Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2014 Nov;42(8):1299–1311. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9886-5

The etiology of the association between child antisocial behavior and maternal negativity varies across aggressive and non-aggressive rule-breaking forms of antisocial behavior

Ashlea M Klahr 1, Kelly L Klump 1, S Alexandra Burt 1
PMCID: PMC4198501  NIHMSID: NIHMS603051  PMID: 24906982

Abstract

There is a robust association between negative parenting and child antisocial behavior problems. However, the etiology of this association remains unclear. Extant literature has reported strikingly different conclusions across studies, with some highlighting genetic mediation and others highlighting environmental mediation. One possible reason for these discrepancies across studies may be the failure to differentiate between aggressive and non-aggressive (rule-breaking) dimensions of childhood antisocial behavior, given their notably different etiologies and developmental trajectories (Burt, 2012). The current study sought to examine the phenotypic and etiologic associations of maternal negativity with aggressive and rule-breaking antisocial behavior, respectively. Participants included 824 mothers and their twin children between the ages of 6 and 10. Our results highlighted clear etiologic distinctions in the associations of aggression and rule-breaking with maternal negativity. Aggression was associated with maternal negativity via both genetic and environmental factors, whereas the association between non-aggressive rule-breaking and maternal negativity was entirely environmental in origin. These findings provide additional support for the presence of meaningful distinctions between aggressive and non-aggressive forms of antisocial behavior, and highlight the complex relationship between parenting and child outcome.

Keywords: negative parenting, antisocial behavior, aggression, rule-breaking, gene-environment correlation


Antisocial behavior in childhood is a costly public health problem that can have serious consequences (prison, professional underachievement) into adulthood (Alink & Egeland, 2013). Understanding the factors that contribute to antisocial behavior problems in childhood is thus an important goal, in order to inform targeted prevention and intervention strategies. Negative parenting, including verbal criticism, parent-child conflict, and harsh discipline, has been robustly associated with antisocial behavior problems in children and youth (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997; Dishion, Patterson, Stoolmiller, & Skinner, 1991; Kerr, Lopez, Olson, & Sameroff, 2004; Patterson & Fisher, 2002). A recent meta-analysis confirmed the association between negative parenting and delinquency, reporting medium effect sizes for both parental hostility and rejection. In addition, the authors indicated that 67% of children who experience high levels of negative parenting demonstrate high levels of antisocial behavior, compared to only 33% of youth who experience low levels of parental negativity (Hoeve et al., 2009). Critically, however, we have relatively little insight into the genetic and/or environmental factors underlying their association (Lysenko, Barker, & Jaffee, 2013). Multiple etiologic pathways have been proposed, with varying degrees of empirical support. These pathways are outlined in more detail below.

Pathway 1: Environmental Mediation

The first possibility is that negative parenting is associated with child antisocial behavior via environmentally-mediated pathways, such that negative parenting causes an increase in children’s antisocial behaviors. Nix and colleagues, for example, found that mother’s harsh discipline of their preschool children predicted children’s externalizing problems up through 3rd grade, even after controlling for the children’s initial level of externalizing problems (Nix et al., 1999). Several twin studies also lend support to the notion that the association between parenting and antisocial behavior is shared environmental in origin (Burt, Krueger, McGue, & Iacono, 2003; Burt, McGue, Krueger, & Iacono, 2005). In addition, a handful of adoption studies have implicated environmentally-driven effects (an important set of constructive replications given that the confound of shared genes is entirely circumvented in non-biologically related families; Burt, McGue, Krueger, & Iacono, 2007; Elam et al., 2013; Klahr, McGue, Iacono, & Burt, 2011; Klahr, Rueter, McGue, Iacono, & Burt, 2011). Klahr and colleagues, for example, examined the association between parent-child conflict and conduct problems over time in a longitudinal sample of adoptive families and found that parent-child conflict predicted conduct problems 4 years later, but not the reverse. Such findings are clearly suggestive of an environmental or “causal” association between parenting and child antisocial behavior (Klahr, McGue, et al., 2011).

Pathway 2: Genetic Mediation

Prior research has confirmed that both childhood antisocial behavior (Burt, 2009) and negative parenting (Klahr & Burt, 2014) are heritable. Given this, it is possible that common genes account for their association. This genetic overlap could take one (or more) forms. The first of these is passive gene-environment correlation (passive rGE; Plomin, DeFries, & Loehlin, 1977; Scarr & McCartney, 1983), whereby a given set of genes predispose individuals to both negative parenting behaviors and to externalizing problems. More specifically, to the extent that negative parenting is a function of the parent’s own tendency towards externalizing problems, a tendency passed on to the child, the association between negative parenting and child antisocial behavior problems could be a reflection of common genes. Available data has provided some support for passive rGE effects in the association between overall family functioning and child antisocial behavior (Braungart-Rieker, Rende, Plomin, DeFries, & Fulker, 1995; McGue, Sharma, & Benson, 1996). Namely, McGue, et al. (1996) found that the association between family functioning and adolescent externalizing problems was greater in biological than adoptive families. Importantly, however, this study examined overall family functioning rather than parenting; it is thus unclear whether these findings apply to parenting more specifically. Indeed, available studies examining this question indicate that the effects of passive rGE on the association between parenting and child antisocial behavior are small to non-existent (Harold et al., 2013; Marceau et al., 2013; Narusyte et al., 2011b). For example, Marceau and colleagues (2013) modeled passive rGE effects (along with evocative rGE and environmental pathways) underlying the association between negative parenting and adolescent externalizing problems using the Extended Children of Twins model (Narusyte et al., 2008). They found no evidence for passive rGE effects. Rather, their results highlighted the role of evocative rGE, as described below.

Another possible form of genetic mediation is evocative rGE, in which an individual’s genetically-influenced characteristics evoke particular environmental experiences or responses (Plomin et al., 1977; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). In this case, children exhibiting externalizing behavior would elicit negative parenting consistent with their genetic proclivities towards externalizing (Klahr, Thomas, Hopwood, Klump, & Burt, 2013). Consistent with this possibility, research suggests that the characteristics of children are powerful drivers of parenting behavior (Anderson, Lytton, & Romney, 1986; Cunningham & Barkley, 1979; Karraker & Coleman, 2005). Moreover, a number of studies have demonstrated that genetic influences on child behavior problems and parenting at least partially overlap (Alemany, Rijsdijk, Haworth, Fañanás, & Plomin, 2013; Button, Lau, Maughan, & Eley, 2008; Marceau et al., 2013; Narusyte et al., 2011b), results that are typically interpreted as evidence of evocative rGE. For example, Pike and colleagues examined the etiology of the association between maternal and paternal negativity and child antisocial behavior in a sample of over 700 adolescent sibling pairs (including twins, full siblings, half siblings, and unrelated siblings). Their results suggested that the overlap between parenting and antisocial behavior was primarily mediated by genetic (i.e., evocative rGE) factors (Pike, McGuire, Hetherington, Reiss, & Plomin, 1996). In addition, research with adoptees has found that children (O’Connor, Deater-Deckard, Fulker, Rutter, & Plomin, 1998) and adolescents (Ge et al., 1996) at genetic risk for antisocial behavior (as indexed by birth mother self-reported antisocial behavior) were more likely to receive negative parenting from their adoptive parents. Finally, genetically informed cross-lagged longitudinal studies have highlighted the role of both evocative and environmental effects (Larsson, Viding, Rijsdijk, & Plomin, 2008; Tuvblad, Bezdjian, Raine, & Baker, 2013).

