Abstract
Background
Practitioners and researchers are interested in assessing children’s dietary intake and physical activity together to maximize resources and minimize subject burden.
Objective
To investigate differences in dietary and/or physical-activity recall accuracy by content (diet-only; physical-activity-only; diet-&-physical-activity), retention interval (same-day-recalls-in-the-afternoon; previous-day-recalls-in-the-morning), and grade (third; fifth).
Design
Children (n=144; 66% African American, 13% White, 12% Hispanic, 9% Other; 50% girls) from four schools were randomly selected for interviews about one of three contents. Each content group was equally divided by retention interval, each equally divided by grade, each equally divided by sex. Information concerning diet and physical activity at school was validated with school-provided breakfast and lunch observations, and accelerometry, respectively. Dietary accuracy measures were food-item omission and intrusion rates, and kilocalorie correspondence rate and inflation ratio. Physical activity accuracy measures were absolute and arithmetic differences for moderate-to-vigorous-physical-activity minutes.
Statistical analyses performed
For each accuracy measure, linear models determined effects of content, retention interval, grade, and their two-way and three-way interactions; ethnicity and sex were control variables.
Results
Content was significant within four interactions: intrusion rate (content-×-retention-interval-×-grade; p=.0004), correspondence rate (content-×-grade; p=.0004), inflation ratio (content-×-grade; p=.0104), and arithmetic difference (content-×-retention-interval-×-grade; p=.0070). Retention interval was significant for correspondence rate (p=.0004), inflation ratio (p=.0014), and three interactions: omission rate (retention-interval-×-grade; p=.0095), intrusion rate, and arithmetic difference (both already mentioned). Grade was significant for absolute difference (p=.0233) and five interactions mentioned. Content effects depended on other factors. Grade effects were mixed. Dietary accuracy was better with same-day than previous-day retention interval.
Conclusions
Results do not support integrating dietary intake and physical activity in children’s recalls, but do support using shorter rather than longer retention intervals to yield more accurate dietary recalls. Further validation studies need to clarify age effects and identify evidence-based practices to improve children’s accuracy for recalling dietary intake and/or physical activity.
Keywords: children, recall accuracy, dietary recall, physical activity, school
INTRODUCTION
Parents report children’s dietary intake and/or physical activity, but studies1-4 underscore concerns about such reports. Children eat meals and conduct physical activity at school where parents are not present, so it is unrealistic to expect parents to accurately report children’s intake and/or physical activity that occur at school. Although studies rely on children to self-report either dietary intake5-12 or physical activity,13 children’s reporting accuracy is of concern.
Validation studies of children’s dietary recalls have identified omissions (items eaten but unreported) and intrusions (items uneaten but reported);3,14-24 results show that dietary recall accuracy is improved when the retention interval (elapsed time between to-be-reported meals and the interview) is minimized.15,25,26 Accelerometers have been increasingly used to study children’s physical activity objectively,27 but self-report instruments are more common, especially for large studies,28,29 more economical, and provide information about the type and context of physical activity that accelerometers cannot.30 Considering the childhood obesity epidemic31 and that schools are common targets for obesity prevention and health promotion,32-34 there is interest for integrating assessment of children’s dietary intake and physical activity to maximize resources and minimize subject burden.35
Children have simultaneously reported dietary intake and physical activity in two validation studies. One study35 evaluated self-administered recall software to simultaneously assess diet and physical activity. Seven-to-15-year-old children (n=121) wore accelerometers; the next day, children completed a self-administered diet-&-physical-activity recall, and then an interviewer-administered diet-only recall. Results showed good dietary agreement between self-administered and interviewer-administered recalls, and good physical activity agreement between the self-administered recall and accelerometry.35 Limitations included no physical-activity-only recalls to compare to diet-&-physical-activity recalls; relative validity for dietary intake was back-to-back recalls (with self-administered recalls always first) with assessment for food groups only; and using previous-day retention interval only.
Another study, a pilot,36 observed 32 children (third-grade and fifth-grade) eating school-provided meals, and interviewed each child once (in the afternoon about that day or in the morning about the previous day) to obtain a diet-only or diet-&-physical-activity recall. Dietary accuracy results showed differences by retention interval (better for same-day than previous-day) but not by content or grade. Confidence intervals suggested that larger studies would find no difference by content, but might by grade.36 Limitations included the small sample and no assessment of physical activity recall accuracy.
This study’s objective was to investigate differences in dietary and/or physical activity recall accuracy by content (diet-only; physical-activity-only; diet-&-physical-activity), retention interval (same-day recalls in the afternoon; previous-day recalls in the morning), and grade (third; fifth). Dietary intake and physical activity at school were validated with direct observation of school-provided meals and accelerometry, respectively. Accuracy was hypothesized to be better for the integrated (diet-&-physical-activity) than single (diet-only; physical-activity-only) content, shorter (same-day recalls in the afternoon) than longer (previous-day recalls in the morning) retention interval, and older (fifth-grade) than younger (third-grade) children. Interview length was anticipated to be longer with the integrated than single content.
METHODS
The Institutional Review Board for research involving humans approved the study. Written parental consent and child assent were obtained.
Sample size calculations
Before data collection, past studies’ results15,18-20 were used to estimate omission rates of 28% for same-day recalls in the afternoon and 57% for previous-day recalls in the morning, and intrusion rates of 12% for same-day recalls in the afternoon and 36% for previous-day recalls in the morning. With 144 children overall, within a fixed-effects analysis of variance (ANOVA) including two grades, three contents (although the eventual models considered only two at a time), and two retention intervals, main effects tests had 75% and 89% power to reject equality for omission rates and intrusion rates, respectively, in models with two- and three-way interactions (where s=0.25 for unspecified effects). Power calculations used PASS (2005, Kaysville, UT).
Participants
Data were collected during the 2010-2011 school year. Children from 21 third-grade and 21 fifth-grade classes at four elementary schools in one district were invited to participate. At these schools, eligibility to receive free or reduced-price school-provided meals was 40.3% to 54.0%. Of the 877 children invited to participate, 513 children (58.5%) agreed. Grade, ethnicity, and sex composition of participants was similar to that of children invited.
From the consented children across all schools and grades, three subsets of children were randomly selected. Subsets One and Three’s children were observed eating school-provided breakfast and lunch in the cafeteria on a school day; Subset Three’s children also wore accelerometers at school that same day. Subset Two’s Children wore accelerometers at school on a school day. Each subset child was interviewed only once about time at school for his/her observation and/or accelerometer day (i.e., from arrival at school until school dismissed) with content as diet-only (Subset One), physical-activity-only (Subset Two), or diet-&-physical-activity (Subset Three). Data collection continued until 144 children were interviewed and, as Figure 1 shows, each subset had 48 children with 24 per retention interval, and within retention interval, 12 per grade with six per sex. School staff and children did not know in advance when observations and/or interviews would occur, when accelerometers would be worn, nor assignment to content and/or retention interval. More children were recruited than needed to ensure random selection, so children could not determine who specifically was being observed, and so more children wore accelerometers than were interviewed. When recruited, children were told that they might each be interviewed zero to two times, so that being interviewed did not indicate that a child would not be interviewed again.
