Abstract
The conclusion that pigeons and other birds can find their way home from unfamiliar areas by means of olfactory signals is well based on a variety of experiments and supporting investigations of the chemical atmosphere. Here I argue that alternative concepts proposing other sources of geopositional information are disproved by experimental findings or, at least, are not experimentally supported and hardly realistic.
Keywords: avian navigation, homing pigeon, olfaction, atmospheric trace gases, geomagnetism, gravity, infrasound
The first scientific searches for mechanisms enabling goal-oriented avian navigation focused on the three systems used by human navigators for centuries, i.e., on the magnetic field of the earth, on celestial bodies, and on dead reckoning (in animals: path integration during the trip away from home). These candidate systems have been examined thoroughly by homing experiments with passively displaced pigeons; none of them turned out to imply a ‘map’ function providing information on the birds’ current position relative to their home site.1 An unexpected sense, however, olfaction, which in theory had never been considered potentially useful for long-distance navigation, proved to be crucial. With sectioned olfactory nerves, pigeons failed to orient homeward.2 Since that discovery in 1971, a large range of empirical evidence has been accumulated1-8 which would not have left any doubt that homing pigeons find their way home over hundreds of kilometers by deducing positional information from atmospheric trace gases perceived by the sense of smell, if such a conclusion would not have provoked intuitive disbelief. Despite this understandable scepticism, the study supplemented by this Addendum article7 confirms that a navigation system using natural airborne volatiles is a realistic model.
In the following, I substantiate my conclusion that no competing alternative or additional mechanism is recognizable that might explain how pigeons navigate home from unfamiliar areas. To prevent any misunderstandings, it should be noted that the question is not whether a particular sensory input is involved in the homing process as a whole, but only whether it provides information on the current position of the birds relative to their home site, i.e., so-called ‘map’ information. There is general agreement that birds have ‘compasses’, even two, a sun compass and a magnetic compass,1,5,9,10 and that these compasses are necessary components of various considered homing systems. Furthermore, it should be emphasized that I exclusively discuss home-finding from reliably unfamiliar areas where any knowledge of learned local features (e.g., visual landmarks) cannot be helpful.
The Potential Sensory Basis of an Avian ‘Map’ as Conceived by Humans
In most theories, navigation toward a defined home site based on external geopositional signals (i.e., any mechanism except egocentric path integration11) requires some kind of coordinates, in physical terms gradients of some quantifiable variables, whose compass alignments must be known in order to enable extrapolation from the home position to any more or less distant position in any arbitrary direction. It is well known that pigeons can obtain this knowledge (if actually necessary) while living at their home site in an aviary (Figs. 3.7,1 3.81) and hence while they cannot determine the compass alignments of increase and decrease of possibly relevant variables during exploration flights around their loft. This general problem is not solved by any preconceived navigation hypothesis that proposes the use of a compass-aligned coordinate system (unless it proposes inherited knowledge which would fit only if coordinates were worldwide equally oriented).
Sensing geomagnetism
In a global scale, the magnetic field of the earth has, in principle, a north-south gradient of total intensity as well as of inclination. Even on a global map, however, the isolines of the two variables do not at all run in parallel and west-east everywhere, but in some regions they deviate from each other by up to 90° so that, in theory, they could be used there as two coordinates (Fig. 112). In most areas, however, they can hardly be used that way. On smaller regional and local scales, the geomagnetic topography is often so irregular and unpredictable that extrapolations from one position to another without local exploration are impossible.
Various responses of pigeons to various kinds of artificial magnetic fields or to natural anomalies have been reported. As far as they did not clearly concern compass orientation, they were mostly suspected to affect a ‘map sense’. However, no kind of artificial magnetic treatment or sectioning of presumably relevant nerves have prevented pigeons from returning to their loft at speeds similar to those of untreated control birds.1,13-16 Some observed effects on initial orientation alone, i.e., without an effect on homing performance, merely indicate that magnetic stimuli exert some transient disturbance of the birds’ behavior, but they do not prove that the pigeons derive relevant ‘map cues’ from magnetic stimuli (cf. Figures 6.4,1 6.51). Similarly, proven or claimed irregular or in a particular way oriented responses of pigeons flying over a local topography with variably oriented gradients of magnetic intensity17-19 do not imply evidence that magnetic signals are used to guide the way home.