In sum, available studies have most strongly supported evocative rGE and/or shared environmental origins to the association between negative parenting and antisocial behavior. These processes are not necessarily mutually exclusive, particularly as there is clear evidence for bidirectional effects between child behavior and parenting from a large number of longitudinal examinations (e.g., Belsky, Pasco Fearon, & Bell, 2007; Lysenko et al., 2013; Miner & Clarke-Stewart, 2008; Stice & Barrera, 1994). Nevertheless, studies do differ rather dramatically on the extent to which they support evocative rGE or environmental mediation. What might explain these differences across studies? Possibilities include specific methodological factors (e.g., twin vs. adoption study, informant-reports), developmental processes (e.g., differences across early childhood, middle childhood, and adolescence), and/or sex differences (e.g., mothers vs. fathers or male vs. female children; Leve, Kim, & Pears, 2005; Meier, Slutske, Heath, & Martin, 2009; Narusyte et al., 2011a).

Critically, however, prior studies have also failed to distinguish between different dimensions of antisocial behavior. As reviewed in Burt (2012), there is now substantial evidence that aggression (e.g., fighting, hitting, threatening) and rule-breaking (i.e., nonaggressive delinquent acts such as stealing, vandalism, truancy, lying) are distinguishable but related dimensions of antisocial behavior characterized by distinct etiologies, developmental trajectories, and phenotypic correlates. For example, aggression demonstrates higher rank-order stability than does rule-breaking (Stanger, Achenbach, & Verhulst, 1997; Tremblay, 2003) and appears to be a particularly heritable form of antisocial behavior (65% of the variance in aggression is genetic in origin, as compared to 48% in rule-breaking; Burt, 2009). Moreover, only 38% of the genetic factors influencing aggression also influence rule-breaking (Burt, 2013), collectively indicating that the genetic architecture of aggression differs to a large extent from that of rule-breaking. Genetic influences on aggression also appear to remain stable across childhood and adolescence while genetic influences on rule-breaking increase substantially over adolescence (Burt & Klump, 2009; Burt & Neiderhiser, 2009). Aggression is also more strongly associated with hostile perceptions of others (Burt, Mikolajewski, & Larson, 2009) and the tendency to experience negative emotions (Burt & Donnellan, 2008; Burt & Larson, 2007). Rule-breaking, by contrast, is more strongly associated with impulsivity (Burt & Donnellan, 2008).

Given the many developmental and etiologic differences between the aggressive and rule-breaking dimensions of antisocial behavior, it is quite possible that the two dimensions are differentially associated with negative parenting, at least at an etiologic level. To our knowledge, however, no study to date has examined this possibility. In order to address this question, the current study examined the association of maternal negativity with aggression and rule-breaking, respectively, in a sample of over 800 families with school-aged twins. This design allowed us to simultaneously examine the roles of evocative rGE and environmental mediation in these associations. Because of the child-based nature of this research design, etiological influences are inferred based on differences in the genetic relatedness at the level of the child (i.e., those who are being parented, rather than those who are parenting). Estimates of genetic influences within this design are thus estimates of the effect of the child's genetic makeup on the behavior of their mother, an effect that is presumably driven via evocative rGE processes (as the child’s genes cannot directly influence the behavior of others; Klahr & Burt, 2014). A significant association between genetic influences on parenting and genetic influences on aggression or rule-breaking therefore suggests an evocative rGE effect of child antisocial behavior on parenting.

By contrast, shared environmental influences within a child-based design should capture those influences on parenting behavior that are shared between the twin children and which act to increase similarity in the parenting that they receive, regardless of their degree of genetic relatedness (Klahr & Burt, 2014). These shared environmental influences are thought to include such factors as family socioeconomic status, neighborhood characteristics, and importantly, characteristics of the parent (i.e., personality and beliefs about parenting). Significant overlap of shared environmental influences between parenting and child antisocial behavior may thus index a causal effect of parenting (a between-family effect, such that higher levels of negative parenting are associated with higher levels of child antisocial behavior, regardless of the genetic relatedness of the twins). Finally, significant non-shared environmental overlap is consistent with a potentially causal association between differential negative parenting and child antisocial behavior (a within-family effect; e.g., one twin is treated more negatively than the other and demonstrates higher levels of antisocial behavior as a result). However, it may also index other non-shared factors that influence both parenting and child behavior (e.g., one child is part of a more deviant peer group that influences the child’s antisocial behavior and evokes differential negativity). Table 1 summarizes the possible processes underlying the association between parenting and child antisocial behavior and how these possibilities are indexed within our current design.

Table 1.

Potential Etiologic Processes Underlying the Association between Parenting and Child Antisocial Behavior

Association Type Explanation In Our Analyses
Environmental Negative parenting increases child antisocial
behavior or other environmental factors (e.g.,
community violence) impact both parenting and
child behavior.
Significant shared environmental correlation
between negative parenting and antisocial
behavior.
Passive rGE The same genes that contribute to negative
parenting also contribute to parent and child
antisocial behavior.
Because all children, including MZ twins, share
50% of their genes with each biological parent,
passive rGE cannot be examined with any
certainty in child-based designs using biologically
related family members (including those in our
sample).
Evocative rGE Genetically-influenced child antisocial behavior
evokes negative parenting.
Significant genetic correlation between parenting
and antisocial behavior.

Methods

Participants

Participants for this study were drawn from the population-based Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR), which includes several independent twin projects (Burt & Klump, 2013; Klump & Burt, 2006). The current study included 824 twin families who were assessed as part of the on-going Twin Study of Behavioral and Emotional Development in Children (TBED-C) within the MSUTR. The TBED-C includes both a completed population-based sample (n=500 families; 497 biological mothers and one biological grandmother) and an independent at-risk sample for which inclusion criteria also specified that participating twin families lived in moderately to severely disadvantaged neighborhoods (current n=324 families; 297 biological mothers, one step-mother, and one biological grandmother). Conclusions are essentially unchanged with and without the at-risk sample and thus these families were retained for analysis. Assessments took place either in our laboratory at Michigan State University or in participant’s homes (in the event that families were unable to travel to the university; n=38 families from the population-based sample and 62 at-risk families). Children provided informed assent, while parents provided informed consent for themselves and their children. The twins ranged in age from 6 to 10 years (M = 8.16, SD = 1.46; although a few twins had turned 11 by the time they participated) and were 47.6% female. To be eligible for participation in the TBED-C, neither twin could have a cognitive or physical condition (as assessed via parental screen; e.g., a significant developmental delay) that would preclude completion of the assessment.

Families for both samples were recruited via State of Michigan birth records in collaboration with the Michigan Department of Community Health (MDCH). The MDCH manages birth records in Michigan, and is therefore able to identify all twin births in the state. MDCH identified twins living within 120 miles of East Lansing who were between the ages of 6 and 10 years. Twins were identified either directly from birth records or via the Michigan Twins Project, a large-scale population-based registry of twins in lower Michigan that were themselves recruited via birth records. MDCH then utilized the Michigan Bureau of Integration, Information, and Planning Services database to locate current addresses through parent drivers’ license information. Using these addresses, MDCH mailed pre-made recruitment packets to parents of twins. A reply postcard was included for parents to indicate their interest in participating. Interested families were then contacted directly by project staff. Parents who did not respond to the first mailing were sent additional mailings approximately one month apart until either a reply was received or up to four letters had been mailed.