Direct meal observations
School-provided meals were observed by three researchers trained to follow a written protocol using established procedures through practice and assessment of pre-data-collection of interobserver reliability.15,18-20,25,36 Before and weekly throughout data collection, interobserver reliability was assessed for pairs of observers using established procedures.15,19,20,25,36,37 During data collection, interobserver reliability was assessed on 23 children (12 girls) for breakfast and 20 children (10 girls) for lunch; mean agreement between observers to within one-fourth serving on amounts eaten was acceptable (98%—breakfast ; 94%—lunch). For non-interobserver reliability observations, each researcher observed one to three children simultaneously during regular meal periods. Children were seated using their school’s typical arrangement. Observations covered entire meal periods to account for food trading.21,38-40 Researchers used paper forms to record items and amounts eaten in servings of standardized school-meal portions.
Accelerometry
ActiGraph accelerometers (Model GT3X, The Actigraph, LLC; Pensacola, FL) were placed on children as they arrived for breakfast in the school cafeteria for the specified day of wear and worn until researchers collected monitors in the afternoon before school dismissed. Children wore monitors on elastic belts on their right hips (anterior to iliac crests). Accelerometers were initialized to save data in one-minute intervals.35,41,42 Data were summarized for minutes of moderate to vigorous physical activity (MVPA), calculated using age-specific cut points for a threshold of four metabolic equivalents.43 Sixty minutes of consecutive zeroes were considered non-wear time. Minutes per day of MVPA was the main outcome variable from accelerometry data.
Interviews
Three researchers conducted individual, face-to-face interviews in private locations at school after lunch on Mondays through Fridays (for same-day recalls in the afternoon), and after breakfast on Tuesdays through Fridays (for previous-day recalls in the morning). Although all three researchers also conducted observations, a child’s interviewer had not observed that child’s meals. Interviewers were trained using modeling, practice, and assessment of pre-data-collection quality control for interviews. Six written multiple-pass interview protocols were created by crossing three contents with two retention intervals. Interview protocols, described in Figure 2, were modeled on the Nutrition Data System for Research protocol (Nutrition Coordinating Center, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN) and adapted for retention interval from past studies.15,25,36 Researchers incorporated physical activity information into the appropriate interview protocols based on a qualitative study of interviewer-administered physical activity recalls by children.44 Children reported amounts eaten in servings of standardized school-meal portions using qualitative terms (Figure 2, footnote b) as in past studies.15,18-20,25,36 Interviewers used paper forms to document beginning and ending times, and to note information children reported. Interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed. Each interviewed child was mailed a $10 check. Quality control for interviews was assessed using established procedures;15,19,20,25,36,45 a non-interviewing researcher reviewed each interview’s audio-recording and typed transcript for protocol adherence. Ten children’s interviews failed to abide by the specified protocol. Another five children’s interviews had inadequate accelerometer data. These 15 children’s interviews were excluded from further analyses, and replaced by another 15 children’s interviews.
Outcome measures
Dietary
Accuracy was assessed for only the school-meal parts of recalls because only school meals were observed. As in past studies,15,18-20,25,36 for reported items to be treated as reports about school meals, children had to identify “school” as the location where items were eaten, refer to breakfast as “school breakfast” or “breakfast”, refer to lunch as “school lunch” or “lunch”, and report mealtimes to within one hour of observed mealtimes.
For each meal per child, there were two sets of foods; one set contained foods observed eaten, and the other set contained foods reported eaten. According to an established classification system,14-16,18-20,25,36,46-49 foods in both sets were matches, foods only in the reported set were intrusions, and foods only in the observed set were omissions. As in past studies,15,18-20,25,36 a meal-component weight was assigned to each food observed eaten and/or reported eaten at a school meal (Table 1, footnote b).
Table 1.
Condition | n | Omission Ratea,b | Intrusion Rateb,c | Correspondence Rated | Inflation Ratioe | n dietary measures passedf | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
0 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | ||||||
--------Least Squares Mean (Standard Error)-------- | -------n of children------- | |||||||||
Content | ||||||||||
Diet only | 48 | 40.7 (4.8) | 26.8 (3.1) | 54.4 (3.3) | 26.9 (2.1) | 13 | 3 | 11 | 7 | 14 |
Diet & Physical Activity | 47g | 40.1 (2.8) | 28.9 (1.3) | 52.8 (1.6) | 32.4 (1.7) | 10 | 6 | 12 | 13 | 6 |
Retention Interval | ***h | ***i | ||||||||
Same-day recall in afternoon | 47g | 26.1 (3.9) | 18.0 (1.3) | 68.6 (2.5) | 25.4 (0.4) | 5 | 1 | 8 | 16 | 17 |
Previous-day recall in morning | 48 | 54.7 (3.7) | 37.7 (4.1) | 38.5 (2.2) | 34.0 (1.9) | 18 | 8 | 15 | 4 | 3 |
Grade | ||||||||||
Third | 47g | 43.5 (3.8) | 28.5 (2.5) | 48.8 (0.7) | 27.3 (2.7) | 14 | 6 | 12 | 8 | 7 |
Fifth | 48 | 37.3 (3.7) | 27.3 (2.0) | 58.4 (3.9) | 32.0 (2.2) | 9 | 3 | 11 | 12 | 13 |
Content by Retention Interval | ||||||||||
Diet only - Same-day recall in afternoon | 24 | 25.9 (5.6) | 16.7 (0.4) | 68.8 (2.2) | 18.7 (3.0) | 2 | 1 | 3 | 6 | 12 |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in afternoon | 23g | 26.2 (2.5) | 19.4 (2.5) | 68.4 (3.0) | 32.0 (2.7) | 3 | 0 | 5 | 10 | 5 |
Diet only - Previous-day recall in morning | 24 | 55.4 (4.3) | 37.0 (6.0) | 39.9 (4.5) | 35.1 (7.0) | 11 | 2 | 8 | 1 | 2 |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in morning | 24 | 54.0 (3.1) | 38.5 (2.2) | 37.2 (1.5) | 32.8 (5.0) | 7 | 6 | 7 | 3 | 1 |
Content by Grade | ***j | *k | ||||||||
Diet only - Third | 24 | 45.5 (6.2) | 34.5 (3.4) | 46.6 (2.3) | 31.0 (3.1) | 9 | 3 | 4 | 4 | 4 |
Diet & physical activity - Third | 23g | 41.4 (1.7) | 22.5 (1.6) | 50.9 (1.0) | 22.9 (2.4) | 5 | 3 | 8 | 4 | 3 |
Diet only - Fifth | 24 | 35.8 (3.5) | 19.2 (4.2) | 62.1 (4.3) | 23.6 (2.7) | 4 | 0 | 7 | 3 | 10 |
Diet & physical activity - Fifth | 24 | 38.9 (3.9) | 35.4 (0.9) | 54.7 (3.6) | 41.2 (1.1) | 5 | 3 | 4 | 9 | 3 |
Retention Interval by Grade | **l | |||||||||
Same-day recall in afternoon - Third | 23g | 33.1 (5.1) | 18.4 (3.3) | 61.9 (2.5) | 20.7 (6.4) | 5 | 1 | 5 | 6 | 6 |
Previous-day recall in morning - Third | 24 | 53.8 (3.5) | 38.6 (5.9) | 35.7 (1.7) | 33.9 (4.1) | 9 | 5 | 7 | 2 | 1 |
Same-day recall in afternoon - Fifth | 24 | 19.0 (3.4) | 17.7 (1.3) | 75.4 (3.3) | 30.0 (5.7) | 0 | 0 | 3 | 10 | 11 |
Previous-day recall in morning - Fifth | 24 | 55.6 (4.1) | 36.9 (2.9) | 41.4 (4.6) | 34.0 (1.5) | 9 | 3 | 8 | 2 | 2 |
Content by Retention Interval by Grade | ***m | |||||||||
Diet only - Same-day recall in afternoon - Third | 12 | 32.9 (9.4) | 17.3 (2.3) | 59.4 (3.6) | 16.7 (6.9) | 2 | 1 | 2 | 4 | 3 |
Diet only - Same-day-recall in afternoon - Fifth | 12 | 18.9 (2.9) | 16.1 (2.4) | 78.3 (2.0) | 20.7 (3.9) | 0 | 0 | 1 | 2 | 9 |
Diet only - Previous-day recall in morning - Third | 12 | 58.1 (4.5) | 51.7 (7.5) | 33.8 (2.4) | 45.3 (6.2) | 7 | 2 | 2 | 0 | 1 |
Diet only - Previous-day recall in morning - Fifth | 12 | 52.7 (4.4) | 22.2 (6.2) | 46.0 (7.1) | 25.0 (8.2) | 4 | 0 | 6 | 1 | 1 |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in afternoon - Third | 11g | 33.3 (1.0) | 19.5 (4.4) | 64.3 (1.4) | 24.6 (6.0) | 3 | 0 | 3 | 2 | 3 |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in afternoon - Fifth | 12 | 19.2 (3.9) | 19.2 (1.3) | 72.5 (4.7) | 39.4 (10.7) | 0 | 0 | 2 | 8 | 2 |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in morning - Third | 12 | 49.5 (2.4) | 25.5 (4.4) | 37.6 (3.3) | 22.6 (2.0) | 2 | 3 | 5 | 2 | 0 |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in morning - Fifth | 12 | 58.6 (3.9) | 51.5 (0.6) | 36.8 (2.5) | 43.1 (10.9) | 5 | 3 | 2 | 1 | 1 |
Omission rate = percentage of items observed eaten but unreported calculated as (sum of weighted omissions / [sum of weighted omissions + sum of weighted matches]) × 100%; values range from 0% to 100% with smaller rates reflecting better accuracy.
Meal-component weight assigned to each food item with combination entrée = 2, condiment = .33, and remaining meal components = 1. Children reported amounts eaten in servings of standardized school-meal portions using qualitative terms none, a taste, a little bit, half, most, all, and more than 1 serving. Amounts observed and/or reported eaten were scored in servings of standardized school-meal portions as none = 0, a taste = .1, a little bit = .25, half = .5, most = .75, all = 1, or actual number of servings if > 1.
Intrusion rate = percentage of items reported eaten but unobserved calculated as (sum of weighted intrusions / [sum of weighted intrusions + sum of weighted matches]) × 100%; defined values range from 0% to 100% with smaller rates reflecting better accuracy.
Correspondence rate = (corresponding amounts in kilocalories from matches / observed amounts in kilocalories) × 100; values may range from 0% (indicating that nothing observed was reported eaten) to 100% (indicating that all observed items and observed amounts were reported correctly); higher rates reflect better accuracy.
Inflation ratio = ([over-reported amounts in kilocalories from matches and intrusions] / observed amounts in kilocalories) × 100; values may range from 0% (indicating no intrusions and no over-reported amounts of matches) to infinity (because there is no limit on what a child can report); lower ratios reflect better accuracy.
Similar to methods used in past studies to define acceptable dietary recall accuracy,18-20 arbitrary criteria was applied which established omission rates, intrusion rates, and inflation ratios of ≤30% as passing and >30% as failing, and correspondence rates of ≥70% as passing and <70% as failing. The numbers of children in the right-hand columns show that the estimated marginal probability of passing all four dietary measures was higher for the single than integrated content, for the shorter than longer retention interval, and for the fifth than third grade. Joint probabilities of passing all four measures were highest for the single content–shorter retention interval, single content–fifth grade, and shorter retention interval–fifth grade combinations, as well as for the single content–shorter retention interval–fifth grade combination.
One third-grade girl’s interview with diet-&-physical activity content and same-day recall in the afternoon retention interval had to be dropped during analyses when it was discovered that the accelerometer had recorded less than one hour of data.
Correspondence rate was better with same-day recalls in the afternoon (68.6%) than previous-day recalls in the morning (38.5%).
Inflation ratio was better with same-day recalls in the afternoon (25.4%) than previous-day recalls in the morning (34.0%).
Correspondence rate was better for third-grade children for diet-&-physical-activity (50.9%) than diet-only content (46.6%), but for fifth-grade children for diet-only (62.1%) than diet-&-physical-activity content (54.7%).
Inflation ratio was better for third-grade children for diet-&-physical-activity (22.9%) than diet-only (31.0%) content, but for fifth-grade children for diet-only (23.6%) than diet-&-physical-activity (41.2%) content.
Omission rate was better for fifth-grade (19.0%) than third-grade children (33.1%) with same-day recalls in the afternoon, but similar by grade (53.8%; 55.6%) with previous-day recalls in the morning.
Intrusion rate was better for third-grade children for diet-&-physical-activity (25.5%) than diet-only content (51.7%) with previous-day recalls in the morning, but similar by content (17.3%; 19.5%) with same-day recalls in the afternoon. Intrusion rate was better for fifth-grade children for diet-only (22.2%) than diet-&-physical-activity content (51.5%) with previous-day recalls in the morning, but similar by content (16.1%; 19.2%) with same-day recalls in the afternoon.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001
For each child’s two school meals, weighted matches, omissions, and intrusions were summed, and the child’s omission rate—percentage of items observed eaten but unreported—and intrusion rate—percentage of items reported eaten but unobserved—were calculated (Table 1, footnotes a and c). Smaller values for omission and intrusion rates indicate better accuracy.
Amounts observed and/or reported eaten were scored in servings of standardized school-meal portions (Table 1, footnote b) as in past studies.15,18-20,25,36 Children’s accuracy for reporting energy intake (in kilocalories) was assessed as in past studies.50-55 For each observed and/or reported item, the Nutrition Data System for Research database was used to obtain kilocalories for standard school-meal portions. For each match, (a) the reported amount corresponded exactly to the observed amount, (b) the reported amount corresponded to part of the observed amount and the rest of the observed amount was unreported, or (c) part of the reported amount corresponded to the observed amount and the rest was over-reported. For each omission, the entire observed amount was unreported. For each intrusion, the entire reported amount was over-reported. Each corresponding, unreported, and over-reported amount was multiplied by the per-serving kilocalorie value to obtain corresponding, unreported, and over-reported kilocalories for each item. These amounts were summed for a child’s two school meals, and correspondence rate—percentage of kilocalories observed eaten and reported correctly—and inflation ratio—percentage of kilocalories reported eaten but unobserved—were calculated for each child (Table 1, footnotes d and e). Larger values for correspondence rate, but smaller values for inflation ratio, indicate better accuracy.
Physical activity
Physical activity information from interviews was scored to estimate MVPA minutes per day at school as follows: Reported physical activity was considered to be of at least moderate intensity when children reported medium or fast pace. Minutes reported as medium or fast pace were summed to create the interview MVPA variable. This variable excluded physical activity reported as slow pace, for which pace was not reported, for which duration was not reported in minutes, and reported as part of a transition period (e.g., changing classes, going to bathroom or lunch). When ranges of minutes spent in physical activity were reported, the lower end was used because it was anticipated that children would over estimate their MVPA. Analyses included physical activity reported during periods of monitor wear only. During analyses, it was discovered that one accelerometer recorded less than one hour of data; thus, that interview was dropped, which brought Subset Three’s final sample analyzed to 47 children.
Children’s accuracy for reporting MVPA was evaluated using two variables—absolute and arithmetic differences between interview MVPA and accelerometer MVPA. A child’s absolute difference close to zero indicates better accuracy by reflecting the magnitude of error for reporting MVPA minutes, but not whether under- or over-reporting occurred. For arithmetic difference (interview minus accelerometer), negative and positive values indicate average under- and over-reporting, respectively; although values close to zero indicate better accuracy, under- and over-reporting can offset each other, so a small average for a group may disguise large reporting errors balanced over the two directions.
Analyses
Schools provided children’s race/ethnicity (reported by parents) to researchers. Researchers re-categorized race/ethnicity from nine categories reported to four* due to small numbers for some.
Generalized estimating equation methodology accounted for possible correlation in response values within the same interviewer; an exchangeable correlation structure was assumed. For all models, residual analysis was performed to check assumptions of constant error variance and normality of errors. Simultaneous Wald tests jointly tested for factor effects in each model. The Benjamini-Hochberg procedure56 adjusted for simultaneous tests on multiple factors and in multiple models, controlling the false discovery rate at .05; thus, adjusted p values are shown. Statistical analyses used SAS/STAT® (Version 9.2, ©2002-2008, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) with a .05 level of significance.
Summary statistics were calculated for interview length. Significance testing was not conducted because this variable does not assess accuracy, although descriptive information is useful from a practical viewpoint (e.g., scheduling research staff time; estimating class time that children will miss for interviews).35 Each interview’s length in minutes was calculated by subtracting beginning from ending time.
Dietary
For Subsets One and Three, various linear models were fit to determine the effects of content (diet-only; diet-&-physical-activity), retention interval (same-day recalls in the afternoon; previous-day recalls in the morning), and grade (third; fifth) on dietary recall accuracy. Models included these three factors of interest and their two-way and three-way interactions with ethnicity and sex as control variables. Separate ANOVA models were fit with omission rate, intrusion rate, correspondence rate, and inflation ratio as dependent variables. Inflation ratio was square-root-transformed to satisfy the normality assumption.
Physical activity
For Subsets Two and Three, two separate linear models were fit to determine the effects of content (physical-activity-only; diet-&-physical-activity), retention interval, grade, and their two-way and three-way interactions (with ethnicity and sex as control variables) on absolute and arithmetic differences as dependent variables. Absolute difference was square-root transformed to satisfy constant-variance and normality assumptions.
RESULTS
Of the 143 children included in analyses, there were 66% African American, 13% White, 12% Hispanic, and 9% Other. There were 71 girls.
Dietary
Table 1 shows descriptives for dietary recall accuracy measures.
Omission rate
The retention-interval-×-grade interaction (p=.0095) was significant. When the retention interval was same-day recalls in the afternoon, omission rate was better for fifth-grade (19.0%) than third-grade children (33.1%). When the retention interval was previous-day recalls in the morning, omission rate was similar across grades (53.8%; 55.6%).
Intrusion rate
The content-×-retention-interval-×-grade interaction (p=.0004) was significant. For third-grade children, intrusion rate was better for diet-&-physical-activity (25.5%) than diet-only content (51.7%) when the retention interval was previous-day recalls in the morning, but similar by content (17.3%; 19.5%) when the retention interval was same-day recalls in the afternoon. For fifth-grade children, intrusion rate was better for diet-only (22.2%) than diet-&-physical-activity content (51.5%) when the retention interval was previous-day recalls in the morning, but similar by content (16.1%; 19.2%) when the retention interval was same-day recalls in the afternoon.
Correspondence rate
The content-×-grade interaction (p=.0004) and retention interval (p=.0004) were significant. For third-grade children, correspondence rate was better for diet-&-physical-activity (50.9%) than diet-only content (46.6%), but for fifth-grade children, it was better for diet-only (62.1%) than diet-&-physical-activity content (54.7%). Correspondence rate was better when the retention interval was same-day recalls in the afternoon (68.6%) than when it was previous-day recalls in the morning (38.5%).
Inflation ratio
The content-×-grade interaction (p=.0104) and retention interval (p=.0014) were significant. For third-grade children, inflation ratio was better for diet-&-physical-activity (22.9%) than diet-only (31.0%) content, but for fifth-grade children, it was better for diet-only (23.6%) than diet-&-physical-activity (41.2%) content. Inflation ratio was better when retention interval was same-day recalls in the afternoon (25.4%) than when it was previous-day recalls in the morning (34.0%).
Physical activity
Table 2 shows descriptives for total number of accelerometer-determined minutes of MVPA and physical activity recall accuracy measures.
Table 2.
Condition | n | Total number of accelerometer MVPA minutes | Absolute difference in MVPA minutesb | Arithmetic difference in MVPA minutesc |
---|---|---|---|---|
Least Squares Mean (Standard Error) | ||||
Content | ||||
Physical activity only | 48 | 21.4 (1.9) | 23.2 (1.6) | 9.0 (0.8) |
Diet & physical activity | 47d | 25.6 (1.8) | 24.0 (1.3) | 3.3 (2.8) |
Retention Interval | ||||
Same-day recall in afternoon | 47d | 24.5 (2.0) | 23.4 (0.4) | 2.3 (0.5) |
Previous-day recall in morning | 48 | 22.4 (1.7) | 23.8 (0.8) | 10.0 (3.1) |
Grade | *e | |||
Third | 47d | 27.3 (1.7) | 26.8 (0.9) | 4.1 (3.0) |
Fifth | 48 | 19.7 (1.8) | 20.4 (0.6) | 8.2 (0.5) |
Content by Retention Interval | ||||
Physical activity only - Same-day recall in afternoon | 24 | 22.3 (2.9) | 24.3 (3.1) | 5.2 (0.7) |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in afternoon | 23d | 26.8 (2.7) | 22.5 (3.4) | −0.6 (1.2) |
Physical activity only - Previous-day recall in morning | 24 | 20.5 (2.5) | 22.1 (0.6) | 12.8 (1.8) |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in morning | 24 | 24.3 (2.3) | 25.6 (1.2) | 7.1 (4.5) |
Content by Grade | ||||
Physical activity only - Third | 24 | 23.0 (2.3) | 27.2 (3.4) | 8.1 (3.5) |
Diet & physical activity - Third | 23d | 31.8 (2.2) | 26.4 (1.6) | 0.1 (2.6) |
Physical activity only - Fifth | 24 | 19.8 (3,0) | 19.2 (0.2) | 9.9 (2.0) |
Diet & physical activity - Fifth | 24 | 19.6 (2.2) | 21.7 (1.0) | 6.5 (3.0) |
Retention Interval by Grade | ||||
Same-day recall in afternoon - Third | 23d | 29.1 (2.6) | 28.8 (2.5) | 0.7 (1.6) |
Previous-day recall in morning - Third | 24 | 25.6 (2.2) | 24.7 (0.8) | 7.4 (7.6) |
Same-day recall in afternoon - Fifth | 24 | 20.1 (2.8) | 17.9 (2.9) | 3.8 (2.4) |
Previous-day recall in morning - Fifth | 24 | 19.3 (2.4) | 23.0 (1.9) | 12.5 (1.4) |
Content by Retention Interval by Grade | **f | |||
Physical activity only - Same-day recall in afternoon - Third | 12 | 26.7 (3.9) | 33.1 (8.9) | 5.9 (1.5) |
Physical activity only - Same-day recall in afternoon - Fifth | 12 | 18.0 (4.1) | 15.5 (3.0) | 4.4 (0.6) |
Physical activity only - Previous-day recall in morning - Third | 12 | 19.4 (2.1) | 21.3 (2.1) | 10.2 (8.0) |
Physical activity only - Previous-day recall in morning - Fifth | 12 | 21.7 (4.5) | 22.9 (2.5) | 15.4 (4.5) |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in afternoon - Third | 11d | 31.8 (3.4) | 24.6 (3.9) | −4.5 (2.0) |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in afternoon - Fifth | 12 | 22.3 (3.9) | 20.3 (3.0) | 3.2 (4.2) |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in morning - Third | 12 | 31.8 (3.1) | 28.2 (1.0) | 4.6 (7.2) |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in morning - Fifth | 12 | 16.9 (1.8) | 23.0 (1.4) | 9.7 (1.7) |
MVPA = moderate to vigorous physical activity
Absolute difference between interview-MVPA and accelerometer-MVPA = absolute difference between minutes of MVPA reported during the interview compared to minutes of MVPA from the accelerometer. Values close to zero indicate better accuracy, but not whether under- or over-reporting occurred.
Arithmetic difference between interview-MVPA and accelerometer-MVPA = arithmetic difference between minutes of MVPA reported during the interview minus minutes of MVPA from the accelerometer. Values close to zero indicate better accuracy; negative and positive values indicate average under- and over-reporting, respectively. Under-reporting and over-reporting can offset each other, so a small average for a group may disguise large reporting errors balanced over the two directions.
One third-grade girl’s interview with diet-&-physical-activity content and same-day recall in the afternoon retention interval had to be dropped during analyses when it was discovered that the accelerometer had recorded less than one hour of data.
Absolute difference was better for fifth-grade (20.4 minutes) than third-grade (26.8 minutes) children.
Arithmetic difference was best for third-grade children for diet-&-physical-activity–same-day-recalls-in-the-afternoon, diet-&-physical-activity–previous-day-recalls-in-the-morning, and physical-activity-only–same-day-recalls-in-the-afternoon (−4.5 to 5.9 minutes) and worst for physical-activity-only–previous-day-recalls-in-the-morning (10.2 minutes). For fifth-grade children, arithmetic difference was best with same-day-recalls-in-the-afternoon regardless of content (3.2 and 4.4 minutes) and worst with previous-day-recalls-in-the-morning, for which diet-&-physical-activity content (9.7 minutes) was better than physical-activity-only content (15.4 minutes).
p < .05,
p < .01
Absolute difference
Grade (p=.0233) was significant. Absolute difference was better for fifth-grade (20.4 minutes) than third-grade (26.8 minutes) children.
Arithmetic difference
The content-×-retention-interval-×-grade interaction (p=.0070) was significant. For third-grade children, arithmetic difference was best for diet-&-physical-activity–same-day-recalls-in-the-afternoon, diet-&-physical-activity–previous-day-recalls-in-the-morning, and physical-activity-only–same-day-recalls-in-the-afternoon (−4.5 to 5.9 minutes) and worst for physical-activity-only–previous-day-recalls-in-the-morning (10.2 minutes). For fifth-grade children, arithmetic difference was best with same-day-recalls-in-the-afternoon retention interval regardless of content (3.2 and 4.4 minutes) and worst with previous-day-recalls-in-the-morning retention interval, for which diet-&-physical-activity content (9.7 minutes) was better than physical-activity-only content (15.4 minutes).
Interview length
Table 3 shows descriptives for interview length. Average length for diet-&-physical-activity content (20.5 minutes) was almost twice as long as for diet-only (11.7 minutes) or physical-activity-only (12.4 minutes) content, but similar by retention interval and grade (14 to 15 minutes).
Table 3.
na | Interview length | ||
---|---|---|---|
Content | Diet only | 48 | 11.7 (0.4) |
Physical activity only | 48 | 12.4 (0.2) | |
Diet & physical activity | 47 | 20.5 (0.8) | |
Retention Interval | Same-day recall in the afternoon | 71 | 15.3 (0.3) |
Previous-day recall in the morning | 72 | 14.5 (0.4) | |
Grade | Third | 71 | 15.5 (0.6) |
Fifth | 72 | 14.3 (0.1) | |
Content by Retention Interval | Diet only - Same-day recall in the afternoon | 24 | 11.9 (0.1) |
Physical activity only - Same-day recall in the afternoon | 24 | 13.1 (0.1) | |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in the afternoon | 23 | 21.0 (0.8) | |
Diet only - Previous-day recall in the morning | 24 | 11.6 (0.8) | |
Physical activity only - Previous-day recall in the morning | 24 | 11.8 (0.4) | |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in the morning | 24 | 20.2 (0.7) | |
Content by Grade | Diet only - Third | 24 | 11.9 (0.4) |
Physical activity only - Third | 24 | 12.4 (0.5) | |
Diet & physical activity - Third | 23 | 22.3 (1.2) | |
Diet only - Fifth | 24 | 11.6 (0.5) | |
Physical activity only - Fifth | 24 | 12.5 (0.9) | |
Diet & physical activity - Fifth | 24 | 18.8 (0.4) | |
Retention Interval by Grade | Same-day recall in the afternoon - Third | 35 | 15.4 (0.5) |
Previous-day recall in the morning - Third | 36 | 15.7 (0.9) | |
Same-day recall in the afternoon -Fifth | 36 | 15.2 (0.3) | |
Previous-day recall in the morning -Fifth | 36 | 13.4 (0.1) | |
Content by Retention Interval by Grade | Diet only - Same-day recall in the afternoon - Third | 12 | 10.5 (0.2) |
Diet only - Same-day recall in the afternoon - Fifth | 12 | 13.2 (0.1) | |
Diet only - Previous-day recall in the morning - Third | 12 | 13.3 (0.6) | |
Diet only - Previous-day recall in the morning - Fifth | 12 | 9.9 (1.0) | |
Physical activity only - Same-day recall in the afternoon - Third | 12 | 12.9 (0.8) | |
Physical activityonly - Same-day recall in the afternoon - Fifth | 12 | 13.3 (0.9) | |
Physical activity only - Previous-day recall in the morning - Third | 12 | 11.9 (0.5) | |
Physical activity only - Previous-day recall in the morning - Fifth | 12 | 11.7 (1.3) | |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in the afternoon - Third | 11 | 22.8 (0.8) | |
Diet & physical activity - Same-day recall in the afternoon - Fifth | 12 | 19.2 (0.8) | |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in the morning - Third | 12 | 21.9 (1.6) | |
Diet & physical activity - Previous-day recall in the morning - Fifth | 12 | 18.4 (0.2) |
The final sample consisted of 143 children because one third-grade girl’s interview with diet-&-physical-activity content and same-day recall in the afternoon retention interval had to be dropped during analyses when it was discovered that the accelerometer had recorded less than one hour of data.
DISCUSSION
Content, retention interval, and grade were three factors of interest for this study. There has been speculation that combining children’s dietary and physical activity recall would improve accuracy.35,57 However, the effects of content differed depending on the levels of other factors, as shown by four significant interactions. First, content interacted with retention interval and grade on intrusion rate, although only for the longer retention interval, with better accuracy for younger children with the integrated than single content, but vice versa for older children. Second, content interacted with grade on correspondence rate and inflation ratio with better accuracy for younger children with the integrated than single content, but vice versa for older children. Third, content interacted with retention interval and grade on arithmetic difference with better accuracy for younger children for the integrated content irrespective of retention interval and for the single content with the shorter retention interval. However, older children were most accurate for the shorter retention interval irrespective of content. With the longer retention interval, accuracy was better for the integrated than single content. These collective results imply that content’s effects on children’s dietary and/or physical activity recall accuracy are complex rather than straightforward.
Retention interval was significant for correspondence rate and inflation ratio, with better accuracy for shorter than longer retention intervals. Furthermore, in addition to three-way interactions on intrusion rate and arithmetic difference, retention interval interacted significantly with grade on omission rate, such that accuracy was better for older than younger children, but only with the shorter retention interval. Although there is evidence that physical activity accuracy declines as time passes, most physical activity recalls concern the previous day(s).58 The authors know of no study that has validated children’s accuracy for physical activity recalls about the previous versus same day. Only one past study36 has examined retention interval on dietary recall accuracy with third-grade children.
Grade was significant for absolute difference, with better accuracy for older than younger children, and within three two-way interactions (omission rate, correspondence rate, inflation ratio) and two three-way interactions (intrusion rate, arithmetic difference). Most past cross-sectional studies that examined elementary-school children’s dietary recall accuracy and grade found that accuracy improved with increased age,3,16,49,59 although one study found no age effect.24 Past cross-sectional studies showed that elementary-school children’s physical-activity recall accuracy improved with increased age.60,61 However, for this cross-sectional study, the effects of grade usually differed depending on the levels of other factors, as shown by five significant interactions.
The cognitive developmental literature indicates that third-grade children understand causal relationships and can manipulate thoughts and intentions, but their cognitions are tied to reality (personal experiences)62 and their cognitive ability (viewing process) is still quite linear; this is often referred to as concrete operational thought. Most third-grade children cannot yet consider complex relationships, or proficiently think in abstract terms. By fifth grade, children are more likely to have developed abstract cognitive abilities and to have begun to formulate cognitive beliefs and attributions specific to health behaviors. Some of this study’s grade interactions were counterintuitive (i.e., better accuracy for third- than fifth-grade children). For example, content interacted with grade on correspondence rate and inflation ratio; however, the improvements have little practical utility. Specifically, for third-grade children with integrated content, an average correspondence rate of 50.9% meant they correctly reported only half of the kilocalories observed eaten, and an average inflation ratio of 22.9% meant that one fourth of kilocalories reported eaten were unobserved.
This study’s third- and fifth-grade children quite accurately reported amounts eaten for matches in qualitative terms. On average, reported amounts for matches differed from observed amounts by only 0.04 servings. However, on average, children had eaten 0.77 serving of items omitted, and reported having eaten 0.73 serving of intrusions. These results are similar to past studies’ results with fourth-grade children.15,18,20,25
Pairwise correlations between dietary accuracy measures were calculated to determine whether children who committed one type of error also committed another, especially between the single and integrated contents. There was a strong positive association between intrusion rate and inflation ratio (diet-only r=0.80; diet-&-physical-activity r=0.59), and a strong negative association between omission rate and correspondence ratio (diet-only r= −0.88; diet-&-physical-activity r= −0.92). Other pairwise correlations were moderately positive (omission rate/intrusion rate), moderately negative (intrusion rate/correspondence ratio), or weak. Based on a series of z-tests with Bonferroni adjustments, the corresponding pairwise correlations were not significantly different (each adjusted p>0.30) between the single and integrated contents.
Considering that all four dietary accuracy measures are important, what constitutes acceptable dietary recall accuracy? Similar to past studies,18-20 arbitrary criteria was applied which established omission rates, intrusion rates, and inflation ratios of ≤30% as passing and >30% as failing, and correspondence rates of ≥70% as passing and <70% as failing. With these criteria, a child’s recall had “passing” accuracy even if s/he omitted up to about one third of items and kilocalories eaten, and if up to about one third of items and kilocalories s/he reported were intruded. Of the 95 children analyzed with diet-only or diet-&-physical-activity content, dietary recall accuracy overall was mediocre as 23, 9, 23, 20, and 20 children passed zero, one, two, three, and four measures, respectively. As Table 1’s right-hand columns show, the estimated marginal probability of passing all four dietary measures was higher for the single than integrated content, for the shorter than longer retention interval, and for the fifth than third grade. Joint probabilities of passing all four measures were highest for the single content–shorter retention interval, single content–fifth grade, and shorter retention interval–fifth grade combinations, as well as for the single content–shorter retention interval–fifth grade combination.
Limitations
Budgetary constraints precluded observations of physical activity; thus, children’s accuracy for reporting specific types of physical activity (e.g., soccer) could not be investigated. Data collection cells could not be stratified by ethnicity. A 2011 paper63 found that cognitive development influenced fourth-grade children’s measures of dietary reporting error. This study’s objective was to investigate the effect of content (single versus integrated) on reporting accuracy, and the effects of retention interval and grade. Randomization of children to content and retention interval groups minimized any differences in cognitive ability among groups that would benefit any group in terms of accuracy. Although investigating the joint effect of cognitive ability and content was not this study’s objective, it is an area for future research.
Strengths
Dietary recalls and physical activity recalls were validated using direct meal observations and accelerometry, respectively. This avoided problems inherent with relative validity (such as back-to-back recalls by children or other methods that rely on memory [e.g., paper questionnaires]) and recalls by parents (who are not present to witness children’s intake or physical activity at school). Rigorous quality control procedures were consistently implemented for observations and interviews.
CONCLUSIONS
This study’s findings do not support a recommendation to integrate dietary intake and physical activity in elementary-school children’s recalls. However, this study’s findings clearly indicate the use of shorter rather than longer retention intervals to yield more accurate dietary recalls from children. Further validation studies are needed to clarify grade (i.e., age) effects and to identify evidence-based practices to improve children’s accuracy for recalling dietary intake and/or physical activity.
Acknowledgments
Funding/Support Disclosure
This research was supported by competitive grant R21HL093406 (with SD Baxter and RR Pate as Co-Principal Investigators) from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute of the National Institutes of Health. The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute or the National Institutes of Health.
Gratitude is expressed to Richland School District Two in South Carolina and staff and children at the four elementary schools for allowing data collection.
Dr. Baxter’s current and past research has been funded externally by competitive grants from the National Institutes of Health and the United States Department of Agriculture. She has served as a grant reviewer for the National Institutes of Health and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. She is on the Board of Editors for the Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics.
Dr. Hitchcock’s research has been funded by the South Carolina Department of Education and the University of South Carolina Research and Productivity Program.
Dr. Pate’s current and past research has been funded externally by competitive grants from the National Institutes of Health and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. He has served as a grant reviewer for the National Institutes of Health and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. He is on the Board of Editors for the Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, Childhood Obesity, and Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews.
Dr. Wilson’s current and past research has been funded externally by competitive grants from the National Institutes of Health and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. She has served as a grant reviewer for the National Institutes of Health. She is on the Board of Editors for Health Psychology and Ethnicity and Disease.
Footnotes
The nine race/ethnicity categories and number of children were (1) African American = 94, (2) African American/American Indian or Alaska Native = 1, (3) African American/Hispanic = 1, (4) African American/White = 5, (5) Hispanic = 17, (6) White = 19, (7) White/African American/Asian = 1, (8) White/Hispanic = 4, and (9) White/Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander = 1. The four race/ethnicity categories used for analyses were (1) African American, (2) White, (3) Hispanic, and (4) Other.
Conflict of interest disclosure
Mrs. Guinn, Mrs. Vaadi, Mrs. Puryear, Mrs. Royer, Dr. McIver, and Dr. Dowda have no competing interest to disclose.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Suzanne D. Baxter, Email: sbaxter@mailbox.sc.edu.
David B. Hitchcock, Email: hitchcock@stat.sc.edu.
Caroline H. Guinn, Email: cguinn@mailbox.sc.edu.
Kate K. Vaadi, Email: vaadi@mailbox.sc.edu.
Megan P. Puryear, Email: pricev@mailbox.sc.edu.
Julie A. Royer, Email: royerj@mailbox.sc.edu.
Kerry L. McIver, Email: mciverkl@mailbox.sc.edu.
Marsha Dowda, Email: mdowda@mailbox.sc.edu.
Russell R. Pate, Email: rpate@mailbox.sc.edu.
Dawn K. Wilson, Email: wilsondk@mailbox.sc.edu.
References
- 1.Baranowski T, Dworkin RJ, Cieslik CJ, et al. Reliability and validity of self report of aerobic activity: Family Health Project. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1984;55(4):309–317. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Byers T, Treiber F, Gunter E, et al. The accuracy of parental reports of their children’s intake of fruits and vegetables: Validation of a food frequency questionnaire with serum levels of carotenoids and vitamins C, A, and E. Epidemiology. 1993;4(4):350–355. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Emmons L, Hayes M. Accuracy of 24-hr. recalls of young children. J Am Diet Assoc. 1973;62(4):409–415. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Guinn CH, Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Frye FHA, Kopec CT. Which fourth-grade children participate in school breakfast and do their parents know it? J Nutr Educ Behav. 2002;34:159–165. doi: 10.1016/s1499-4046(06)60085-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Luepker RV, Perry CL, McKinlay SM, et al. Outcomes of a field trial to improve children’s dietary patterns and physical activity: The Child and Adolescent Trial for Cardiovascular Health (CATCH) JAMA. 1996;275(10):768–776. doi: 10.1001/jama.1996.03530340032026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Lytle LA, Dixon LB, Cunningham-Sabo L, et al. Dietary intakes of Native American children: Findings from the Pathways Feasibility Study. J Am Diet Assoc. 2002;102(4):555–558. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8223(02)90129-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor G, et al. Changing fruit and vegetable consumption among children: The 5-A-Day Power Plus program in St. Paul, Minnesota. Am J Public Health. 1998;88(4):603–609. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.4.603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Receveur O, Morou K, Gray-Donald K, Macaulay AC. Consumption of key food items is associated with excess weight among elementary-school-aged children in a Canadian First Nations Community. J Am Diet Assoc. 2008;108:362–366. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rockett HR, Breitenbach M, Frazier AL, et al. Validation of a youth/adolescent food frequency questionnaire. Prev Med. 1997;26(6):808–816. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1997.0200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Thiagarajah K, Fly AD, Hoelscher DM, et al. Validating the food behavior questions from the elementary school SPAN questionnaire. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2008;40:305–310. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2007.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.US Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service, Office of Research, Nutrition, and Analysis. [November 11, 2013];School Nutrition Dietary Assessment Study-III: Volume II: Student Participation and Dietary Intakes, Executive Summary, Report No CN-07- SNDA-III. 2007 http://www.fns.usda.gov/school-nutrition-dietary-assessment-study-iii.
- 12.Wong SS, Boushey CJ, Novotny R, Gustafson DR. Evaluation of a computerized food frequency questionnaire to estimate calcium intake of Asian, Hispanic, and Non-Hispanic white youth. J Am Diet Assoc. 2008;108:539–543. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sirard J, Pate RR. Physical activity assessment in children and adolescents. Sports Med. 2001;31:439–454. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200131060-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Baranowski T, Islam N, Baranowski J, et al. The Food Intake Recording Software System is valid among fourth-grade children. J Am Diet Assoc. 2002;102:380–385. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8223(02)90088-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Baxter SD, Smith AF, Litaker MS, et al. Recency affects reporting accuracy of children’s dietary recalls. Ann Epidemiol. 2004;14:385–390. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2003.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Davis HC. Prompting methods affect the accuracy of children’s school lunch recalls. J Am Diet Assoc. 2000;100:911–918. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(00)00264-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Davis HC, Johnson MH. Impact of gender, ethnicity, meal component, and time interval between eating and reporting on accuracy of fourth-graders’ self-reports of school lunch. J Am Diet Assoc. 1997;97:1293–1298. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(97)00309-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Litaker MS, Frye FHA, Guinn CH. Low accuracy and low consistency of fourth-graders’ school breakfast and school lunch recalls. J Am Diet Assoc. 2002;102(3):386–395. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8223(02)90089-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Litaker MS, et al. Accuracy of fourth-graders’ dietary recalls of school breakfast and school lunch validated with observations: In-person versus telephone interviews. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2003;35(3):124–134. doi: 10.1016/s1499-4046(06)60196-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Smith AF, et al. Reverse versus forward order reporting and the accuracy of fourth-graders’ recalls of school breakfast and school lunch. Prev Med. 2003;36(5):601–614. doi: 10.1016/s0091-7435(02)00030-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Crawford PB, Obarzanek E, Morrison J, Sabry ZI. Comparative advantage of 3-day food records over 24-hour recall and 5-day food frequency validated by observation of 9- and 10-year-old girls. J Am Diet Assoc. 1994;94:626–630. doi: 10.1016/0002-8223(94)90158-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Domel SB, Thompson WO, Baranowski T, Smith AF. How children remember what they have eaten. J Am Diet Assoc. 1994;94(11):1267–1272. doi: 10.1016/0002-8223(94)92458-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Meredith A, Matthews A, Zickefoose M, Weagley E, Wayave M, Brown EG. How well do school children recall what they have eaten? J Am Diet Assoc. 1951;27:749–751. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Todd KS, Kretsch MJ. Accuracy of the self-reported dietary recall of new immigrant and refugee children. Nutr Res. 1986;6:1031–1043. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Baxter SD, Hardin JW, Guinn CH, Royer JA, Mackelprang AJ, Smith AF. Fourth-grade children’s dietary recall accuracy is influenced by retention interval (target period and interview time) J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:846–856. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2009.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Moore GF, Tapper K, Murphey S, Clark R, Lynch R, Moore L. Validation of a self-completion measure of breakfast foods, snacks and fruits and vegetables consumed by 9-to 11-year-old schoolchildren. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2007;61:420–430. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Freedson PS, Pober D, Janz KF. Calibration of accelerometer output for children. Medicine & Science for Sports & Exercise. 2005;37(11 Suppl):S523–S530. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000185658.28284.ba. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kohl HW, Fulton JE, Caspersen CJ. Assessment of physical activity among children and adolescents: A review and synthesis. Prev Med. 2000;31:S54–S76. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sallis JF, Saelens BE. Assessment of physcial activity by self-report: Status, limitations, and future directions. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2000;71(2):1–14. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2000.11082780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Welk GJ, Wickel E, Peterson M, Heitzler CD, Fulton JE, Potter LD. Reliability and validity of questions on the Youth Media Campaign Longitudinal Survey. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(4):612–621. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e3180305c59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ogden CL, Carrol MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity and trends in body mass index among US children and adolescents, 1999-2010. JAMA. 2012;307(5):483–490. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.American Dietetic Association. Position of the American Dietetic Association: Local support for nutrition integrity in schools. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110:1244–1254. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.French SA, Stables G. Environmental interventions to promote vegetable and fruit consumption among youth in school settings. Prev Med. 2003;37:593–610. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.US Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service. [November 11, 2013];Local School Wellness Policy. Food and Nutrition Service web site at http://www.fns.usda.gov/tn/Healthy/wellnesspolicy.html.
- 35.Moore HJ, Ells LJ, McLure SA, et al. The development and evaluation of a novel computer program to assess previous-day dietary and physical activity behaviours in school children: The Synchronised Nutrition and Activity Program™ (SNAP™) Br J Nutr. 2008;99:1266–1274. doi: 10.1017/S0007114507862428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Baxter SD, Hitchcock DB, Guinn CH, et al. A pilot study of the effects of interview content, retention interval, and grade on accuracy of dietary information from children. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2013;45(4):368–373. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2013.01.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Baglio ML, Baxter SD, Guinn CH, Thompson WO, Shaffer NM, Frye FHA. Assessment of interobserver reliability in nutrition studies that use direct observation of school meals. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104:1385–1393. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2004.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Davis HC. Trading of food during school lunch by first- and fourth-grade children. Nutr Res. 2001;21:499–503. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Domel SB, Baranowski T, Leonard SB, Davis H, Riley P, Baranowski J. Accuracy of fourth- and fifth-grade students’ food records compared with school-lunch observations. Am J Clin Nutr. 1994;59(Suppl):218S–220S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/59.1.218S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Reger C, O’ Neil CE, Nicklas TA, Myers L, Berenson GS. Plate waste of school lunches served to children in a low-socioeconomic elementary school in south Louisiana. Sch Food Service Res Rev. 1996;20(Suppl):13–19. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Treuth MS, Sherwood NE, Butte NF, et al. Validity and reliability of activity measures in African-American girls for GEMS. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003;35:532–539. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000053702.03884.3F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Masse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M. Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(1):181–188. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31815a51b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Trost SG, Pate RR, Sallis JF, et al. Age and gender differences in objectively measured physical activity in youth. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2002;34:350–355. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200202000-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wilson DK, Baxter SD, Guinn CH, Pate RR, McIver KL. A qualitative study of interviewer-administered physical activity recalls by children. J Phys Act Health. 2013;10(6):833–849. doi: 10.1123/jpah.10.6.833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Shaffer NM, Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Baglio ML, Guinn CH, Frye FHA. Quality control for interviews to obtain dietary recalls from children for research studies. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104:1577–1585. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2004.07.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Paxton A, Baxter SD, Fleming P, Ammerman A. Validation of the School Lunch Recall questionnaire to capture school lunch intake of third- to fifth-grade students. J Am Diet Assoc. 2011;111(3):419–424. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Smith AF. Cognitive Processes in Long-term Dietary Recall. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 1991. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 6, No. 4. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Smith AF, Jobe JB, Mingay DJ. Retrieval from memory of dietary information. Appl Cognit Psychol. 1991;5:269–296. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Warren JM, Henry CJK, Livingstone MBE, Lightowler HJ, Bradshaw SM, Perwaiz S. How well do children aged 5-7 years recall food eaten at school lunch? Public Health Nutr. 2003;6:41–47. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Baxter SD, Guinn CH, Royer JA, Hardin JW, Smith AF. Shortening the retention interval of 24-hour dietary recalls increases fourth-grade children’s accuracy for reporting energy and macronutrient intake at school meals. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110(8):1178–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Baxter SD, Royer JA, Hardin JW, Guinn CH, Smith AF. Fourth-grade children are less accurate in reporting school breakfast than school lunch during 24-hour dietary recalls. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2007;39:126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2006.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Baxter SD, Smith AF, Hardin JW, Nichols MN. Conclusions about children’s reporting accuracy for energy and macronutrients over multiple interviews depend on the analytic approach for comparing reported information to reference information. J Am Diet Assoc. 2007;107:595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Baxter SD, Smith AF, Hardin JW, Nichols MN. Conventional energy and macronutrient variables distort the accuracy of children’s dietary reports: Illustrative data from a validation study of effect of order prompts. Prev Med. 2007;44:34–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2006.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Baxter SD, Smith AF, Litaker MS, et al. Body mass index, sex, interview protocol, and children’s accuracy for reporting kilocalories observed eaten at school meals. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006;106:1656–1662. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2006.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Smith AF, Baxter SD, Hardin JW, Nichols MN. Conventional analyses of data from dietary validation studies may misestimate reporting accuracy: Illustration from a study of the effect of interview modality on children’s reporting accuracy. Public Health Nutr. 2007;10:1247–1256. doi: 10.1017/S136898000768714X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B (Methodological) 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Frank GC, Berenson GS, Schilling PE, Moore MC. Adapting the 24-hr. recall for epidemiologic studies of school children. J Am Diet Assoc. 1977;71(1):26–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sallis JF, Buono MJ, Roby JJ, Micale FG, Nelson JA. Seven-day recall and other physical activity self-reports in children and adolescents. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993;25(1):99–108. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199301000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Reynolds LA, Johnson SB, Silverstein J. Assessing daily diabetes management by 24-hour recall interview: The validity of children’s reports. J Pediatr Psychol. 1990;15(4):493–509. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/15.4.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Simons-Morton BG, Taylor WC, Wei Huang I. Validity of the physical activity interview and Caltrac with preadolescent children. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1994;65(1):84–88. doi: 10.1080/02701367.1994.10762212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Sallis JF, Buono MJ, Roby J, Carlson D, Nelson JA. The Caltrac accelerometer as a physical activity monitor for school-age children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1990;22(5):698–703. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199010000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Flavell JH. Cognitive development: children’s knowledge about mind. Annu Rev Psychol. 1999;50:21–45. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Smith AF, Baxter SD, Hardin JW, Guinn CH, Royer JA. Relation of children’s dietary reporting accuracy to cognitive ability. Am J Epidemiol. 2011;173:103–109. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]