Observing celestial bodies
From the 1950s to the early 1970s, the sun (and the stellar sky for migratory birds) was the focus of experimentation and debate not only as a basis of compass orientation, but also as a potential basis of goal-oriented ‘map’ navigation by pigeons.20,21 I refrain from discussing results and arguments for and against sun navigation as for about four decades the topic has no longer been promoted by anybody.1,22,23
Sensing force of gravity
Recently, Blaser et al.24 suggested a “gravity vector theory” which proposes that pigeons determine the direction of the gravity vector, i.e., the vertical, at their home site and in some way memorize its absolute alignment in space, thus keeping it available at any other location as well. When displaced to a distant site, they deduce from the angle between the vertical measured there and the memorized home vertical the direction (azimuth) and possibly the distance toward home (1° difference = 111 km distance). The logic of this procedure is simple (see Figure 125); in principle, it would be easily applicable by an extraterrestrial player, provided that he is sensitive enough to measure the tilt angle of ca. 0.5° between the verticals determined at two sites 50 km apart from each other together with its azimuthal orientation. It remains unclear, however, how the underlying physical and neural machinery might operate in the real earthbound world in which, as far as I know, only the centripetal gravity vector at its current position is accessible to an organism. If the pigeons, nevertheless, would refer to the azimuth of the tilt angle, they would not need an extra compass and should not be affected by a shift of their time-dependent sun compass (even if they would use the current arbitrary sun position to stay on course). In reality, however, the initial bearings of pigeons are deflected away from the tilt azimuth by a clock-shift, in unfamiliar areas by an angle well corresponding to the angle between expected and real sun azimuth; thus, not even some conflict is indicated (Fig. 5.2,1 C vs. A).
To test for possible influences of gravity anomalies, Blaser et al. released pigeons from two lofts, one situated in a strong anomaly, the other 8 km apart in a gravitationally almost quiet area, at a site about 50 km distant in the same direction from both lofts. Initial orientation and paths of the two groups were clearly different and are interpreted as being related to the topographical isolines of gravity. However, responses to other features, e.g., visual or olfactory, and/or effects of preceding training releases from different directions to the two lofts are also possible. A single descriptive case study cannot reveal causal connections.
Hearing infrasound
During the 1970s and somewhat later, infrasonic waves propagating from oceanic coasts, mountain ridges etc. along the earth’s surface over hundreds and thousands of kilometers were considered to assess their possible suitability as a regional triangulation network.26,27 Pigeons can perceive infrasound at frequencies as low as 0.05 Hz, provided that their inner ear with the cochlea is intact.27 With their cochleae removed, however, pigeons homed from unfamiliar areas over a distance of 150–160 km at the same speeds as control birds and their initial bearings were clearly oriented toward home.28
Irrespective of these findings, Hagstrum has revitalized the idea of “infrasonic ‘map’ cues” in a number of more recent publications.29,30 They include, however, neither a clearly described concept of a theoretical navigation system based on infrasound nor any indication exceeding the level of assumption that particular orientation patterns of pigeons might have been elicited by hearing infrasonic input. No statistical evidence of anything is presented (see Supplemental Material).
Sensing undefined stimuli
Many related reviews and other publications remain open about the types of physical parameters used by pigeons to determine their current position relative to their home site, merely mentioning a number of debated possibilities and arguments without reaching evaluative decisions. Some studies explicitly question or exclude that any ‘map cues’ used by pigeons are perceived by olfaction, although without suggesting an alternative sensory input31-33 (see ref. 8). Other studies operate with fictitious “geophysical gradients” as if they were realities.9,10 However, waiting for the emergence of yet undiscovered geophysical factors and sensory inputs, while denying the proved crucial role of olfactory cues, implies no perspective for a solution to the problem of goal-oriented navigation.
Using a multifactorial ‘map’ system
Indecisiveness about crucial navigational mechanisms has sometimes led to the assumption that pigeon navigation relies on a multitude of environmental signals (e.g., magnetic, olfactory, infrasonic, and/or unknown) to obtain information on the direction toward home. None of these signals is thought to be generally essential because birds may opportunistically employ different types of cue depending on varying circumstances.9,10,34 However, as long as the components of such a redundant system are not clearly determined and checked for their functional involvement, it explains everything and nothing.
Not all of the factors involved in the homing flight of a pigeon are components of a navigational process, and therefore not all of them must be counted as ‘map factors’. Some factors merely distract the bird from a course it would have chosen on a purely navigational basis. Most obvious are distractions caused by the visual topography, e.g., attraction by a village or avoiding flying over a forest (cf. Figure 3.211), but magnetic noise during flight over a magnetic anomaly also appears to temporarily distract pigeons from flying homeward more directly (what they do when magnetoreception is prevented,19 thus indicating that it is not part of the home-finding system).
A redundant multifactorial, at least bifactorial home-guiding system is actually used within areas that have been familiarized during preceding homing flights, so that henceforth the pigeons can refer to learned visual landmarks in addition to olfactory cues1,5 (cf. Figures 8.1,1 8.51). However, such vision-guided homing35,36 using a ‘familiar area map’ must not be confused with ‘true’ navigation, which, in its pure form, can only be investigated at locations sufficiently far away from any visually distinctive familiar features. Pigeons having reliably no olfactory access to natural air before and after take-off, if released reliably without visual access to familiar landmarks, are, according to current knowledge, unable to direct their courses homeward.1,3-8 Thus, under these conditions no nonolfactory ‘map cues’ can help.
The Real Sensory Basis of an Avian ‘Map’ as Evolved by Birds
Birds were obviously more successful than humans in detecting environmental parameters that are useful for home-finding over fairly short distances (20–50 km) as well as over distances that extend (at least in some regions) to 200, 300, and > 500 km. A large volume of empirical evidence has been presented and discussed elsewhere,1,3-7 which induces the conclusion that the chemical atmosphere is the decisive medium and that olfaction is the decisive sense that enable goal-oriented navigation over unfamiliar territories.
(Also this conclusion has not been left without contradiction. Suggesting that olfactory signals do not contain navigational information, but merely activate an unknown 'non-olfactory map system', Jorge et al.32,33 deny even this remaining sensory basis of pigeon homing and thus drop back to a zero point of any understanding of goal-oriented avian navigation. However, their inconsistent ‘activation hypothesis’ does not withstand critical inspection and is incompatible with a number of experimental findings.8 In their reply to my critique, Phillips and Jorge37 do not challenge my arguments.)
Olfactory navigation, as I presently see it,1,5,7 implies a solution of a problem that other hypotheses based on gradients can hardly resolve (see above): birds do not need to move across environmental gradients during exploration flights to determine their alignments, because the atmospheric ratio gradients are moved by winds across the birds at home, even if they are confined in an aviary (cf. Figure 16).
In the context of navigation, I am reluctant to speak about atmospheric odors, because we immediately associate the word odor with our subjective perceptions and connected emotions. The processing of airborne chemosignals by navigating birds, however, which must not allow adaptation to continuous olfactory inputs, is certainly not fully analogous to our everyday experience of the sense of smell. Therefore, we should be cautious in drawing rash conclusions about what appears possible or impossible in a field that we erroneously may consider trivial and well known to ourselves.
Concluding Remarks
It appears unlikely that a global system of any kind (geomagnetism, celestial bodies, gravity) can be used by animals for goal-oriented navigation over hundreds of kilometers as well as down to 50 and 20 km. On the other hand, a regional system such as olfactory navigation must have and obviously has some upper range limits which appear to be quite variable depending on relevant geographical conditions (cf. Figures 7.15–171). The title of this article restricts the problem to pigeon homing, although olfactory navigation is certainly applied by most or all other avian navigators as well.1,5,38 I do not exclude that some of them, covering in their migrations several thousand kilometers, make use of additional less precise nonolfactory, most probably magnetic global indicators of position or at least of latitude.39 Such coarse indicators operating in larger scales, however, are inappropriate for guidance to a small familiar home area around a geographically well-defined location.
All the aforementioned alternatives to olfactory navigation are theoretically unconvincing and experimentally disproved or, at least, not supported in any way. There is no indication that pigeons, when passively displaced to definitely unfamiliar areas, can gain any nonolfactory information about their position relative to their home loft. The olfactory information, however, which is obviously available to the pigeons, is not yet deciphered in its material components, i.e., the chemical compounds actually used by birds are not yet identified and the formation of the observed regularities in the gradual changes of their proportional composition in the atmosphere over hundreds of kilometers1,5-7 is not yet coherently elucidated. Instead of perpetuating doubts about the existence of olfactory navigation without having a competing alternative in mind, we should concentrate future efforts of research on trying to decode the functionality of olfactory navigation and to understand its atmospheric basis.
Supplementary Material
Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest
No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.
References
- 1.Wallraff HG. Avian Navigation: Pigeon Homing as a Paradigm. Berlin:Springer, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Papi F, Fiore L, Fiaschi V, Benvenuti S. The influence of olfactory nerve section on the homing capacity of carrier pigeons. Monit Zool Ital (NS) 1971;5:265–7. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Papi F. Pigeon navigation: solved problems and open questions. Monit Zool Ital (NS) 1986;20:471–517. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Papi F. Olfactory navigation. In: Berthold P, ed. Orientation in Birds. Basel:Birkhäuser, 1991:52-85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wallraff HG. Avian olfactory navigation: its empirical foundation and conceptual state. Anim Behav. 2004;67:189–204. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.06.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gagliardo A. Forty years of olfactory navigation in birds. J Exp Biol. 2013;216:2165–71. doi: 10.1242/jeb.070250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wallraff HG. Ratios among atmospheric trace gases together with winds imply exploitable information for bird navigation: a model elucidating experimental results. Biogeosciences. 2013;10:6929–43. doi: 10.5194/bg-10-6929-2013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wallraff HG. Do olfactory stimuli provide positional information for home-oriented avian navigation? Anim Behav. 2014;90:e1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W. Avian navigation. Auk. 2009;126:717–43. doi: 10.1525/auk.2009.11009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wiltschko R. Navigation without technical aids: how pigeons find their way home. Eur J Nav. 2012;10:22–31. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wallraff HG. Path integration by passively displaced homing pigeons? Anim Behav. 2000;60:F30–6. doi: 10.1006/S0003-3472(00)91600-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Boström JE, Åkesson S, Alerstam T. Where on earth can animals use a geomagnetic bi-coordinate map for navigation? Ecography. 2012;35:1039–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0587.2012.07507.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wallraff HG. The magnetic map of homing pigeons: an evergreen phantom. J Theor Biol. 1999;197:265–9. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1998.0874. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gagliardo A, Ioalè P, Savini M, Wild M. Navigational abilities of homing pigeons deprived of olfactory or trigeminally mediated magnetic information when young. J Exp Biol. 2008;211:2046–51. doi: 10.1242/jeb.017608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mora CV, Walker MM. Consistent effect of an attached magnet on the initial orientation of homing pigeons, Columba livia. Anim Behav. 2012;84:377–83. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Holland R, Filannino C, Gagliardo A. A magnetic pulse does not affect homing pigeon navigation: a GPS tracking experiment. J Exp Biol. 2013;216:2192–200. doi: 10.1242/jeb.083543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dennis TE, Rayner MJ, Walker MM. Evidence that pigeons orient to geomagnetic intensity during homing. Proc Biol Sci. 2007;274:1153–8. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.3768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Mora CV, Walker MM. Do release-site biases reflect response to the Earth’s magnetic field during position determination by homing pigeons? Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276:3295–302. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wiltschko R, Schiffner I, Fuhrmann P, Wiltschko W. The role of the magnetite-based receptors in the beak in pigeon homing. Curr Biol. 2010;20:1534–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Matthews GVT. Sun navigation in homing pigeons. J Exp Biol. 1953;30:243–67. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pennycuick CJ. The physical basis of astro-navigation in birds: theoretical considerations. J Exp Biol. 1960;37:573–93. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kramer G. Experiments on bird orientation and their interpretation. Ibis. 1957;99:196–227. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-919X.1957.tb01947.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Keeton WT. The orientational and navigational basis of homing in birds. Adv Stud Behav. 1974;5:47–132. doi: 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60020-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Blaser N, Guskov SI, Meskenaite V, Kanevskyi VA, Lipp H-P. Altered orientation and flight paths of pigeons reared on gravity anomalies: a GPS tracking study. PLoS One. 2013;8:e77102. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Köhler K-L. Do pigeons use their eyes for navigation? A new technique! In: Schmidt-Koenig K, Keeton WT, Eds. Animal Migration, Navigation, and Homing. Berlinm Springer, 1978:57-64. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wallraff HG. Weitere Volierenversuche mit Brieftauben: wahrscheinlicher Einfluß dynamischer Faktoren der Atmosphäre auf die Orientierung. Z Vgl Physiol. 1970;68:182–201. doi: 10.1007/BF00297694. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kreithen ML, Quine DB. Infrasound detection by the homing pigeon: a behavioral audiogram. J Comp Physiol. 1979;129:1–4. doi: 10.1007/BF00679906. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wallraff HG. Homing of pigeons after extirpation of their cochleae and lagenae. Nat New Biol. 1972;236:223–4. doi: 10.1038/newbio236223a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hagstrum JT. Infrasound and the avian navigational map. J Exp Biol. 2000;203:1103–11. doi: 10.1242/jeb.203.7.1103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hagstrum JT. Atmospheric propagation modeling indicates homing pigeons use loft-specific infrasonic ‘map’ cues. J Exp Biol. 2013;216:687–99. doi: 10.1242/jeb.072934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wiltschko R. The function of olfactory input in pigeon orientation: does it provide navigational information or play another role? J Exp Biol. 1996;199:113–9. doi: 10.1242/jeb.199.1.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jorge PE, Marques AE, Phillips JB. Activational rather than navigational effects of odors on homing of young pigeons. Curr Biol. 2009;19:650–4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jorge PE, Marques PAM, Phillips JB. Activational effects of odours on avian navigation. Proc Biol Sci. 2010;277:45–9. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Walcott C. Multi-modal orientation cues in homing pigeons. Integr Comp Biol. 2005;45:574–81. doi: 10.1093/icb/45.3.574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schiffner I, Wiltschko R. Development of the navigational system in homing pigeons: increase in complexity of the navigational map. J Exp Biol. 2013;216:2675–81. doi: 10.1242/jeb.085662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Guilford T, Biro D. Route following and the pigeon’s familiar area map. J Exp Biol. 2014;217:169–79. doi: 10.1242/jeb.092908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Phillips JB, Jorge PE. Olfactory navigation: failure to attempt replication of critical experiments keeps controversy alive. Reply to Wallraff. Anim Behav. 2014;90:e7–9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gagliardo A, Bried J, Lambardi P, Luschi P, Wikelski M, Bonadonna F. Oceanic navigation in Cory’s shearwaters: evidence for a crucial role of olfactory cues for homing after displacement. J Exp Biol. 2013;216:2798–805. doi: 10.1242/jeb.085738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bingman VP, Cheng K. Mechanisms of animal global navigation: comparative perspectives and enduring challenges. Ethol Ecol Evol. 2005;17:295–318. doi: 10.1080/08927014.2005.9522584. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.