This recruitment strategy yielded an overall response rate of 62% for the population-based sample and 56% for the at-risk sample, which is similar to or better than those of other twin registries that use anonymous recruitment mailings (Baker, Barton, & Raine, 2002; Hay, McStephen, Levy, & Pearsall-Jones, 2002). Importantly, as detailed in Table 1 of Burt & Klump (2013), the final population-based sample was broadly representative of the area population and of recruited families more specifically (as assessed via a brief questionnaire screen administered to 70% of non-participating families). Participating twins did not differ from non-participating twins in their average levels of conduct problems, emotional symptoms, and hyperactivity as assessed via the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman & Scott, 1999; Cohen’s d standardized effect sizes = −.05, .01, and −.08, respectively; all p ≥ .29). Participating families also did not differ from non-participating families on most demographic characteristics (as detailed in Table 1 of Burt & Klump, 2013).

Participating population-based families endorsed ethnic group memberships at rates comparable to area inhabitants (e.g., Caucasian: 86.4% and 85.5%, African-American: 5.4% and 6.3% for the participating families and the local census, respectively). As anticipated, however, the at-risk sample was significantly more racially diverse (15% African American, 75% Caucasian) than was the population-based sample. The at-risk sample also reported lower household incomes (d = .30, p <.01), lower availability of neighborhood resources (d = .20, p < .05), and a higher number of neighborhood problems (d = .37, p < .01) compared to the population-based sample. Moreover, they reported higher levels of maternal negative parenting (d = .15, p < .01), maternal rule-breaking (d = .17, p < .05), paternal rule-breaking (d = .16, p < .05) and child rule-breaking (d = .11, p < .05), although maternal, paternal, and child aggression did not differ across the two samples.

Zygosity was established using physical similarity questionnaires administered to the twins’ primary caregiver (Peeters, Gestel, Vlietinck, Derom, & Derom, 1998), a common method for establishing zygosity in large twin samples. On average, the physical similarity questionnaires used by the MSUTR have accuracy rates of 95% or better (Peeters et al., 1998). The current study included 351 MZ twin pairs and 473 DZ twin pairs.

Measures

Aggression (AGG)

Both mothers and fathers reported on each of their twins’ behavior during the past 6 months using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), a commonly used measure of children’s adaptive and maladaptive behaviors (Achenbach, 1991). We focused here on the 18-item aggression scale (e.g., “threatens others”, “attacks others”; α = .88 for mother and .86 for fathers). Both parents also completed the Sub-types of Antisocial Behavior (STAB) questionnaire on each of their twins behavior during the past year (Burt & Donnellan, 2009). For the current study, we focused on the 10-item aggression (e.g., “he/she got into physical fights”, “he/she swore or yelled at others”; α = .89 for mother and .87 for fathers) subscale. Maternal and paternal reports of their children’s levels of aggression were moderately correlated (CBCL: r = .54; STAB: r = .51). Correlations within informants and across scales for aggression were also moderate-to-large in magnitude (Mothers: r = .67, Fathers: r = .60). We thus created a composite by standardizing scores from mother and father reports on the CBCL and the STAB and computing the overall mean. The AGG composite evidenced adequate internal consistency reliability for both mother and father reports (α = .89 and .86, respectively).

Rule-breaking (RB)

For our measure of rule-breaking, we focused on the 17-item rule-breaking scale on the CBCL (e.g., “breaks rules”, “cheats”) and the 11-item rule-breaking on the STAB (e.g., “he/she littered public areas”, “he/she stole property from school”), as completed by both mothers and fathers. For both scales, it is worth noting that some behaviors were virtually non-existent (e.g., smokes tobacco, uses illicit drugs) or very rare in this sample (e.g., drinks alcohol, thinks about sex too much), not surprising given the age range of our participants (6–10 years). In order to create a more developmentally sensitive measure of RB, we submitted the CBCL and STAB rule-breaking items to a principle-axis exploratory factor analysis, separately for mother and father reports. The scree plot suggested a clear one-factor solution for both informants. We then selected the items with a principle factor loading greater than .5 for either informant. This resulted in a 13-item measure of rule-breaking behaviors (6 items from the STAB and 7 items from the CBCL; α for modified RB scale = .75 for mothers and .68 for fathers). Final items are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

Items included in the Final 13-item Rule-breaking Scale

Scale Item
CBCL Breaks rules at home, school, or elsewhere
CBCL Hangs around with others who get in trouble
CBCL Lying or cheating
CBCL Steals at home
CBCL Steals outside of home
CBCL Swearing or obscene language
CBCL Vandalism
STAB He/she shoplifted things
STAB He/she littered public areas by smashing bottles, tipping trash cans, etc.
STAB He/she stole a bicycle
STAB He/she stole property from school or work
STAB He/she left home for an extended period of time without telling family/friends
STAB He/she was suspended or expelled from school

Note. CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist (includes behaviors within the past 6 months); STAB = Sub-types of Antisocial Behavior (includes behaviors within the past year).

Negative Mothering

Child and parent reports

The Parental Environment Questionnaire (PEQ) was administered to measure various qualities of the parent-child relationship using parallel parent and child forms. The current study focused on the 12-item parent-child conflict scale. Items included “I often criticize my child”, “I often lose my temper with my child”, and “Once in a while my child has been really scared of me”, with alternations in wording appropriate for parents and children. Each informant rated items on a 4-point scale (1=definitely true; 2 = somewhat true, 3 = somewhat false; 4 = definitely false). Mothers individually rated their relationship with each of their twin children. Children reported on their relationship with their mother. Consistent with prior studies (Burt et al., 2003, 2005, 2007), child and maternal ratings of conflict were significantly if modestly correlated (r = .18), and were thus averaged to create a composite of reported mother-child conflict.

Observer ratings

Parenting was also observed during an 8 minute video-taped interaction between each mother-child dyad (i.e., there were two such interactions per family, one with each twin). The on-campus interactions took place in laboratory offices that were setup to resemble living rooms, with cameras inconspicuously installed in the ceiling. For those assessments occurring in participants’ homes, interactions took place in a family living space with a video camera placed on a tripod in the room. Each mother-child dyad was asked to use an Etch-a Sketch and work together to draw specific pictures, with the mother and child only using one dial each (a mildly frustrating task). The interactions were then scored by trained undergraduate research assistants using the Parent-Child Interaction System or PARCHISY (Deater-Deckard, Pylas, & Petrill, 1997).

For the current study, we focused on two measures of maternal negativity: negative content (e.g., use of criticism, physical control of the dials, and physical control of the child’s hand or body) and negative affect with child (e.g., rejection, frowning, cold/harsh tone). Observer reliability was assessed by randomly assigning 10% of all tapes to be rated by a second observer, and then comparing the primary and secondary ratings using intraclass correlations. Intraclass correlations between coders were r ≥ .96 for both scales. Mother-child dyad ratings were available for 96.2% of twins (3.8% of videos were not codeable; e.g., there was no sound, the file was corrupted). In order to capture overall levels of maternal negativity, observer-ratings were combined with parent and child-reports by standardizing each variable and computing the overall mean. As found in numerous independent studies using observer and informant-reports of parenting and behavior (Arseneault et al., 2003; Burt, Klahr, Rueter, McGue, & Iacono, 2011; Burt & Klump, 2014), observed and reported negativity were significantly, if modestly, correlated (r = .12).

Statistical Analyses

Twin methodology uses the difference in the proportion of segregating genes shared between monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs to estimate genetic and environmental contributions to variance in observed behaviors or characteristics (phenotypes). MZ or identical twins result from a single fertilized zygote splitting and hence share 100% of their segregating genes. DZ or fraternal twins are the result of two independent conceptions and so, like all full siblings, share an average of 50% of their segregating genes. Utilizing these differences, the variance within observed behaviors is partitioned into three components: additive genetic effects (A), shared environmental effects (C), and non-shared environmental effects plus measurement error (E). The additive genetic component (A) is the effect of individual genes summed over loci. Additive genetics effects, if acting alone, would result in MZ correlations that are double those of DZ correlations. The shared environment (C) is that part of the environment that acts to make the twins within a pair similar to each other regardless of the proportion of genes shared. The non-shared environment (E) includes environmental factors unique to each twin within a pair along with measurement error. The non-shared environment differentiates each twin within a pair, making them less similar.

The Equal Environments Assumption is foundational to these analyses, as it assumes that MZ twin pairs are no more likely to share the environmental factors that are etiologically relevant to the phenotype under study than are DZ twin pairs. Under this assumption, any differences in the similarity of MZ and DZ twins are due to differences in their genetic similarity. The Equal Environments Assumption has been repeatedly tested and found to be valid for numerous phenotypes (Hettema, Neale, & Kendler, 1995; Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, 1993; Morris-Yates, Andrews, Howie, & Henderson, 1990; Scarr & Carter-Saltzman, 1979), but it remains an assumption for any particular phenotype until subjected to empirical testing. Parenting does appear to differ somewhat across zygosity (i.e., parents treat MZ twins more similarly than DZ twins). However, because twin similarities are associated with actual rather than perceived zygosity (Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, 1994), these differences are likely a function of evocative rGE rather than differences in parental treatment.

For our primary analyses, we made use of a bivariate correlated factors model (see Figure 1) in which the variance within and the covariance between child behavior and parenting were decomposed into their genetic and environmental components. This enabled us to obtain estimates of etiological influences on child behavior and parenting, as well as on their overlap. In particular, genetic and environmental correlations index the proportion of genetic and environmental influences on child behavior that are shared with parenting behavior. A genetic correlation of 1.0 would indicate that the genetic influences on child behavior overlap entirely with those on maternal negativity (i.e., the same genes influence both phenotypes). A correlation of zero would indicate no genetic overlap. These models thus enabled us to explicitly estimate the extent to which genetic and environmental influences on child aggression and rule-breaking, respectively, overlap with those on maternal negativity.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Bivariate Correlated Factors Model

Note. Path diagram of a full ACE Correlated Factors model for child aggression and maternal negative parenting. The variance in each phenotype is parsed into additive genetic effects (A), shared environmental effects (C), and nonshared environmental effects (E). Genetic and environmental correlations are indicated via the double-headed arrows. Paths (indicated via single-headed arrows) are squared to estimate the percentage of variance accounted for.

Because there was a small amount of missing data, we made use of Full-Information Maximum-Likelihood raw data techniques (FIML), which produce less biased and more efficient and consistent estimates than techniques like pairwise or listwise deletion in the face of missing data (Little & Rubin, 1987). Mx (Neale, Boker, Xie, & Maes, 2003) was used to fit models to the raw data. When fitting models to raw data, variances, covariances, and means of those data are first freely estimated by minimizing minus twice the log-likelihood (−2lnL). The minimized value of −2lnL in the baseline model was then compared with the −2lnL obtained in the biometric models to yield a likelihood-ratio chi-square test. The chi-square was then converted to the Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC; AIC = χ2 - (2*df); Akaike, 1987), so as to measure model fit relative to parsimony. Negative AIC values are generally thought to indicate that the biometric model provides a good fit to the data. To control for any effect of child age, sex, or ethnicity, these variables were regressed out of the data prior to analysis (McGue & Bouchard, 1984).

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 3. Boys exhibited higher levels of aggression and rule-breaking than girls (all p <.05; Cohen’s d ranged from .13 - .41). Mothers also exhibited higher levels of negative parenting with their sons than with their daughters, across all informants (all p <.05; Cohen’s d ranged from .09 - .27). As expected, aggression and rule-breaking were positively associated with each other (r = .60, p <.01) and with maternal negativity (r’s = .33 and .27 with aggression and rule-breaking, respectively; both p <.01). 8.4% of boys and 5.2% of girls scored above the borderline clinically significant cut-off for aggression, as defined by the Achenbach scoring system (T > 65; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). For rule-breaking, 6.1% of boys and 2.0% of girls scored above the cut-off.

Table 3.

Mean Levels of Aggression, Rule-breaking, and Negative Parenting Separately by Child Sex

Males Females
Mean SD Min Max N Mean SD Min Max N
Aggression
CBCL-Mother report 5.16** 5.37 0 31 855 4.01 4.54 0 28 783
CBCL- Father report 5.01** 4.52 0 25 709 4.14 4.45 0 25 659
STAB-Mother report 19.59** 6.18 10 47 842 17.26 5.27 10 35 775
STAB-Father report 18.79** 5.35 10 46 697 16.79 4.85 10 37 653
Rule-breaking
CBCL-Mother report 1.89** 2.23 0 14 855 1.20 1.62 0 14 783
CBCL- Father report 1.78** 1.99 0 17 709 1.27 1.53 0 8 659
STAB-Mother report 11.26** 0.88 11 20 842 11.11 0.56 11 21 775
STAB-Father report 11.25* 1.78 11 25 697 11.08 0.35 11 15 653
Negative Parenting
Observed Negative
Content 1.67 0.78 1 6 831 1.60 0.72 1 4 751
Observed Negative Affect 1.35* 0.66 1 6 831 1.28 0.56 1 4 754
Conflict- Mother report 21.21** 6.08 12 42 847 20.31 5.76 12 41 776
Conflict- Twin report 21.33** 6.21 12 46 837 19.71 5.64 12 45 784

Note. CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist. STAB = Sub-types of Antisocial Behavior.

*

Mean are significantly different between males and females, p <.05.

**

p < .01.

Intraclass Correlations

Prior to model fitting analyses, intraclass and cross-twin, cross-trait correlations were computed for MZ and DZ twin pairs on the negative parenting, aggression, and rule-breaking composites. MZ intraclass correlations that are double those of DZ intraclass correlations are indicative of genetic effects, whereas MZ correlations that are less than double but still greater than DZ correlations suggest the importance of genetic and shared environmental effects. As seen in Table 4, intraclass correlations suggest both genetic and shared environmental effects may be important for aggression and rule-breaking, as well as for negative parenting. Cross-twin, cross-trait correlations are also presented in Table 4. These correlations give a preliminary indication of the degree of genetic and environmental overlap between parenting and child behavior. Cross-twin, cross trait correlations for aggression and negative parenting suggest the possibility of genetically- and shared environmentally-mediated overlap (MZr = 0.32, DZr = 0.19). The pattern differs somewhat for rule-breaking, pointing instead to predominantly shared environmentally-mediated overlap (MZr = 0.23, DZr = 0.18).

Table 4.

Intraclass and Cross-twin Cross-trait Correlations for Aggression, Rule-breaking, and Negative Parenting

1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Aggression- a - 0.62 0.30 0.42 0.33 0.19
2. Rule-breaking- a 0.57 - 0.25 0.33 0.44 0.18
3. Negative parenting- a 0.37 0.30 - 0.19 0.18 0.47
4. Aggression- b 0.68 0.46 0.32 - 0.62 0.30
5. Rule-breaking - b 0.46 0.68 0.22 0.57 - 0.25
6. Negative parenting- b 0.32 0.23 0.58 0.38 0.30 -

Note. a = twin a; b = twin b; MZ correlations are below the diagonal, DZ correlations are above.

Intraclass correlations are in bold. Cross-twin cross-trait correlations are highlighted in blue.

Bivariate Model-fitting Analyses

Fit indices for both models indicated a reasonable fit to the data (Aggression: baseline model: −2lnL = 6265.12 on 3268 df; full model: −2lnL = 6297.72 on 3285 df, AIC = −35.39; Rule-breaking: baseline model: −2lnL = 6656.38 on 3268 df; full model: −2lnL = 6681.62 on 3285 df, AIC = −42.76). Parameter estimates for genetic and environmental influences on aggression, rule-breaking, and negative parenting are presented in Table 5. Genetic, shared, and non-shared environmental estimates were significant for both aggression and rule-breaking. For maternal negativity, there was evidence of evocative genetic (22%), shared environmental (37%), and non-shared environmental (42%) influences.

Table 5.

Genetic and Environmental Etiology of Aggression, Rule-breaking, and Negative Parenting

A C E
Aggression 55% (.40, .71) 14% (.01, .28) 30% (.26, .36)
Rule-breaking 58% (.42, .71) 14% (.03, .27) 29% (.24, .34)
Maternal negativity 22% (.04, .39) 37% (.22, .50) 42% (.36, .49)

Note. A = genetic variance; C = shared environmental variance; E = non-shared environmental variance. 95% confidence intervals are presented in parentheses beside their respective correlations. Significant estimates are in bold.

We also examined the genetic and environmental correlations between child behavior and negative parenting (see Figure 2). For aggression and maternal negativity, the overlap was both genetic (rA = .40; 95% CI [.05, .95]) and shared environmental (rC = .62; 95% CI [.18, 1.00]) in origin. By contrast, the association between negative parenting and rule-breaking was largely shared environmental in origin (rC = .82; 95% CI [.39, 1.00]). There was no significant genetic overlap (rA = .05; 95% CI [−.37, .39]). Notably, a comparison of the genetic correlations using fisher’s r to Z transformation indicated that the genetic correlation between child rule-breaking and maternal negativity was significantly smaller than the genetic correlation between child aggression and maternal negativity (p < .01), while the shared environmental correlation was significantly larger for rule-breaking and negativity (p < .01). Lastly, there was evidence of small but significant non-shared environmental overlap between maternal negativity and aggression (rE = .14; 95% CI [.04, .24]) and rule-breaking (rE = .19; 95% CI [.09, .29]).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Results from Model-Fitting Analyses

Discussion

The current study examined the etiology of the association between two dimensions of antisocial behavior in children, aggression and rule-breaking, and the presence of maternal negativity. The results highlighted an important difference across aggression and rule-breaking. Although the association between aggression and maternal negativity was partially genetic in origin, the association between rule-breaking and maternal negativity was primarily shared environmental in origin. Such findings suggest that evocative rGE processes are specific to the relationship between maternal negativity and child aggression, and do not contribute to the association with non-aggressive rule-breaking. Put differently, mothers appear to be responding to their child’s genetically-influenced aggressive behavior with higher levels of negativity towards their child. Genetic influences on child rule-breaking, by contrast, do not appear to elicit this reaction. That said, less than half of the evocative genetic influences on maternal negativity overlapped with genetic influences on child aggression, suggesting other genetically-influenced child characteristics are also important for evoking negative parenting. Although it remains unclear what these other characteristics might be (temperament, internalizing problems, and/or ADHD symptoms are all possible candidates as these characteristics are both genetically-influenced and associated with parenting), they do not appear to include non-aggressive rule-breaking antisocial behavior.

There was also evidence for small but significant non-shared environmental overlap with maternal negativity, although the shared environment overlap was far more prominent. Indeed, shared environmental overlap was significant and moderate-to-large in magnitude for both aggression and rule-breaking. This environmental mediation was particularly important for rule-breaking, both because of the absence of genetic overlap, but also because the shared environmental correlation with maternal negativity was significantly stronger for rule-breaking as compared with aggression.

There are several limitations to the existing study. First, our assessment of child aggression and rule-breaking relied solely upon parent report. Although we were able to utilize reports from both mothers and fathers, additional informants would be beneficial. Fortunately, our measures of parenting included parent-reports, child-reports, and observer-ratings and so we were able to minimize shared-informant effects between our measures of antisocial behavior and parenting. Relying solely upon parent reports of child aggression and rule-breaking is also less of a concern given the age range of our participants (6–10 years), both because of concerns regarding child reliability, but also because parents are relatively well-informed on their child’s acting out behaviors during middle childhood (at least as compared to adolescence; Dishion & McMahon, 1998; Patterson & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1984). This points to a second limitation, namely that the results of this study are only applicable during middle childhood and not during other developmental stages. Prior research has highlighted substantial developmental shifts in the etiology and phenotypic expression of aggression and rule-breaking across development (Burt & Klump, 2009; Burt & Neiderhiser, 2009; Burt, in press; Stanger et al., 1997; Tremblay, 2003). Additional research would therefore be needed before making any conclusions regarding the etiology of the relationships among aggression, rule-breaking, and maternal negativity in the preschool and adolescent periods. Also, given that most of our sample exhibited low levels of aggression and rule-breaking, the current findings are most applicable for aggressive and rule-breaking behaviors within the normal range. Additional research should examine these associations in a clinic-based sample.

Next, our analyses focused solely on maternal negativity, to the exclusion of paternal negativity. Behavioral genetic research suggests the presence of etiological differences between maternal and paternal negativity (Klahr & Burt, 2014). Given these differences, we plan to explicitly compare etiological differences in the associations between maternal and paternal parenting and child outcomes in future analyses. In addition, other aspects of the parent-child relationship are also associated with antisocial behavior (e.g., positivity/warmth, control/limit setting, and attachment; Marcus & Kramer, 2001; Pettit, Laird, Dodge, Bates, & Criss, 2001; Stormshak, Bierman, McMahon, & Lengua, 2000; Troy & Sroufe, 1987) and may exhibit differential associations with aggression vs. rule-breaking. Sub-types of negative parenting (e.g., verbal criticism vs. physical punishment) may also demonstrate differential associations. Additional research in this domain is needed. Finally, the current study is unable to disambiguate the effects of passive rGE from environmentally-mediated effects. Because we examined biological families, we cannot rule-out the possibility that shared genes may account for what appears to be an environmental association. That said, existing empirical literature (Marceau et al., 2013; Narusyte et al., 2011b) has not identified passive rGE effects in the association between parenting and child antisocial behavior, implying that passive rGE is unlikely account for the association between parenting and offspring behavior problems in this study. Building on these results, several adoption studies have found evidence of a shared environmentally-driven association between parenting and antisocial behavior in general (Burt et al., 2007; Klahr et al., 2011a, 2011b), results that again argue against passive rGE (because passive rGE is entirely circumvented in non-biologically related family members).

In spite of these limitations, the current study has several important implications. The results highlight a possible explanation for differing conclusions about genetic vs. environmental mediation in the existing behavior genetic literature (e.g., Klahr et al., 2011a; Marceau et al., 2013) and for differing conclusions among longitudinal investigations of parenting and behavior problems that implicate parent-driven effects (Gardner, Ward, Burton, & Wilson, 2003) vs. child-driven effects (Reitz, Deković, Meijer, & Engels, 2006). These results also add to the large body of literature supporting a distinction between aggressive and rule-breaking dimensions of antisocial behavior in children. For example, aggression is more strongly associated with negative emotion and affective dysfunction (Burt & Donnellan, 2008, 2009; Burt, 2012; Moffitt, 1993, 2003; Tackett, 2010; Verona, Patrick, & Lang, 2002) while rule-breaking is more strongly associated with impulsivity (Burt & Donnellan, 2008, 2009; Burt, Donnellan, & Tackett, 2012; Hopwood et al., 2009; Moffitt, 2003; Tackett, 2010). In addition, aggression is uniquely associated with executive dysfunction (Barker et al., 2007; Barker, Oliver, Viding, Salekin, & Maughan, 2011; Hancock, Tapscott, & Hoaken, 2010; Miura, 2009). The current study adds to such work by suggesting that even those correlates that are associated with both aggression and rule-breaking have different etiologic underpinnings. In particular, evocative rGE effects on maternal negativity are in part a function of child aggression, but are entirely unrelated to non-aggressive rule-breaking in those same children.

Next, the shared environmentally-mediated associations between maternal negativity and child aggression and rule-breaking, respectively, are consistent with a potentially causal effect of parenting on these behaviors. Such results dovetail nicely with the well-established role of parenting in the initiation and maintenance of antisocial behaviors in the treatment literature (Barkley, 1997; Klahr & Burt, in press; McMahon, Forehand, & Foster, 2005). Because our study is cross-sectional, it is worth noting that other third variable shared environmental factors may also be at play, in addition to parenting. For example, neighborhood disadvantage and financial strain are associated with negative parenting (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & García Coll, 2001; Klebanov, Brooks-Gunn, & Duncan, 1994; McLeod & Shanahan, 1993) and with antisocial behavior in children (Costello, Foley, & Angold, 2006; Côté, Vaillancourt, LeBlanc, Nagin, & Tremblay, 2006). As both neighborhood disadvantage and financial strain are considered family-wide variables, they may well contribute to the common shared environmental variance between parenting and child antisocial behavior.

Finally, the current results have implications for understanding both the etiology of negative parenting and the transactional nature of parenting and child behavior problems. A given adult’s parenting behaviors are not just a reflection of the parent, but are also dependent on the characteristics of the child being parented. Our results further suggest that these evocative effects are more prominent for aggressive behavior problems than they are for rule-breaking behaviors. Although most of our sample exhibited behavior problems and negative parenting in the normative range, parental reactions to normative acting out behaviors in children can serve to either promote normative development or foster a trajectory of worsening behavior problems (i.e., the coercive cycle; Patterson, 1982). As a result, many intervention programs attempt to teach parents how to constructively respond to normative behavior problems before these problems reach clinical severity (e.g., The Chicago Parent Program; Gross et al., 2009). The evocative association between aggression and maternal negativity identified within the current population-based sample thus has implications related to tailoring prevention and treatment for primarily aggressive vs. primarily rule-breaking sub-types of antisocial behavior (Klahr & Burt, in press). In particular, although behavioral parent-management training is considered the gold-standard treatment for antisocial behavior (Brestan & Eyberg, 1998), the early age of onset and high levels of rank-order stability associated with aggressive behavior problems (Tremblay, 2003) suggests the importance of early intervention for the most aggressive preschool children. This early intervention might focus on how parents respond to aggressive behaviors in their children and subsequently help to curtail the maintenance and escalation of negative parenting practices for children who are likely to continue to be more aggressive than their peers across development.

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by R01-MH081813 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and by R01-HD066040 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), awarded to Drs. Burt and Klump. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH, the NICHD, or the National Institutes of Health. The authors thank all participating twins and their families for making this work possible.

References

  1. Achenbach TM. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont. 1991 [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM, Rescorla L. In: ASEBA School-age Forms and Profiles. Aseba, editor. Burlington: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  3. Akaike H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987;52:317–332. [Google Scholar]
  4. Alemany S, Rijsdijk FV, Haworth CMA, Fañanás L, Plomin R. Genetic origin of the relationship between parental negativity and behavior problems from early childhood to adolescence: a longitudinal genetically sensitive study. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(2):487–500. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412001198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alink LRA, Egeland B. The roles of antisocial history and emerging adulthood developmental adaption in predicting adult antisocial behavior. Aggressive Behavior. 2013;39(2):131–140. doi: 10.1002/ab.21464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anderson KE, Lytton H, Romney DM. Mothers’ interactions with normal and conduct-disordered boys: Who affects whom? Developmental Psychology. 1986;22(5):604. [Google Scholar]
  7. Arseneault L, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Rijsdijk FV, Jaffee SR, Measelle JR. Strong genetic effects on cross-situational antisocial behaviour among 5-year-old children according to mothers, teachers, examiner-observers, and twins’ self-reports. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44(6):832–848. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baker LA, Barton M, Raine A. The southern California twin register at the University of Southern California. Twin Research: The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies. 2002;5(5):456. doi: 10.1375/136905202320906273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barker ED, Oliver BR, Viding E, Salekin RT, Maughan B. The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament and early parenting on adolescent callous-unemotional traits: A 14-year longitudinal investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52(8):878–888. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barker ED, Séguin JR, White HR, Bates ME, Lacourse E, Carbonneau R, Tremblay RE. Developmental trajectories of male physical violence and theft: relations to neurocognitive performance. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64(5):592–599. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barkley RA. Defiant children: A clinician’s manual for assessment and parent training. The Guilford Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  12. Belsky J, Pasco Fearon RM, Bell B. Parenting, attention and externalizing problems: testing mediation longitudinally, repeatedly and reciprocally. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(12):1233–1242. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01807.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, García Coll C. The home environments of children in the United States part I: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development. 2001;72(6):1844–1867. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Braungart-Rieker J, Rende RD, Plomin R, DeFries JC, Fulker DW. Genetic mediation of longitudinal associations between family environment and childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7(02):233–245. [Google Scholar]
  15. Brestan EV, Eyberg SM. Effective psychosocial treatments of conduct-disordered children and adolescents: 29 years, 82 studies, and 5,272 kids. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1998;27(2):180–189. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2702_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burt SA. Evidence for meaningful etiological distinctions within the broader construct of antisocial behavior. In: Rhee SH, A R, editors. Behavior Genetics of Psychopatholog. New York: Springer; in press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Burt SA. Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29(2):163–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Burt SA. How do we optimally conceptualize the heterogeneity within antisocial behavior? An argument for aggressive versus non-aggressive behavioral dimensions. Clinical Psychology Review. 2012;32(4):263–279. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Burt SA. Do etiological influences on aggression overlap with those on rule breaking? A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2013;43(9):1801–1812. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Burt SA, Donnellan MB. Personality correlates of aggressive and non-aggressive antisocial behavior. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;44(1):53–63. [Google Scholar]
  21. Burt SA, Donnellan MB. Development and validation of the Subtypes of Antisocial Behavior Questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior. 2009;35(5):376–398. doi: 10.1002/ab.20314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Burt SA, Donnellan MB, Tackett JL. Should social aggression be considered “antisocial”? Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2012;34(2):153–163. [Google Scholar]
  23. Burt SA, Klahr AM, Rueter Ma, McGue M, Iacono WG. Confirming the etiology of adolescent acting-out behaviors: an examination of observer-ratings in a sample of adoptive and biological siblings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52(5):519–526. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02334.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Burt SA, Klump KL. The etiological moderation of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior by age. Twin Research and Human Genetics: The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies. 2009;12(4):343–350. doi: 10.1375/twin.12.4.343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Burt SA, Klump KL. The Michigan state university twin registry (MSUTR): An update. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2013;16(1):344. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Burt SA, Klump KL. Parent-child conflict as an etiological moderator of childhood conduct problems: an example of a “bioecological” gene-environment interaction. Psychological Medicine. 2014;44:1065–1076. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Burt SA, Krueger RF, McGue M, Iacono W. Parent-child conflict and the comorbidity among childhood externalizing disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(5):505. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.5.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Burt SA, Larson CL. Differential affective responses in those with aggressive versus non-aggressive antisocial behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;43(6):1481–1492. [Google Scholar]
  29. Burt SA, McGue M, Krueger RF, Iacono WG. How are parent-child conflict and childhood externalizing symptoms related over time? Results from a genetically informative cross-lagged study. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17(01):145–165. doi: 10.1017/S095457940505008X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Burt SA, McGue M, Krueger RF, Iacono WG. Environmental contributions to adolescent delinquency: a fresh look at the shared environment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2007;35(5):787–800. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9135-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Burt SA, Mikolajewski AJ, Larson CL. Do aggression and rule-breaking have different interpersonal correlates? A study of antisocial behavior subtypes, negative affect, and hostile perceptions of others. Aggressive Behavior. 2009;35(6):453–461. doi: 10.1002/ab.20324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Burt SA, Neiderhiser JM. Aggressive versus nonaggressive antisocial behavior: distinctive etiological moderation by age. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(4):1164–1176. doi: 10.1037/a0016130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Button TMM, Lau JYF, Maughan B, Eley TC. Parental punitive discipline, negative life events and gene-environment interplay in the development of externalizing behavior. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38(01):29–39. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Costello EJ, Foley DL, Angold A. 10-year research update review: the epidemiology of child and adolescent psychiatric disorders: II. Developmental epidemiology. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45(1):8–25. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000184929.41423.c0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Côté SM, Vaillancourt T, LeBlanc JC, Nagin DS, Tremblay RE. The development of physical aggression from toddlerhood to pre-adolescence: a nation wide longitudinal study of Canadian children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34(1):71–85. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-9001-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Cunningham CE, Barkley RA. The interactions of normal and hyperactive children with their mothers in free play and structured tasks. Child Development. 1979;50(1):217–224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Deater-Deckard K, Dodge KA. Externalizing Behavior Problems and Discipline Revisited: Nonlinear Effects and Variation by Culture, Context, and Gender. Psychological Inquiry. 1997;8(3):161–175. [Google Scholar]
  38. Deater-Deckard K, Pylas MV, Petrill SA. The Parent-child interaction system (PARCHISY) London: Institute of Psychiatry; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  39. Dishion TJ, McMahon RJ. Parental Monitoring and the Prevention of Child and Adolescent Problem Behavior: A Conceptual and Empirical Formulation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 1998;1(1):61–75. doi: 10.1023/a:1021800432380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dishion TJ, Patterson GR, Stoolmiller M, Skinner ML. Family, school, and behavioral antecedents to early adolescent involvement with antisocial peers. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27:172–180. [Google Scholar]
  41. Elam KK, Harold GT, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Shaw DS, Natsuaki MN, Leve LD. Adoptive parent hostility and children’s peer behavior problems: Examining the role of genetically informed child attributes on adoptive parent behavior. Developmental Psychology. 2013 doi: 10.1037/a0035470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gardner F, Ward S, Burton J, Wilson C. The Role of Mother-Child Joint Play in the Early Development of Children’s Conduct Problems: A Longitudinal Observational Study. Social Development. 2003;12(3):361–378. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ge X, Conger RD, Cadoret RJ, Neiderhiser JM, Yates W, Troughton E, Stewart MA. The developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual influence model of child antisocial behavior and parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32(4):574–589. [Google Scholar]
  44. Goodman R, Scott S. Comparing the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and the Child Behavior Checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27(1):17–24. doi: 10.1023/a:1022658222914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gross D, Garvey C, Julion W, Fogg L, Tucker S, Mokros H. Efficacy of the Chicago parent program with low-income African American and Latino parents of young children. Prevention Science. 2009;10(1):54–65. doi: 10.1007/s11121-008-0116-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hancock M, Tapscott JL, Hoaken PNS. Role of executive dysfunction in predicting frequency and severity of violence. Aggressive Behavior. 2010;36(5):338–349. doi: 10.1002/ab.20353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Harold GT, Leve LD, Elam KK, Thapar A, Neiderhiser JM, Natsuaki MN, Reiss D. The nature of nurture: Disentangling passive genotype-environment correlation from family relationship influences on children’s externalizing problems. Journal of Family Psychology. 2013;27:12–21. doi: 10.1037/a0031190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hay DA, McStephen M, Levy F, Pearsall-Jones J. Recruitment and attrition in twin register studies of childhood behavior: the example of the Australian twin ADHD project. Twin Research. 2002;5(5):324–328. doi: 10.1375/136905202320906039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS. Physical similarity and the equal-environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric disorders. Behavior Genetics. 1995;25(4):327–335. doi: 10.1007/BF02197281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hoeve M, Dubas JS, Eichelsheim VI, van der Laan PH, Smeenk W, Gerris JRM. The relationship between parenting and delinquency: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37(6):749–775. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9310-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hopwood CJ, Burt SA, Markowitz JC, Yen S, Shea MT, Sanislow CA, Morey LC. The construct validity of rule-breaking and aggression in an adult clinical sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2009;43(8):803–808. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Karraker KH, Coleman PK. The effects of child characteristics on parenting. In: Luster T, Okagaki L, editors. Parenting: An Ecological Perspective. London, UK: Routledge; 2005. p. 464. Retrieved from http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=ynKRAgAAQBAJ&pgis=1. [Google Scholar]
  53. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. A test of the equal-environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness. Behavior Genetics. 1993;23(1):21–27. doi: 10.1007/BF01067551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. Parental treatment and the equal environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness. Psychological Medicine. 1994;24(03):579. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700027732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kerr DCR, Lopez NL, Olson SL, Sameroff AJ. Parental discipline and externalizing behavior problems in early childhood: The roles of moral regulation and child gender. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2004;32(4):369–383. doi: 10.1023/b:jacp.0000030291.72775.96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Klahr AM, Burt SA. Practitioner review: Evaluation of the known behavioral heterogeneity in Conduct Disorder to improve its assessment and treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. in press doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Klahr AM, Burt SA. Elucidating the etiology of individual differences in parenting: A meta-analysis of behavioral genetic research. Psychological Bulletin. 2014;140:544–586. doi: 10.1037/a0034205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Klahr AM, McGue M, Iacono WG, Burt SA. The association between parent-child conflict and adolescent conduct problems over time: Results from a longitudinal adoption study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120(1):46. doi: 10.1037/a0021350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Klahr AM, Rueter MA, McGue M, Iacono WG, Burt SA. The relationship between parent-child conflict and adolescent antisocial behavior: Confirming shared environmental mediation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39(5):683–694. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9505-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Klahr AM, Thomas KM, Hopwood CJ, Klump KL, Burt SA. Evocative gene-enviornment correlation in the mother-child relationship: A twin study of interpersonal processes. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(1):105–118. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Klebanov PK, Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. Does neighborhood and family poverty affect mothers’ parenting, mental health, and social support? Journal of Marriage & the Family. 1994;56(2):441–455. [Google Scholar]
  62. Klump KL, Burt SA. The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): genetic, environmental and neurobiological influences on behavior across development. Twin Research and Human Genetics: The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies. 2006;9(6):971–977. doi: 10.1375/183242706779462868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Larsson H, Viding E, Rijsdijk FV, Plomin R. Relationships between parental negativity and childhood antisocial behavior over time: a bidirectional effects model in a longitudinal genetically informative design. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(5):633–645. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9151-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Leve LD, Kim HK, Pears KC. Childhood temperament and family environment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing trajectories from ages 5 to 17. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33(5):505–520. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-6734-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Little RJA, Rubin DB. The analysis of social science data with missing values. Sociological Methods & Research. 1987;18(2-3):292–326. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lysenko LJ, Barker ED, Jaffee SR. Sex differences in the relationship between harsh discipline and conduct problems. Social Development. 2013;22(1):197–214. [Google Scholar]
  67. Marceau K, Horwitz BN, Narusyte J, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Reiss D, Neiderhiser JM. Gene-environment correlation underlying the association between parental negativity and adolescent externalizing problems. Child Development. 2013;84(6):2031–2046. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Marcus RF, Kramer C. Reactive and proactive aggression: attachment and social competence predictors. The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 2001;162(3):260–275. doi: 10.1080/00221320109597483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. McGue M, Bouchard TJ. Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics. 1984;14(4):325–343. doi: 10.1007/BF01080045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. McGue M, Sharma A, Benson P. The effect of common rearing on adolescent adjustment: Evidence from a U.S. adoption cohort. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32(4):604–613. [Google Scholar]
  71. McLeod JD, Shanahan MJ. Poverty, parenting, and children’s mental health. American Sociological Review. 1993;58:351–366. [Google Scholar]
  72. McMahon RJ, Forehand RL, Foster SL. Helping the noncompliant child: Family-based treatment for oppositional behavior. The Guilford Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  73. Meier MH, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Martin NG. The role of harsh discipline in explaining sex differences in conduct disorder: a study of opposite-sex twin pairs. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37(5):653–664. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9309-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Miner JL, Clarke-Stewart KA. Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: Relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:771–786. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Miura H. Differences in frontal lobe function between violent and nonviolent conduct disorder in male adolescents. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2009;63(2):161–166. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01935.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100(4):674–701. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8255953. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Moffitt TE. Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review and a research agenda. In: Lahey BB, editor. Causes of Conduct Disorder and Juvenile Delinquency. New York, NY: Guildford Press; 2003. pp. 49–75. [Google Scholar]
  78. Morris-Yates A, Andrews G, Howie P, Henderson S. Twins: a test of the equal environments assumption. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1990;81(4):322–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1990.tb05457.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Narusyte J, Neiderhiser JM, Andershed AK, D’Onofrio BM, Reiss D, Spotts E, Lichtenstein P. Parental criticism and externalizing behavior problems in adolescents: The role of environment and genotype–environment correlation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011a;120(2):365. doi: 10.1037/a0021815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Narusyte J, Neiderhiser JM, Andershed A-K, D’Onofrio BM, Reiss D, Spotts E, Lichtenstein P. Parental criticism and externalizing behavior problems in adolescents: The role of environment and genotype-environment correlation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011b;120(2):365–376. doi: 10.1037/a0021815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Narusyte J, Neiderhiser JM, D’Onofrio BM, Reiss D, Spotts EL, Ganiban J, Lichtenstein P. Testing different types of genotype-environment correlation: An extended children-of-twins model. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(6):1591–1603. doi: 10.1037/a0013911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Neale M, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH. Mx- Statistical Modeling. Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University Medical School: Box 980126; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  83. Nix RL, Pinderhughes EE, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS, McFadyen-Ketchum SA. The relation between mothers’ hostile attribution tendencies and children's externalizing behavior problems: The mediating role of mothers' harsh discipline practices. Child Development. 1999;70(4):896–909. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. O’Connor TG, Deater-Deckard K, Fulker D, Rutter M, Plomin R. Genotype-environment correlations in late childhood and early adolescence: antisocial behavioral problems and coercive parenting. Developmental Psychology. 1998;34(5):970–981. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.970. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9779743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Patterson GR. Coercive Family Process. Eugene, OR: Castalia Publishing Co; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  86. Patterson GR, Fisher PA. Recent developments in our understanding of parenting: Bidirectional effects, causal models, and the search for parsimony. Handbook of Parenting. 2002;5:59–88. [Google Scholar]
  87. Patterson GR, Stouthamer-Loeber M. The correlation of family management practices and delinquency. Child Development. 1984;55(4):1299–1307. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Peeters H, Gestel S, Van Vlietinck R, Derom C, Derom R. Validation of a telephone zygosity questionnaire in twins of known zygosity. Behavior Genetics. 1998;28(3):159–163. doi: 10.1023/a:1021416112215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Pettit GS, Laird RD, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Criss MM. Antecedents and behavior-problem outcomes of parental monitoring and psychological control in early adolescence. Child Development. 2001;72(2):583–598. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Pike A, McGuire S, Hetherington EM, Reiss D, Plomin R. Family environment and adolescent depressive symptoms and antisocial behavior?: A multivariate genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32(4):590–603. [Google Scholar]
  91. Plomin R, DeFries JC, Loehlin JC. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 1977;84(2):309–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Reitz E, Deković M, Meijer AM, Engels RCME. Longitudinal relations among parenting, best friends, and early adolescent problem behavior: Testing bidirectional effects. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2006;26(3):272–295. [Google Scholar]
  93. Scarr S, Carter-Saltzman L. Twin method: Defense of a critical assumption. Behavior Genetics. 1979;9(6):527–542. doi: 10.1007/BF01067349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Scarr S, McCartney K. How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype greater than environment effects. Child Development. 1983;54(2):424–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1983.tb03884.x. Retrieved from http://ukpmc.ac.uk/abstract/MED/6683622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Stanger C, Achenbach TM, Verhulst F. Accelerated longitudinal comparisons of aggressive versus delinquent syndromes. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;9(01):43–58. doi: 10.1017/s0954579497001053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Stice E, Barrera MJ. A longitudinal examination of the reciprocal relations between perceived parenting and adolescents’ substance use and externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology. 1994;31(2):322–335. [Google Scholar]
  97. Stormshak EA, Bierman KL, McMahon RJ, Lengua LJ. Parenting practices and child disruptive behavior problems in early elementary school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2000;29(1):17–29. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Tackett JL. Toward an externalizing spectrum in DSM-V: Incorporating developmental concerns. Child Development Perspectives. 2010;4(3):161–167. [Google Scholar]
  99. Tremblay RE. Why socialization fails: The case of chronic physical aggression. In: Lahey BB, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, editors. Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency. New York, NY: Guildford Press; 2003. pp. 182–224. [Google Scholar]
  100. Troy M, Sroufe LA. Victimization among preschoolers: role of attachment relationship history. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1987;26(2):166–72. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198703000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Tuvblad C, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA. Psychopathic personality and negative parent-to-child affect: A longitudinal cross-lag twin study. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2013;41(5):331–341. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2013.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Verona E, Patrick CJ, Lang AR. A direct assessment of the role of state and trait negative emotion in aggressive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111(2):249–258. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.2.249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES