emotional well-being comprised an online survey that was administered to 67 adolescents in an urban high school. Path analysis was utilized to explore relationships among the variables. An alternate model with self-compassion as the predictor and mindfulness as the mediator was also investigated. Results suggested that both mindfulness and self-compassion functioned as mediators in the pathway to emotional well-being. A theorized model is presented which depicts a reciprocal relationship between mindfulness and self-compassion and describes an iterative process that takes place between these two constructs, promoting emotional well-being. Implications for research and practice include conducting longitudinal studies, which assess constructs at three time points to definitively establish mediation, and developing a self-compassion program tailored for adolescents to facilitate improvements in emotional well-being.

Keywords: mindfulness, self-compassion, emotional well-being, stress, adolescence

Introduction

Stress has become an integral and accepted part of daily life in our culture. Whether as a result of our busy lives or the influence and expectations presented by the media, our children also are beginning to show signs of stress. Bailey (2011) explained that the pressures accumulating from school, peer interactions, and day-to-day family living can take a toll on children. Adolescents are exposed to the same stressors of school, activities, and family life, as are their younger counterparts. However, they are also subject to the rapid cognitive, physiological, and psychosocial changes that are characteristic of this stage of development.

Mindfulness, a 2500 year old practice recently introduced in the West, has been associated with reduced stress in adults (see Greeson, 2009). Rooted in Eastern contemplative traditions, mindfulness is a state of consciousness in which one brings awareness and attentiveness to their immediate experience (Grossman, 2010). Recognizing that mindfulness is both an outcome, as when one refers to mindful awareness, and a process, as that which occurs when one engages in mindfulness practice, one definition which encompasses this construct is, “The awareness that arises through intentionally attending in an open, accepting, and discerning way to whatever is arising in the present moment” (cf. Shapiro, 2009, p. 556).

It is important to clarify the difference between mindfulness as an outcome, which will be referred to herein as mindfulness, and mindfulness as a process, referred to herein as mindfulness practice. Mindfulness is described as a state or trait in which an individual becomes increasingly aware and attentive in the moment. It may include specific qualities such as acceptance or the ability to describe an inner experience in the moment that it is taking place. Mindfulness practice, on the other hand, refers to a daily time dedicated to practicing techniques which encourage mindfulness. These techniques include breath awareness, mindful movement (gentle yoga), and a body awareness practice referred to as a body scan. Additionally, a third term, mindfulness intervention, refers to a program or series of classes in which one is taught mindfulness practice techniques and encouraged to develop a daily practice. The most empirically evaluated mindfulness intervention is Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) (Kabat-Zinn, 1982, 1990).

There has been debate about conceptual clarity among definitions of mindfulness in scholarly work. Through much debate and discussion, a two component model of mindfulness has been presented consisting of (a) self-regulation of attention, described as bringing awareness to the focus of attention so that one is able to attend fully to the continually changing field of thoughts, feelings, and sensations, and (b) orientation to experience, which refers to the attitude or approach one takes in attending to the present moment (Bishop et al., 2004). Mindfulness practice encourages an approach of openness, curiosity and acceptance. When practicing this approach with regularity, it is believed that one becomes less likely to avoid or suppress certain emotions, since they are perceived as less threatening. Self-criticism wanes since that which one experiences in the moment is no longer loaded with emotional meaning and self-judgment, but is accepted as an integral part of the condition of being human (Salzberg, 2011). In other words, instead of our thought processes being constantly engaged with trying to make things better and judging ourselves for not meeting our own expectations of ourselves, we “let go” of trying to make things different than they are, and accept that which is. In this process, we exercise a compassionate stance towards ourselves, and with regular practice, this stance eventually becomes integrated in our ongoing relationship with ourselves.

Over the last 30 years, empirical studies have offered ample evidence for the positive physiological and psychological effects of mindfulness for adults (Baer, 2003; Greeson, 2009; Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004; Keng, Smoski, Robins, 2011). These effects have included improvement in chronic pain (Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, & Burney, 1985; Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, Burney, & Sellers, 1987; Morone, Greco, & Weiner, 2008; Pradhan et al., 2007; Zautra et al., 2008) and overall immune function (Bartsch et al., 1992; Massion, Teas, Hebert, Wertheimer, & Kabat-Zinn, 1995). Additionally, anxiety (e.g., Kabat-Zinn et al., 1992; Miller, Fletcher, & Kabat-Zinn, 1995) depression (e.g., Fincune & Mercer, 2006; Kutz et al., 1985; Teasdale, 2004), mood disturbance (Carlson, Ursuliak, Goodey, Angen, & Speca, 2001), social phobia (Boguls, Sijbers, & Voncken, 2006) and stress-level (e.g., Speca, Carlson, Goodey, & Angen, 2000) were significantly lower post-treatment in clinical and non-clinical samples and in several studies these positive outcomes were maintained at follow-up.

Empirical research on adolescents is minimal, however, (Burke, 2010; Twohig, Field, Armstrong, & Dahl, 2010) and most of these efforts have been conducted in intervention studies. These intervention studies reported lower negative affect and greater positive affect in both high school students (Broderick & Metz, 2009; Ciarrochi, Kashdan, Leeson & Jordan, 2010) and 4th through 7th grade students (Schonert-Reichl & Lawlor, 2010), an increase in overall well-being for 14 and 15 year-old boys in an independent British school (Huppert & Johnson, 2010), a decrease in perceived stress among clinic-referred 14-18 year olds (Biegel, Brown, Shapiro & Schubert, 2009), and a decrease in psychological distress in 14-17 year olds who had been diagnosed with a mental health disorder (Tan & Martin, 2013). Recently, a qualitative study reported greater calm and balance, control and self-efficacy in non-clinical 16-24 year olds and participants claimed a greater understanding of both themselves and others (Monshat et al., 2013).

Results of correlational studies reflect findings similar to those of intervention studies, as well as to those of both correlational and interventional studies with adults. For example, in the validation of the Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale for adolescents (MAAS-A), findings indicated that higher MAAS-A scores indicating higher levels of mindfulness were positively associated with greater life satisfaction, happiness, positive affect, self-regulation, and wellness, and negatively associated with negative affect and a tendency to use substances as a coping mechanism among 14-18 year olds from eight Midwestern public schools (Brown, West, Loverich & Biegel, 2011). Similarly, mindfulness was positively associated with overall quality of life and negatively associated with internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors in a sample of 5th through 10th grade students (Greco, Baer, & Smith, 2011). In a sample of Australian tenth graders from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds, awareness and acceptance, two key components of mindfulness, were associated with lower reported levels of fear, hostility, and sadness, and positively associated with positive affect (Ciarroci, Kashdan, Leeson, Heaven, & Jordan, 2010). The results of these correlational analyses indicated strong support for the positive associations between mindfulness and dimensions of emotional well-being among adolescents. The next section explores self-compassion as a potential mechanism, or mediator, which was hypothesized to elucidate the link between mindfulness and dimensions of emotional well-being.

Self-compassion, as defined by Neff (2003a), is a construct that encompasses three main components: self-kindness, a recognition that we are part of a common humanity, and a capacity for holding one's emotional experience in balanced awareness (mindfulness). Self-compassion is not to be confused with self-esteem, which involves a comparison of one's abilities with those of others, resulting in an evaluation or judgment about where one stands in the social hierarchy (Neff & Vonk, 2008). In contrast, self-compassion involves a linking or fundamental connection with others through an understanding of our common humanity (Neff, 2003b).

Self-compassion research is burgeoning, and findings have revealed an association with positive well-being in a number of recent studies. A meta-analysis of 14 self-compassion studies conducted with healthy adults (student and community samples) reported a large effect size indicating a negative association between psychopathology (defined by aggregating anxiety, depression, and stress) and self-compassion (Macbeth and Gumley, 2012). Other research findings with adults have indicated positive correlations with life-satisfaction, happiness, optimism, positive affect, spirituality, emotional intelligence, coping skills, self-improvement motivation and overall psychological well-being in both community samples and university students (Baer, Lykins, & Peters, 2012; Bishop et al., 2004; Breines & Chen, 2012; Heffernan, Quinn Griffin, Sister Rita, & Fitzpatrick, 2010; Leary, Tate, Adams, Allen, & Hancock, 2007; Neely, Schallert, Mohammed, Roberts, & Chen, 2009; Neff & Vonk, 2008; Neff, 2003a; Neff, Hsieh, & Dejitterat, 2005; Neff, Kirkpatrick, & Rude, 2007).

Finally, if mindfulness interventions promote increased self-compassion, then one would expect individuals who have a history of mindfulness practice to be more self-compassionate. In two studies comparing long-term practitioners to those who were novices to mindfulness practice, this outcome was found to be the case (Lykins & Baer, 2009; Neff, 2003a).

Self-compassion may be a particularly salient construct for helping us to understand adolescents because of its capacity to affect well-being. The developmental stage of adolescence can be particularly trying, as adolescents are often consumed with self-judgment and questioning their self-worth (Harter, 1993; Jacobs, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002). Additionally, as adolescents frequently feel alone on the emotional roller-coaster that they are experiencing, the component of common humanity may be particularly relevant to this stage of development. Despite the fact that self-compassion may have the potential to address adolescents’ emotional needs, as of yet there has been only one study that has explored self-compassion with this age group (Neff & McGehee, 2010). In this study, self-compassion was reported to correlate negatively with depression and anxiety, and positively with connectedness (a construct which articulates how strongly an individual feels connected to others). Additionally, it was found to partially mediate the relationship between family functioning variables (i.e., maternal support, attachment) and well-being (i.e., depression, anxiety, connectedness). Mindfulness was not measured in this study.

Thus, although there is some conceptual overlap between mindfulness and self-compassion, in that mindfulness includes a component of acceptance and self-compassion includes a component of maintaining balanced awareness when confronting challenges, they are fundamentally different in theory and applicability. Mindfulness, particularly as it was measured in this study, focuses on present moment awareness and responding to thoughts and feelings without judgment (Greco, Baer, & Smith, 2011). Self-compassion, on the other hand, focuses on the actions utilized when encountering suffering; it entails the active component of engaging in self-soothing behavior (Germer, 2009; Neff & Pommier, 2013). Whereas mindfulness involves bringing attention and awareness to any moment with equanimity and balance, self-compassion is applicable specifically in moments of suffering. Mindfulness practice encourages one to feel one's pain (along with other sentiments including joy); self-compassion addresses not only soothing this pain, but recognizing that it is part of the human experience (Neff, 2003b). Finally, mindfulness focuses on one's relationship with thoughts, feelings and sensations in the present moment; self-compassion specifically focuses on one's relationship with oneself (Baer et al., 2012). Due to these nuanced differences, recent studies consider and measure mindfulness and self-compassion as two distinct constructs (Baer, Lykins, & Peters, 2012; Keng, Smoski, Robins, Ekblad & Brantley, 2011; Kuyken et al., 2010; Robins, Keng, Ekblad, Brantley, 2012; Van Dam, Sheppard, Forsyth, & Earleywine, 2011).

Recognizing that mindfulness brings awareness to one's suffering and that self-compassion addresses and ameliorates that suffering, one would expect that as one becomes more mindful and aware of her suffering, they would initiate self-compassionate and self-comforting behavior. Thus, in this exploratory study of the relationships between the variables of mindfulness, self-compassion, and dimensions of emotional well-being, we posited that mediated through self-compassion, as defined by Neff (2003b), mindfulness will be linked to dimensions of emotional well-being in adolescents.

Method

Sample

Participants were recruited from a population of 9-12 grade high school students in one public high school in the southeast United States. To be able to generalize results across different socioeconomic groups, a school with a diverse socioeconomic student population served as the target school. The racial/ethnic composition of the students was 67.3% White, 25.3% African American, 1.3% Asian/Pacific Islander, 5.7% Hispanic, 0.4% Native American/Alaskan. In addition, 44.2% of students attending the school were classified as economically disadvantaged, based on whether they were receiving free or reduced lunch. Males comprised 50.8% of the student population, and females comprised 49.2%. The sample population of this study was somewhat less racially/ethnically diverse than the population of the school, and proportionally there were more females who participated in the study than males (see Table 1). Permission was requested from the school district through completing and submitting a district required proposal. Their approval of the project stipulated, however, that no class time could be used to administer the questionnaire. All students in the school were eligible to participate in this study.

Table 1.

Descriptive Data for Demographics of Sample (N = 65)

Variables Percentage of total
Gender
    male 41.8
    female 58.2
Age
    14-15 40.3
    16-18 59.7
Education level of parents Mother Father
    Less than high school 3.0 3.0
    High school graduate 10.4 14.9
    Some college 17.9 10.4
    College degree 29.9 34.3
    Master's degree 25.4 13.4
    Doctorate or professional degree 13.4 20.9
    No answer 3.0
Race/Ethnicity
    Black 11.9
    White 73.1
    Asian 1.5
    American Indian 1.5
    Hispanic/Latino 3.0
    Other 9.0
Number of computers in household
    0-1 15.2
    2-3 50.7
    4-5 22.4
    6 or more 10.5
Number of vacations in last year
    0 17.9
    1 29.9
    2 26.9
    3-4 23.9
    No answer 1.5
Number of cars in household
    0 1.5
    1 10.4
    2 32.8
    3 40.3
    4 10.4
    5-7 4.5

Note. Two participants did not provide descriptive data.

Procedure

A packet was handed out to all 1201 students in their English classes, which included a letter of introduction explaining the study, a consent form for parents and an assent form for students. English class was chosen as the venue to distribute packets because all students in the school attended an English class. As an incentive to encourage students to participate in the study, all student participants were entered in a drawing for an Ipad 2 and provided with a free cookie coupon. On the day that students took home the packet, a message was sent through the school's email/phone message system alerting parents of the opportunity for their child to participate, and an announcement was made over the school intercom system. Over the week, several additional announcements were made over the school intercom system, reminders were sent to parents through the email/phone call system, and 200 flyers were posted at the school advertising the study.

Prior to the week when the online survey would be available, 63 students had submitted consent and assent forms. Of these, 34 did not come to the school library to take the survey. When contacted, 13 of these students took the survey the following week. In addition, 24 additional students brought in consent forms during the week of data collection and were permitted to take the survey. In total, 89 students, or 7.41% of all students invited provided consent and assent forms, and 67 students, or 5.66% took the online survey.

Data Collection

The following measures comprised the survey that students took online.

Mindfulness

Children and Adolescent Mindfulness Measure (CAMM; Greco et al., 2011). This measure assesses mindfulness skills, which include both attention in the moment and acceptance of one's internal experiences. Factor analysis of this 10-item scale resulted in a one factor solution with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.80. Participants indicate their responses to each item using a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (never true) to 4 (always true). Examples of items on this scale include: “It's hard for me to pay attention to only one thing at a time” and “I tell myself that I shouldn't feel the way I'm feeling.” Construct validity was established through positive correlations of the scores from this measure with quality of life, academic competence, and social skills and negative correlations with somatic complaints and internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (Greco et al., 2011).

Positive and negative affect

Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS; Watson, Clark & Tellegen, 1988). The PANAS is comprised of two subscales: positive affect and negative affect. Positive affect is defined as “the extent to which a person feels enthusiastic, active, and alert” (Watson et al., 1988, p. 1063). Negative affect is defined as “a general dimension of subjective distress and unpleasurable engagement that subsumes a variety of aversive mood states, that includes anger, contempt, disgust, guilt, fear and nervousness” (Watson, et al., 1988, p. 1063). This scale contains 10 emotion words that assess positive emotions and 10 words that assess negative emotions. Examples of positive emotion words are strong, inspired, and excited. Examples of negative emotion words are ashamed, upset, and afraid. The participant is asked to indicate how much he or she has experienced each of these emotions over the past few days. Participants indicate their responses to each item using a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 4 (most of the time). Higher scores for PA indicate higher positive affect, and higher scores for NA indicate higher negative affect. The two subscales have been shown to have low correlation with each other (r = −.22), are internally consistent (Cronbach's alpha = .84 to .87 for negative affect, and .86 to .90 for positive affect) and stable over a 2-month time period (r = .48 for PA, r = .42 for NA) (Watson et al., 1988). In addition, past research demonstrates evidence for convergent and discriminant validity (Watson et al., 1988).

Self-Compassion

Self-Compassion Scale (SCS; Neff, 2003a). Self-compassion is defined as “the ability to hold one's feelings of suffering with a sense of warmth, connection, and concern” (Neff & McGehee, 2010, p. 226). The 6 subscales that comprise the 26-item self-compassion scale are self-kindness (5 items, e.g. “When I'm going through a very hard time, I give myself the caring and tenderness I need”); self-judgment (5 items, e.g. “I'm disapproving and judgmental about my own flaws and inadequacies”); common humanity (4 items, e.g. “When I feel inadequate in some way, I try to remind myself that feelings of inadequacy are shared by most people”); isolation (4 items, e.g. “When I fail at something that's important to me, I tend to feel alone in my failure”); mindfulness (4 items, e.g. “When something upsets me I try to keep my emotions in balance”) and over-identification (4 items, e.g. “When I'm feeling down I tend to obsess and fixate on everything that's wrong”). Participants indicate their responses to each item using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (Almost Never) to 5 (Almost Always). Higher score indicates greater self-compassion. Reliability for this scale is excellent; Cronbach's alpha = .93 (Neff, 2003a). To establish construct validity, the self-compassion scale was compared to similar established scales, and was reported to have a statistically significant negative correlation with the self-criticism subscale of the Depression Experience Questionnaire, a statistically significant positive correlation with the Social Connectedness scale, and statistically significant positive correlations with the three subscales of the Trait-Meta Mood Scale which include attention, clarity, and repair (Neff, 2003a).

Life satisfaction

Student's Life Satisfaction Scale (SLSS; Huebner, 1991). Global life satisfaction, a component of subjective well-being, refers to a judgment about one's well-being that is beyond that which is linked directly to well-being in specific domains (e.g. school, peers). Examples of items include “I have a good life” and “There are many things that I would like to change about my life”. Participants indicated their responses to each item using a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (never) to 3 (almost always). Higher scores indicate greater life satisfaction. Based on Diener and colleagues (1985) life satisfaction scale for adults, this scale has been validated for children age 8-14. Construct and discriminant validity were established through correlations with other well-being and affect scales, and comparisons to the correlations between similar scales in adults. Results were in the expected directions. The 7-item scale has a unidimensional factor structure, adequate temporal stability over 1-2 weeks (correlation = 0.74 with student samples from grades 4, 6, and 8), and good internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = 0.82 with student samples from grades 4, 5, 6, and 8) (Huebner, 1991). Further validation, internal consistency and test-retest reliability over one year was established in a later study with a sample of 9th, 10th, 11th, and 12th graders (Huebner, Funk, & Gilman, 2000). Coefficient alphas of .86 (Dew & Huebner, 1994) and .84 (Gilman & Huebner, 1997) were reported in additional studies with adolescent samples. Concurrent validity was evidenced in parent reports (Dew & Huebner, 1994; Gilman & Huebner, 1997) and teacher reports (Huebner & Alderman, 1993).

Perceived Stress

(PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). PSS was measured using a 14-item scale that is designed to assess the degree to which respondents find their lives “unpredictable, uncontrollable, and overloading” (Cohen et al., 1983, p. 387). Theoretically, it reflects cognitive reappraisal theory (Lazarus, 1977), which emphasizes that it is the individual's personal and contextual appraisal of the event that is the chief determining factor in the resulting level of stress, rather than the nature of the objective event itself. Examples of items include, “In the last month, how often have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly?” and “In the last month, how often have you felt nervous or stressed?” Participants indicated their responses to each item using a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Content, predictive, and concurrent validity were established by Cohen and colleagues (1983) in a study of two college samples and one community sample. Construct validity for using this scale with adolescents was established in a study of adolescent psychiatric inpatients (Martin, Kazarian, & Breiter, 1995). In the latter study, a factor analysis of this scale produced two factors, one which was identified as perceived distress and the other which was identified as perceived coping. Cronbach's alpha for this adolescent sample was .86, which was consistent with that found in Cohen and colleagues (1983) college and community samples, and also with another study with early adolescents, which revealed a Cronbach's alpha of .88 (Yarcheski & Mahon, 1999).

In addition to these measures, an 8-item questionnaire of demographic variables was included. This asked about the participant's age, gender, race/ethnicity and factors related to socioeconomic status.

Results

Initial Analyses

Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach's alphas

Means and standard deviations were computed for all scale scores. Normality was investigated, and results indicated that all scales were normally distributed and means were similar to those reported in previous studies (Cohen et al., 1983; Greco et al., 2011; Huebner, 1991; Huebner & Dew, 1995; Huebner et al., 2000; Mahon & Yarcheski, 2007; Neff & McGehee, 2010; Van Dam, Sheppard, Forsyth, & Earleywine, 2011; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988). Cronbach's alphas for all scales were also calculated and found to be similar to those reported in previous studies (see Table 2).

Table 2.

Means, standard deviations, and Cronbach's α for all scales, n = 67

Scale Mean (SD) Cronbach's α
CAMM 22.95 (7.40) .87
SCS 2.98 (0.54) .83
PA 36.05 (6.40) .85
NA 25.92 (8.89) .89
SLSS 2.82(0.75) .89
PSS 28.99 (8.01) .79

Note. CAMM = Children and Adolescent Mindfulness Measure, SCS = Self-compassion Scale, PA = Positive Affect, NA = Negative Affect, SLSS = Student Life Satisfaction Scale, PSS = Perceived Stress Scale, * = Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed); ** = Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed); *** = Correlation is at the .001 level (2-tailed).

Bivariate correlations and VIF values

Bivariate correlations were computed for all scale scores to determine the relationships between variables. With the exception of the relationships between positive affect and self-compassion, and between positive and negative affect, all bivariate correlations were statistically significant and in the expected direction (see Table 3). Furthermore, to confirm statistically that self-compassion and mindfulness are in fact, separate constructs, the VIF value was calculated (VIF = 1.54).

Table 3.

Bivariate Correlations and Confidence Intervals, N = 67

SCS PA NA SLSS PSS
CAMM .59*** .26* −.61*** .67*** −.61***
SCS .22 −.64*** .52*** −.70***
PA −.24 .43*** −.30*
NA −.63*** .76***
SLSS −.69***

Note. CAMM = Children and Adolescent Mindfulness Measure, SCS = Self-compassion Scale, PA = Positive Affect, NA = Negative Affect, SLSS = Student Life Satisfaction Scale, PSS = Perceived Stress Scale

*

Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)

** = Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)

***

Correlation is at the .001 level (2-tailed).

Mediation Analyses

To determine mediation, four conditions must be met (Baron & Kenny, 1986). First, the independent variable (X) must affect the dependent variable (Y); we will call this parameter “c”. Second, the independent variable (X) must affect the mediator variable (M); we will call this parameter “a”. Third, the mediator variable (M) must affect the dependent variable (Y) when controlling for the independent variable (X), this parameter is labeled “b”. Last, when controlling for the mediator (M), the effect of the independent variable (X) on the dependent variable (Y), labeled as “c′”, is eliminated in full mediation (Baron & Kenny, 1986) and reduced in partial mediation (MacKinnon, 2008). In other words, c′ must be less than c (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Diagram of a direct effect (X →Y) and a mediated effect (X →M → Y).

The mediated effect is then calculated as the difference between the direct effect when no mediation is present and the case when mediation is controlled, i.e. c-c′. In this study, a path analysis model was used to estimate the effects of the predictor on the mediator and the mediator on each of the four dependent variables. Four separate models were created to estimate the parameters of the mediator, self-compassion, on the four separate dependent variables.

The procedure to estimate parameter weights was conducted using Mplus software (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2010) with maximum liklihood (ML) as the estimator for all dependent variables. First, the direct path was estimated using the direct model (c), and then the direct path (as well as other paths) was estimated in the mediated model (c′). The difference between these two direct path weights is c - c′, or the estimation of the mediated effect. The statistical significance of the mediated effect was then tested with a bootstrapping procedure. Bootstrapping is a non-parametric method which uses re-sampling with replacement to establish an estimate of a statistic, in this case, the indirect effect (Kline, 2011; MacKinnon, 2008). The bootstrapping method is recommended for determining significance of mediation when utilizing small to medium-sized samples (MacKinnon, Lockwood, & Williams, 2004; Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). Since the four mediated models are just-identified, it was not possible to provide indices of model fit (Mulaik et al., 1989).

It is important to note that because the data are cross-sectional and therefore temporally ambiguous, we cannot conclusively assert the temporal order of variables and determine the direction of influence. In order to do so, one must measure the variables at three separate points in time. However, to explore more extensively the posited mediational role of self-compassion, the mindfulness variable and self-compassion variable were switched and this alternate model also was analyzed.

A priori model: self-compassion as mediator

Mindfulness was entered as the independent variable and self-compassion was entered as the potential mediator and each of the four dependent variables were entered in succession in separate analyses (Figure 2). Results indicated that the path from mediator (self-compassion) to positive affect was non-significant, and therefore this model could not be considered for mediation because it did not meet the criteria established by Baron and Kenny (1986). However, when the decrease in magnitude of the mediated path with negative affect as the dependent variable was tested, bootstrapping analysis determined that this decrease was significant, indicating partial mediation (indirect = –0.03, p < .01, 95% CI [−0.05, −0.01]). When life satisfaction was entered as the dependent variable, the path from self-compassion to life satisfaction was not significant, so no mediation could be tested. When perceived stress was introduced as the dependent variable, however, bootstrapping results indicated a significant mediated effect (indirect = −0.327, p < .001, 95% CI [−0.50, −0.15]). The path weights associated with these results are provided in Table 4.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Direct and mediated model with self-compassion as mediator

Table 4.

A Priori Model with Self-compassion as Mediator

Direct Model Mediated model

DV CAMM→DV CAMM →DV CAMM → SCS SCS → DV
PA .26* .20 (ns) .59** .10 (ns)

NA −.61** −.36** .59** −.42*

SLSS .67** .56*** .59** .20 (ns)

PSS −.61*** −.31*** .59** −.52***

Note. CAMM = Children and Adolescent Mindfulness Measure, SCS = Self-compassion Scale, PA = Positive Affect, NA = Negative Affect, SLSS = Student Life Satisfaction Scale, PSS = Perceived Stress Scale

*

Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)

**

Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)

***

Correlation is at the .001 level (2-tailed).

Alternate model: mindfulness as mediator

Mediation was then examined in the alternate model where self-compassion was the independent variable and mindfulness was the mediator (Figure 3). The paths from mindfulness to positive affect were non-significant, and therefore no mediation could be tested with these samples (Baron & Kenny, 1986).When negative affect was entered as the dependent variable, bootstrapping results demonstrated partial mediation (indirect = –0.21, p < .01, 95% CI [−0.34, −0.05]). Additionally, the bootstrapping procedure indicated that mindfulness fully mediated the path from self-compassion to life satisfaction (indirect = –.33, p < .01, 95% CI [0.18, 0.47]). When perceived stress was introduced as the dependent variable, bootstrapping results indicated partial mediation (indirect = −0.18, p < .01, 95% CI [−0.30, −0.06]). The path weights associated with the mediational analyses of the alternate model are provided in Table 5.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Direct and mediated model with mindfulness as mediator

Table 5.

Alternate Model with Mindfulness as Mediator

Direct model Mediated Model

DV SCS →DV SCS →DV SCS → CAMM CAMM → DV
PA .22 (ns) .10 (ns) .59*** .20 (ns)

NA −.64*** −.42*** .59*** −.36**

SLSS .52*** .19 (ns) .59*** .56***

PSS −.70*** −.52*** .59*** −.31**

Note. CAMM = Children and Adolescent Mindfulness Measure, SCS = Self-compassion Scale, PA = Positive Affect, NA = Negative Affect, SLSS = Student Life Satisfaction Scale, PSS = Perceived Stress Scale

* = Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed)

**

Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed)

***

Correlation is at the .001 level (2-tailed).

Discussion

In recent studies, self-compassion has been reported to be a potentially influential mediator for the relationship between mindfulness and well-being for adult populations (Baer, Lykins, & Peters, 2012; Keng, Smoski, Robins, Ekblad & Brantley, 2011; Kuyken et al., 2010). Additionally, Neff (2003b) suggested that self-compassion may be particularly instrumental in impacting well-being in adolescents because of adolescents’ tendencies toward negative self-judgments (Harter, 1993; Jacobs et al., 2002), and found this to be the case in one study in which mindfulness was not measured (Neff & McGehee, 2010). Recognizing the limitations of a cross-sectional study in investigating mediation, the purpose of this study was to explore the potential role of self-compassion as a mediator in the relationship between mindfulness and emotional well-being outcomes in an adolescent population.

To definitively establish mediation, it is essential to measure variables at three separate time points, with the posited mediator assessed at the middle time point (MacKinnon, 2008). However, as a preliminary exploration of these relationships, cross-sectional data can be used, with the understanding that a follow-up longitudinal study would be the next step. In either case, it is first essential to ascertain a significant relationship between the predictor and outcome variables (Baron & Kenny, 1986). Initial inspection of the correlation matrix indicated that there was a statistically significant relationship in the expected direction between mindfulness (the predictor) and each of the four well-being measures (outcome variables).

The second step to investigate mediation is to determine significance between the predictor (mindfulness) and the mediator (self-compassion) and between the mediator (self-compassion) and the outcome (dimensions of emotional well-being). As expected, there was a statistically significant association between self-compassion and mindfulness. In addition, self-compassion was associated in the expected directions with life satisfaction, negative affect and perceived stress, but not with positive affect.

A priori model

When self-compassion was investigated as a mediator, it partially explained the relationship between mindfulness and both negative affect and perceived stress. Being more attentive, aware, and accepting of that which one is facing in the moment can allow adolescents to become kinder and less critical of themselves. It is conceivable that when adolescents become increasingly aware of their thoughts, they can recognize the degree to which these thoughts are self-critical and harmful, and therefore take steps to treat themselves with greater kindness. This ultimately can lead to improved emotional well-being.

Interestingly, the outcomes that are significantly mediated by self-compassion are the two that are constructs with negative connotations, i.e. negative affect and perceived stress. It appeared that in this adolescent sample, being kinder and more accepting of oneself was associated more with a decrease in negative outcomes than an increase in positive outcomes. This can be attributed to either a genuine greater effect of self-compassion on ameliorating these negative outcomes, or because adolescents relate more acutely to the negative items on the scales, and therefore respond more assuredly.

Alternate Model

Due to the limitations of investigating mediation in the cross-sectional design of this study, an alternate model where self-compassion was the predictor and mindfulness was the mediator was explored. Analyses revealed that mindfulness did, in fact, partially mediate the relationship between self-compassion and negative affect and self-compassion and perceived stress, and fully mediate the relationship between self-compassion and life satisfaction.

Note that when conceptualizing the constructs as they are in the alternate model, the component of mindfulness that seems to emerge as most salient is that of acceptance rather than awareness. This finding is consistent with previous research which found that the acceptance subscale was the component of mindfulness that differed when comparing clinical to non-clinical samples (Cardaciotto et al., 2008). It is plausible that when adolescents are less self-critical and see themselves as part of a greater humanity, they are more able to accept themselves and their present situation, seeing their own shortcomings and momentary frustrations as part of a natural course, rather than an experience that is unique to them. This perspective fosters self-acceptance that can then lead to less stress and greater emotional well-being.

The exploration of mediation in this study suggests a possible reciprocal association and iterative dynamic between mindfulness and self-compassion in that the awareness and attention that mindfulness supports may allow one to become more aware of thoughts. This awareness may then leads to recognition of the degree to which one is self-judging and ruminating, or over-identifying with negative thoughts. This pattern may be particularly salient for adolescents who are frequently self-judgmental (Harter, 1993; Jacobs et al., 2002). This new awareness of the degree to which one is self-critical may then bring about a desire to be kinder to one's self and thus more self-compassionate. Through this, adolescents may then learn to be more accepting of themselves, understanding that their flaws do not have to define or overwhelm them. In fact, their imperfections may be seen as part of what makes them who they are, and as such, they are part of a common humanity of flawed individuals (Figure 4).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Proposed model of reciprocal association between mindfulness and self-compassion

How then might this interchange between mindfulness and self-compassion lead to increased emotional well-being? An acceptance of one's own imperfections and awareness that as part of being human, we are all flawed, may lead adolescents to have greater compassion for others’ imperfections, recognizing that if their own flaws are forgivable, then others’ flaws may be as well. Adolescents may be more willing to let go of potentially conflicting situations with friends, for example, thereby leading to less social pressure and stress among peers. In academic settings, a low test grade may be seen as a learning experience, an opportunity to acknowledge the need to study more, rather than a time to berate oneself for poor performance.

This decreased conflict with others and with oneself might lead to decreased stress and negative affect, and improved emotional functioning (Lazarus, 2006). Greater interpersonal connection can act as a buffer against a fear of social rejection (Collins, 1997) leading to an overall greater satisfaction with one's life. Items such as “My life is going well” and “I have a good life”, items from the life satisfaction scale, are more likely to be responded to in the affirmative if relationships among friends are more stable and one experiences less stress and conflict in day-to-day life. A proposed empirical model of the relationships among these variables is provided (Figure 5).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

New proposed model with reciprocal association between mindfulness and self-compassion

Although this study contributes to the limited literature on mindfulness, self-compassion and adolescents by proposing a reciprocal interaction between mindfulness and self-compassion, there are a number of limitations that should be noted. First, the sample size was small and self-selected. It is possible that the incentive of winning an iPad or engaging in a survey taking place before school, after school, or during lunch might appeal to certain types of students. As such, it is possible that this sample may not generalize to the adolescent population as a whole.

Second, the model with reciprocal association between mindfulness and self-compassion could not be tested because it was not identified. In order for a model to be identified that has two variables with a reciprocal relationship, one of the variables must have a predictor variable or the disturbance terms must be correlated (Kline, 2011). In this study, no predictor variable for either mindfulness or self-compassion was measured, and in path analysis, disturbance terms must remain uncorrelated.

Finally, all data for this study were collected at one point in time. Ideally, mediation studies should be conducted at three distinct time points. This allows for discernment of the impact of one variable on another, important when considering mediation (MacKinnon, 2008). This initial exploratory study suggests that relationships between these variables should be followed up with longitudinal studies that investigate these variables more conclusively. However, in that the relationships between mindfulness, self-compassion, and emotional well-being have not been studied in adolescents, this study offers a significant contribution to the literature in this field.

Conclusion

The adolescent period can be challenging, sometimes fraught with negative self-evaluations and self-judgments. In this study, it was hypothesized that mindfulness could help adolescents through this difficult developmental stage by providing a pathway to emotional well-being, and self-compassion was hypothesized to be a way in which mindfulness could achieve these results. Findings suggest that both mindfulness and self-compassion may function as mediators with emotional well-being, and it was theorized that the two constructs engage in a dynamic iterative process which may culminate in improved well-being in adolescents. With this knowledge, an intervention can be created that teaches adolescents to be both more mindful and self-compassionate, thereby potentially leading to improvements in their emotional health. Implications might include a decrease in youth maladaptive trajectories including substance abuse, youth violence, bullying behaviors, and school absenteeism.

Contributor Information

Karen Bluth, Program on Integrative Medicine, Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, CB #7200, The University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA.

Priscilla W. Blanton, Department of Child and Family Studies, University of Tennessee-Knoxville, 1215 W. Cumberland Ave., Knoxville, TN; pblanton@utk.edu

References

  1. Baer RA. Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice. 2003;10(2):125–143. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bpg015. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baer RA, Lykins ELB, Peters JR. Mindfulness and self-compassion as predictors of psychological wellbeing in long-term meditators and nonmeditators. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2012;7(3):230–238. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bailey M. Parenting your stressed child. New Harbinger Publications; Oakland, CA: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  4. Baron R, Kenny D. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51(6):1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bartsch H, Bartsch C, Simon WE, Flehmig B, Egels I, Lippert TH. Antitumor activity of the pineal gland: Effect of unidentified substances versus the effect of melatonin. Oncology. 1992;49:27–30. doi: 10.1159/000227005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Biegel G, Brown K, Shapiro S, Schubert C. Mindfulness-based stress reduction for the treatment of adolescent psychiatric outpatients: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology. 2009;77(5):855–866. doi: 10.1037/a0016241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson ND, Carmody J, Devins G. Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice. 2004;11(3):230–241. doi: 10.1093/clipsy/bph077. [Google Scholar]
  8. Boguls S, Sijbers GF, Voncken M. Mindfulness and task concentration training for social phobia: a pilot study. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly. 2006;20:33–44. [Google Scholar]
  9. Breines JG, Chen S. Self-compassion increases self-improvement motivation. [Comparative Study Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S.]. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2012;38(9):1133–1143. doi: 10.1177/0146167212445599. doi: 10.1177/0146167212445599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broderick P, Metz S. Learning to BREATHE: A pilot trial of a mindfulness curriculum for adolescents. Advances in school mental health promotion. 2009;2(1):35–46. [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown K, West A, Loverich T, Biegel G. Assessing adolescent mindfulness: Validation of an adapted mindfulness attention awareness scale in adolescent normative and psychiatric populations. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23(4):1023–1033. doi: 10.1037/a0021338. doi: 10.1037/a0021338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burke CA. Mindfulness-based approaches for children and adolescents: A preliminary review of current research in an emergent field. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2010;19(2):133–144. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cardaciotto L, Herbert J, Forman E, Moitra E, Farrow V. The assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance : The Philadelphia mindfulness scale. Assessment. 2008;15(204):204–223. doi: 10.1177/1073191107311467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carlson LE, Ursuliak Z, Goodey E, Angen M, Speca M. The effects of a mindfulness meditation based stress reduction program on mood and symptoms of stress in cancer outpatients: 6-month follow-up. Supportive Care in Cancer. 2001;9:112–123. doi: 10.1007/s005200000206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ciarrochi J, Kashdan TB, Leeson P, Heaven P, Jordan C. On being aware and accepting: A one-year longitudinal study into adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescence. 2010;34(4):695–703. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.09.003. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24(4):385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Collins WA. Relationships and development during adolescence: Interpersonaladaptation to individual change. Personal Relationships. 1997;4:1–14. [Google Scholar]
  18. Dew T, Huebner ES. Adolescents perceived quality of life: An exploratory investigation. Journal of School Psychology. 1994;32:185–199. [Google Scholar]
  19. Diener E, Emmons R, Larsen R, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1985;49:71–76. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fincune A, Mercer SW. An exploratory mixed methods study of the acceptability and effectiveness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for patients with active depression and anxiety in primary care. BMC Psychiatry. 2006;6(14):76–91. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-6-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Germer C. The mindful path to self-compassion: Freeing yourself from destructive thoughts and emotions. Guilford Press; New York: NY: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  22. Gilman R, Huebner ES. Children's reports of their life satisfaction: Convergence across raters, time, and response formats. School Psychology International. 1997;18:229–243. [Google Scholar]
  23. Greco L, Baer RA, Smith GT. Assessing mindfulness in children and adolescents: Development and validation of the child and adolescent mindfulness measure (CAMM). Psychological Assessment. 2011;23(3):606–614. doi: 10.1037/a0022819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Greeson JM. Mindfulness Research Update: 2008. Complementary Health Practice Review 2009. 2009;14(10):10–18. doi: 10.1177/1533210108329862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Grossman P. Mindfulness for psychologists: Paying kind attention to the perceptible. Mindfulness. 2010;1:87–97. [Google Scholar]
  26. Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits - A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2004;57(1):35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(03)00573-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Harter S. Causes and consequences of low self esteem in children and adolescents. In: Baumeister RG, editor. Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard. Plenum Press; New York: 1993. pp. 87–116. [Google Scholar]
  28. Heffernan M, Quinn Griffin MT, Sister Rita M, Fitzpatrick JJ. Self-compassion and emotional intelligence in nurses. International Journal of Nursing Practice. 2010;16(4):366–373. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-172X.2010.01853.x. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-172X.2010.01853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Huebner ES. Initial Development of the Student's Life Satisfaction Scale. School Psychology International. 1991;12(3):231–240. doi: 10.1177/0143034391123010. [Google Scholar]
  30. Huebner ES, Alderman G. Convergent and discriminant validity of a children's life satisfaction scale: Its relationship to self- and teacher reported psychological problems and school functioning. Social Indicators Research. 1993;46:1–22. [Google Scholar]
  31. Huebner ES, Dew T. Preliminary Validation of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule with Adolescents. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment. 1995;13(3):286–293. doi: 10.1177/073428299501300307. [Google Scholar]
  32. Huebner ES, Funk BA, Gilman R. Cross-sectional and Longitudinal Psychosocial Correlates of Adolescent Life Satisfaction Reports. Canadian Journal of School Psychology. 2000;16(1):53–64. doi: 10.1177/082957350001600104. [Google Scholar]
  33. Huppert F, Johnson D. A controlled trial of mindfulness training in schools: The importance of practice for an impact of well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology. 2010;5(4):264–274. [Google Scholar]
  34. Jacobs JE, Lanza S, Osgood D, Eccles JS, Wigfield A. Changes in children's self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development. 2002;73(2):509–527. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry. 1982;4:33–47. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kabat-Zinn J. Full Catastrophe Living. Random House; New York: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kabat-Zinn J, Lipworth L, Burney R. The clinical use of mindfulness meditation for the self-regulation of chronic pain. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1985;8(2):163–190. doi: 10.1007/BF00845519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kabat-Zinn J, Lipworth L, Burney R, Sellers W. Four-year follow-up of a meditation-based program for the self-regulation of chronic pain: Treatment outcoms and compliance. Clinical Journal of Pain. 1987;2:159–173. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kabat-Zinn J, Massion MD, Kristeller J, Peterson LG, Fletcher KE, Phert L, Santorelli S. Effectiveness of a meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;149:936–943. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.7.936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kutz I, Leserman J, Dorrington C, Morrison C, Borysenko J, Benson H. Meditation as an adjunct to psychotherapy. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 1985;43:209–218. doi: 10.1159/000287881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Keng S-L, Smoski MJ, Robins CJ. Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31(6):1041–1056. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3rd ed. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kuyken W, Watkins E, Holden E, White K, Taylor RS, Byford S, Dalgleish T. How does mindfulness-based cognitive therapy work? Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2010;48:1105–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.003. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lazarus R. Stress and emotion: A new synthesis. Springer; New York: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lazarus R. Psychological stress and coping in adaptation and illness. In: Lipowski Z, Lipsi D, Whybrow P, editors. Psychosomatic Medicine: Current Trends. Oxford University Press; New York: 1977. pp. 14–26. [Google Scholar]
  46. Leary MR, Tate EB, Adams CE, Allen AB, Hancock J. Self-compassion and reactions to unpleasant self-relevant events: the implications of treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92(5):887–904. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.887. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lykins E, Baer RA. Psychological functioning in a sample of long-term practitioners of mindfulness meditation. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2009;23:226–241. [Google Scholar]
  48. MacBeth A, Gumley A. Exploring compassion: a meta-analysis of the association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review. 2012;32(6):545–552. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.06.003. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. MacKinnon D. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; New York, NY: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  50. MacKinnon D, Lockwood C, Williams J. Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate and Behavioral Research. 2004;39:99–128. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mahon N, Yarcheski A. Relations of low frustration tolerance beliefs with stress, depression, and anxiety in young adolescents. Psychological Reports. 2007;100:98–100. doi: 10.2466/pr0.100.1.98-100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Martin R, Kazarian S, Breiter H. Perceived stress, life events, dysfunctional attitudes, and depression in adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 1995;17(1):81–95. [Google Scholar]
  53. Massion AO, Teas J, Hebert JR, Wertheimer MD, Kabat-Zinn J. Meditation, melatonin, and breast/prostate cancer: Hypothesis and preliminary data. Medical Hypotheses. 1995;44:39–46. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(95)90299-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Miller JJ, Fletcher KE, Kabat-Zinn J. Three-year follow-up and clinical implications of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction intervention in the treatment of anxiety disorders. General Hospital Psychiatry. 1995;17:192–200. doi: 10.1016/0163-8343(95)00025-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Monshat K, Khong B, Hassed C, Vella-Brodrick D, Norrish J, Burns J, Herrman H. “A Conscious Control Over Life and My Emotions”: Mindfulness Practice and Healthy Young People. A Qualitative Study. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2013;52(5):572–577. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.09.008. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Morone N, Greco C, Weiner D. Mindfulness meditation for the treatment of chronic low back pain in older adults: A randomized controlled pilot study. Pain. 2008;134:310–319. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.04.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mulaik S, James L, Van Alstine J, Bennett N, Lind S, Stilwell CD. Psychological Bulletin. 1989;105:430–445. [Google Scholar]
  58. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. MPlus User's Guide. 6th ed. Author; Los Angeles: 1998-2010. [Google Scholar]
  59. Neely ME, Schallert DL, Mohammed SS, Roberts RM, Chen Y-J. Self-kindness when facing stress: The role of self-compassion, goal regulation, and support in college students’ well-being. Motivation and Emotion. 2009;33(1):88–97. doi: 10.1007/s11031-008-9119-8. [Google Scholar]
  60. Neff K. The development and validation of a scale to measure self-compassion. Self and Identity. 2003a;2:223–250. doi: 10.1080/15298860390209035. [Google Scholar]
  61. Neff K. Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity. 2003b;2:85–101. [Google Scholar]
  62. Neff KD, Hsieh Y, Dejitterat K. Self-compassion, achievement goals, and coping with academic failure. Self and Identity. 2005;4:263–287. [Google Scholar]
  63. Neff KD, Kirkpatrick KL, Rude SS. Self-compassion and adaptive psychological functioning. Journal of Research in Personality. 2007;41(1):139–154. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.004. [Google Scholar]
  64. Neff K, McGehee P. Self-compassion and psychological resisitance among adolescents and young adults. Self and Identity. 2010;9:225–240. [Google Scholar]
  65. Neff K, Pommier E. The relationship between self-compassion and other-focused concern among college undergraduates, community adults, and practicing meditators. Self and Identity. 2013;12(2):160–176. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2011.649546. [Google Scholar]
  66. Neff K, Vonk R. Self-compassion versus global self-esteem: Two different ways of relating to oneself. Journal of Personality. 2008;77:23–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00537.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pradhan EK, Baumgarten M, Langenberg P, Handwerger B, Gilpin AK, Magyari T, Berman B. Effect of mindfulness-based stress reduction in rheumatoid arthritis patients. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 2007;57:1134–1142. doi: 10.1002/art.23010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Preacher K, Hayes A. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Robins CJ, Keng SL, Ekblad AG, Brantley JG. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on emotional experience and expression: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2012;68(1):117–131. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Salzberg S. Real happiness: The power of meditation. Workman Publishing; New York, NY: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  71. Schonert-Reichl KA, Lawlor MS. The effects of a mindfulness-based education program on pre- and early adolescents’ well-being and social and emotional competence. Mindfulness. 2010;1(3):137–151. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0011-8. [Google Scholar]
  72. Shapiro S. The integration of mindfulness and psychology. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2009;65(6):555–560. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Shrout P, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendation. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:422–445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Speca M, Carlson LE, Goodey E, Angen M. A randomized, wait-list controlled clinical trial: The effect of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction program on mood and symptoms of stress in cancer outpatients. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2000;62:613–622. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200009000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tan L, Martin G. Taming the Adolescent Mind: Preliminary report of a mindfulness-based psychological intervention for adolescents with clinical heterogeneous mental health diagnoses. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2013;18(2):300–312. doi: 10.1177/1359104512455182. doi: 10.1177/1359104512455182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Teasdale JD. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. In: Yiend J, editor. Cognition, emotion and psychopathology. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2004. pp. 270–289. [Google Scholar]
  77. Twohig M, Field C, Armstrong A, Dahl A. Acceptance and mindfulness as mechanisms of change in mindfulness-based interventions for children and adolescents. In: Baer RA, editor. Assessing mindfulness and acceptance processes in clients: Illuminating the theory and practice of change. New Harbinger Publications; Oakland: 2010. pp. 225–249. [Google Scholar]
  78. Van Dam NT, Sheppard SC, Forsyth JP, Earleywine M. Self-compassion is a better predictor than mindfulness of symptom severity and quality of life in mixed anxiety and depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2011;25(1):123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.08.011. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Watson T, Clark L, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Yarcheski A, Mahon N. The moderator-mediator role of social support in early adolescence. Western Journal of Nursing Research. 1999;21(5):685–698. doi: 10.1177/01939459922044126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zautra AJ, Davis MC, Reich JW, Nicassario P, Tennen H, Finan P, Irwin M. Comparison of cognitive behavioral and mindfulness meditation interventions on adaptation to rheumatoid arthritis for patients with and without history of recurrent depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76:408–421. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES