Skip to main content
Pediatrics logoLink to Pediatrics
. 2014 Nov;134(5):e1287–e1292. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-1974

Longitudinal Association Between Teen Sexting and Sexual Behavior

Jeff R Temple 1,, HyeJeong Choi 1
PMCID: PMC4210802  PMID: 25287459

Abstract

BACKGROUND:

This study examines the temporal sequencing of sexting and sexual intercourse and the role of active sexting (sending a nude picture) in mediating the relationship between passive sexting (asking or being asked for a nude picture) and sexual behaviors.

METHODS:

Data are from Wave 2 (spring 2011) and Wave 3 (spring 2012) of an ongoing 6-year longitudinal study of high school students in southeast Texas. Participants included 964 ethnically diverse adolescents with a mean age of 16.09 years (56% female; 31% African American, 29% Caucasian, 28% Hispanic, 12% other). Retention rate for 1-year follow-up was 93%. Participants self-reported history of sexual activity (intercourse, risky sex) and sexting (sent, asked, been asked). Using path analysis, we examined whether teen sexting at baseline predicted sexual behavior at 1-year follow-up and whether active sexting mediated the relationship between passive sexting and sexual behavior.

RESULTS:

The odds of being sexually active at Wave 3 were 1.32 times larger for youth who sent a sext at Wave 2, relative to counterparts. However, sexting was not temporally associated with risky sexual behaviors. Consistent with our hypothesis, active sexting at Wave 2 mediated the relationship between asking or being asked for a sext and having sex over the next year.

CONCLUSIONS:

This study extends cross-sectional literature and supports the notion that sexting fits within the context of adolescent sexual development and may be a viable indicator of adolescent sexual activity.

Keywords: teen sexting, adolescents, sexual behavior


What’s Known on This Subject:

Cross-sectional research indicates that teen sexting is common, may be associated with other adolescent behaviors such as substance use, does not appear to be a marker of mental well being, and is probably an indicator of actual sexual behaviors.

What This Study Adds:

Although mounting evidence links teen sexting to sexual behavior, little is known about the temporal sequencing of these 2 behaviors. Knowing which comes first will aid tween- and teen-focused health care providers in their interaction with patients and patients’ parents.

Though still limited, research on teen sexting (defined herein as electronically sending sexually explicit images from 1 adolescent to another) has grown substantially in the past 3 years. Studies composed of middle school and high school students, ethnically diverse youth, community and at-risk adolescents, and regional and national samples have begun answering important questions about this emerging public health issue.1 Specifically, we now know that teen sexting is common, occurring among 15% and 28% of adolescents,25 with a much higher proportion of college students and young adults engaging in this behavior68; is associated with impulsivity in general and substance use in particular6,9,10; does not appear to be a marker of mental well-being7,10; and, perhaps most importantly, is probably an indicator of actual sexual behaviors and possibly risky sexual behaviors.1,57,1113

Despite these advancements in knowledge, all existing research is cross-sectional, thus limiting our ability to determine the temporal relationship between sexting and proposed correlates. For example, although sexting has consistently been linked to sexual behavior, it is unclear whether sexting precedes or follows engagement in sexual activity. An argument can be made for both. Indeed, it is possible that sexting operates as a way of introducing sex into the relationship (sexting → sexual behavior), or it may be that having sexual relations increases the level of comfort in sharing nude images (sexual behavior → sexting). The question of what comes first is not merely academic. If sexting precedes sexual behavior (especially risky sexual behavior), then safe sex interventions could be designed to specifically target sexting youth, and prevention programs could aim to reduce sexting as a means of reducing risky sex.

Another gap in existing knowledge is whether passive sexting (receiving, asking for, or being asked for a nude picture) differs from active sexting (sending a nude picture) as an indicator of sexual behavior. Although limited existing studies indicate that active and passive sexting are similarly associated with sexual behavior,5,6 analyses have not accounted for the fact that nearly all adolescents who sent a sext had also asked and been asked for one. Given that adolescents have control over whether they send a sext and that sending a sext demonstrates some comfort with their own sexuality, we argue that active sexting is the important component in the relationship between sexting and sexual behavior. Thus, we posit that sending a sext will mediate the effects of asking or being asked for a sext on sexual behavior such that passive sexting will be positively associated with sending a sext, and sending a sext will be positively associated with having sex the next year.

The current study extends the literature by examining the temporal sequencing of sexting and sexual behaviors and by examining the role of active sexting in mediating the association between passive sexting and sexual behaviors.

Methods

Sample and Study Design

This study was approved by the institutional review board of the University of Texas Medical Branch. Current data are from Waves 2 and 3 of Dating It Safe, an ongoing longitudinal study of teen dating violence and other high-risk adolescent behaviors.14 Participants in Wave 1 (Spring 2010) included 1042 students recruited from 7 public high schools in 5 Houston-area school districts (62% response rate, which is above the 60% suggested by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). The current data were collected in spring 2011 (Wave 2, retention rate: 92.5%) and spring 2012 (Wave 3, retention rate: 85.8%). Sexting items were not assessed at Wave 1. Study recruitment occurred during school hours in courses with mandated attendance, and both parental permission and student assent were obtained. Assessments at each time point occurred during school hours, and students received a $10 gift card for participating. To increase reliability of adolescent self-report, teachers and other school administrators were not allowed to be present during questionnaire administration, and privacy was emphasized, including instructing participants to not write their names on surveys and informing them that a federal certificate of confidentiality protected their responses. Participants no longer at their original school were surveyed at an alternate location. In Wave 2, participants were 56% female, with a mean age of 16.09 years (SD = 0.79), and they self-reported as African American (31%), white (29%), Hispanic (28%), and other (12%). Because students were recruited primarily when they were high school freshmen, a majority of them in the current study were in the 10th grade (73%).

Measures

Where applicable, frequency, mean, and SD for each variable at each wave are shown in Table 1. Three items assessed lifetime sexting (at Wave 2), including, “Have you ever sent naked pictures of yourself to another through text or e-mail?,” “Have you ever asked someone to send naked pictures of themselves to you?,” and “Have you ever been asked to send a naked picture of yourself through text or e-mail?” These items were included in the model as dummy-coded variables (1 = Yes, 0 = No). Because of the novelty of this topic and as in other studies on sexting,12 questions were developed based on a review of relevant literature15,16 and in consultation with adolescent health experts. Because of potential legal and psychosocial issues, this study limited the definition of “sexting” to naked pictures rather than seminude picture or explicit messages.

TABLE 1.

Frequency, Mean, and SD for all variables

Yes (%) No (%)
Ever had sex (Wave 2) 506 (52.7) 455 (47.3)
Ever had sex (Wave 3) 568 (63.7) 324 (36.3)
Sent a sext (Wave 2) 259 (27.6) 678 (72.4)
Asked for a sext (Wave 2) 295 (31.4) 643 (68.6)
Been asked for a sext (Wave 2) 540 (60.0) 408 (40.0)
Condom use (Wave 2) 355 (46.6) 407 (53.4)
Condom use (Wave 3) 298 (59.5) 203 (40.5)
Frequency (%); Mean; SD
# Sexual partners past year (Wave 2) M = 1.9 SD = 1.48
 1 224 (48.6)
 2 110 (23.9)
 3 60 (13)
 4 22 (4.8)
 5 22 (4.8)
 ≥6 23 (4.9)
# Sexual partners past year (Wave 3) M = 2.11 SD = 1.52
 1 185 (45.8)
 2 91 (22.5)
 3 58 (14.3)
 4 31 (7.7)
 5 12 (3.0)
 ≥6 27 (6.7)
Alcohol or drug use before sex (Wave 2) M = 0.54 SD = 0.78
 Never 292 (62.8)
 Rarely 104 (23.4)
 Sometimes 61 (13.1)
 Always 8 (1.7)
Alcohol or drug use before sex (Wave 3) 0.78 0.91
 Never 201 (51.3)
 Rarely 91 (23.3)
 Sometimes 95 (23.2)
 Always 13 (3.2)

Sexual and Risky Sex Behavior (Wave 3)

Participants were asked whether they “have had sex (intercourse).” Those who reported affirmatively (1 = had sex, 0 = never) were asked 3 additional questions: whether they use condoms during sexual intercourse (1 = nonuser, 0 = user), number of sexual partners in the past year (1 = 1 person, 6 = 6 or more people), and frequency of alcohol or drug use before sex (0 = never, 3 = always). Similar measures have been shown to be reliable indicators of adolescent sexual behavior.17,18 Notably, sexual behavior was limited to intercourse and did not include other sexual activity such as oral sex. A distinction was not made between vaginal and anal intercourse or between heterosexual and homosexual intercourse.

Covariates

Gender (0 = female, 1 = male), grade (1 = 9th, 2 = 10th, 3 = 11th, 4 = ≥12th; no substantive differences emerged when analyses were conducted with age, as opposed to grade, as a covariate), ethnicity (3 dummy-coded variables: 1 = Hispanic, 0 = all other ethnicities; 1 = white, 0 = all other ethnicities; 1 = black, 0 = all other ethnicities), sexual behavior (1 = had sex, 0 = never), and dating behavior (1 = begun dating, 0 = never) at Wave 2 were included in the mediator models to control for the relationship between exogenous and endogenous variables.

Statistical Analysis

Path analysis using Mplus 7.019 (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA) was conducted for primary analyses in the current study. To deal with missingness across waves, we used the full information maximum likelihood method.20 Because the mediator (eg, sending a sext at Wave 2) and dependent variable (eg, had sex at Wave 3) were binary variables, we used weighted least squares with mean- and variance-adjusted parameter estimates. This method performs well when data are not normally distributed and provides unbiased parameter estimates.2123 Because the model examined all possible associations, a fully saturated model was used. By definition, a fully saturated model will always offer a perfect fit to the data; therefore, model fit indices are not reported. To examine the potential mediation effect of sending a sext, we used the indirect command with bootstrap option. This method gives a bias-corrected significance test of the mediation effect.24,25 To estimate a significant mediated path, we applied 5000 bootstrap samples and 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (CIs).

Results

Consistent with our hypothesis, sending a sext at Wave 2 mediated the relationship between asking or being asked for a sext and having sex over the next year (Fig 1). Specifically, being asked (odds ratio [OR] = 5.35, bias-corrected CI, 3.39 to 8.44) and asking for a sext (OR = 4.55; 95% CI, 3.37 to 6.15) were positively associated with sending a sext. For youth who asked for a sext, the odds of sending a sext were 9.91 times as large as the odds for youth who have never asked for a sext. Similarly, for youth who had been asked for a sext, the odds of them sending a sext were 5.35 times as large as the odds for their counterparts. Sending a sext at Wave 2 was positively associated with having sex at Wave 3, OR = 1.32; 95% CI, 1.07 to 1.63. Specifically, the odds of being sexually active at Wave 3 were 1.32 times larger for youth who sent a sext at Wave 2 relative to youth who did not send a sext. Conversely, being asked (OR = 0.91; 95% CI, 0.57 to 1.47) and asking for a sext (OR = 0.88; 95% CI, 0.49 to 1.30) were not significantly associated with sexual intercourse at Wave 3.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Temporal relationship between sexting and sexual behaviors. Note. Path coefficients and correlations are completely standardized. Although not shown here, dating and sexual activity at Wave 2, grade, gender, and ethnicity were included as covariates in the model. All significant (p < .05) paths are highlighted by boldface and marked by asterisks. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

To examine the associations between sexting (asking, being asked, sending) and risky sexual behaviors (unprotected sex, number of sexual partners in the past year, and alcohol or drug use before sex) over time, we tested the same mediation model with only adolescents who reported having sexual intercourse at Wave 2 (N = 506). Previous risky sexual behaviors were included in this model as covariates. Five students who had never started dating but reported a history of sex at Wave 2 were excluded from these analyses (notably, analyses were rerun with these 5 adolescents included, and all results were the same). Consistent with the mediation model described earlier, asking (OR = 4.85; 95% CI, 3.30 to 7.09) and being asked for a sext (OR = 4.62; 95% CI, 2.41 to 8.81) were positively associated with sending a sext for these sexually active youth (Fig 2). However, sending a sext at Wave 2 was not associated with risky sexual behaviors at Wave 3, including unprotected sex (OR = 1.01; 95% CI, 0.83 to 1.22), number of sexual partner in the past year (b = 0.17; CI = –0.00 to 0.35), and alcohol or drug use before sex (b = 0.10; 95% CI, –0.02 to 0.22). Thus, although we observed positive associations between sexting variables (asked, been asked, sent), sending a sext was not positively related with risky sexual behaviors over time. In other words, sending a sext did not mediate the relationship between being asked or asking for a sext and risky sexual behaviors. Counter to expectations, asking for a sext was negatively associated with alcohol or drug use before sex, b = –0.30; 95% CI, –0.57 to –0.04. Notably, we tested a reverse model of the one presented, while controlling for sending, asking, and being asked for a sext at Wave 2, and found that sexual behavior or risky sexual behavior at Wave 2 did not significantly predict sexting at Wave 3.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Temporal relationship between sexting and risky sexual behaviors. Note. Path coefficients and correlations are completely standardized. Although not shown here, previous risky sexual behaviors (eg, Wave 2 unprotected sex, number of sexual partners, alcohol or drug use before sex), grade, gender, and ethnicity were included as covariates in the model. All significant (p < .05) paths are highlighted by boldface and marked by asterisks. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Discussion

In this temporal examination of the relationship between teen sexting and sexual behaviors, we found that sending naked pictures of oneself was associated with being sexually active 1 year later; counter to previous cross-sectional research, sexting was not temporally associated with risky sexual behaviors; and active sexting mediated the relationship between passive sexting and sexual intercourse.

Sexting and Sexual Behavior

Our finding that sexting was associated with sexual behavior over time is consistent with mounting cross-sectional evidence linking sexting to a range of sexual behaviors.1,57,1113 For example, Rice and colleagues3 found that adolescents who sexted had >7 times the odds of having ever engaged in sexual intercourse, relative to their nonsexting counterparts. Furthermore, a recent systematic literature review revealed that all extant studies measuring sexting and sexual activity (n = 8) found an association between the 2 behaviors.1 Current data demonstrating a temporal relationship between sexting and sexual behavior extends these findings by offering a first step in addressing the “chicken or egg” question. Notably, although the odds of having sex were significantly higher among adolescents who reported earlier sexting, the increase was not overwhelming, suggesting the importance of additional factors in determining adolescent sexual activity. That sexting may precede sexual intercourse in some cases is consistent with the notion that sexting may serve as a prelude or gateway behavior to actual sexual behaviors, or as a way to indicate one’s readiness to take intimacy to the next level.5,16,26 Indeed, a recent online study found that 38% of college-aged participants reported that exchanging sexts makes “hooking up with others more likely.”26 Furthermore, Drouin and colleagues27 reported that, even across various forms of relationship statuses (committed, casual, cheating), a frequently identified motive for sexting was to initiate sex. Similarly, in a sample of at-risk middle school students, Houack and colleagues12 found that teen sexters had more intentions to engage in sex in the next 6 months than nonsexters.

Sexting and Risky Sexual Behavior

We did not find a relationship between sexting and risky sexual behavior over time. Findings from cross-sectional research on this association are mixed. Although some studies have found a link between sexting and unsafe sex, alcohol or drug use before sex, and history of multiple sexual partners,5,6,11,13 others have revealed marginal3 or no associations.7 Even when a link is identified, the relationship between sexting and risky sex is more nuanced than the link between sexting and sex. For example, a previous study found that sexting was related to a host of risky sexual behaviors, but only among adolescent girls.5 Furthermore, although Ferguson13 did find an association between sexting and having sex without a birth control method, he did not find sexting to be associated with any other risky sexual behaviors.

Passive Sexting Versus Active Sexting

In support of our hypothesis, sending a sext mediated the relationship between asking and being asked for a sext and engagement in sexual intercourse 1 year later. In other words, sending a nude picture (active sexting), as opposed to asking or being asked for a nude picture (passive sexting), was the salient component in the link between sexting and sexual behavior over time. Being a passive recipient of or asking for a sext is not likely to require the same level of comfort with one’s sexuality as does sending a nude photo. Moreover, sending a nude photo may communicate to the recipient a level of openness to sexual activity, promote a belief that sex is expected,2 and increase sexual advances, all of which may increase the chance of subsequent sexual behavior.

Limitations and Future Directions

Several limitations should be considered in interpreting these results. First, questions on sexting were developed for this study, based primarily on limited available literature at the time, and were not vetted by teens, potentially limiting the validity of our findings. A good next step in this line of research may be the development and testing of a sexting-related self-report instrument. Second, because of increased legal and psychosocial concerns associated with sending naked images, we limited our measure of sexting accordingly. However, future research would benefit by including a more comprehensive measure of sexting that contains explicit messages and seminude content. Third, that our model did not find sexting predictive of risky sexual behaviors may be a result of insufficient power and thus should be explored in future research. Fourth, although the current sample represents a diverse cross-section of students from several high schools or districts, it is possible that regional differences influenced results. Fifth, sexual behavior was limited to sexual intercourse and did not include other sexual activity such as oral sex, which may be differentially associated with sexting. Finally, because of the rapidly evolving nature of social media, future research should inquire about new approaches to sexting, including Snapchat, where images disappear after a predetermined number of seconds. It is possible that adolescents are even more likely to sext if they believe the image is temporary. Despite these limitations and ideas for future research, this is the first study to examine the link between sexting and sexual activity over time and among the first to consider the importance of passive versus active sexting.

Conclusions

The link between teen sexting and actual sexual behavior is becoming well established, with this study extending our knowledge by demonstrating a temporal association between the 2 behaviors. Although additional research is needed, current data indicate that sexting may precede sexual intercourse in some instances and cement the notion that sexting behavior is a viable indicator of adolescent sexual activity. That we did not find a link between sexting and risky sexual behavior over time may suggest that sexting is a new “normal” part of adolescent sexual development and not strictly limited to at-risk adolescents. Furthermore, our findings indicate that sending a naked picture can explain the relationship between any form of sexting and actual sexual behavior. That is, asking and being asked for a naked picture are related to sexual activity through their relationship with sending a sext.

Glossary

CI

confidence interval

OR

odds ratio

Footnotes

Dr Temple contributed to all aspects of manuscript preparation, including conceptualizing and designing the study, drafting the initial manuscript, and editing; Dr Choi contributed to the conception and design, analysis, writing, editing, and review of the manuscript; and both authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

FUNDING: Dr Temple is supported by award K23HD059916 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD) and award 2012-WG-BX-0005 from the National Institute of Justice (NIJ). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NICHD or NIJ. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

  • 1.Klettke B, Hallford DJ, Mellor DJ. Sexting prevalence and correlates: a systematic literature review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2014;34(1):44–53 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fleschler Peskin M, Markham CM, Addy RC, Shegog R, Thiel M, Tortolero SR. Prevalence and patterns of sexting among ethnic minority urban high school students. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2013;16(6):454–459 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rice E, Rhoades H, Winetrobe H, et al. Sexually explicit cell phone messaging associated with sexual risk among adolescents. Pediatrics. 2012;130(4):667–673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Strassberg DS, McKinnon RK, Sustaíta MA, Rullo J. Sexting by high school students: an exploratory and descriptive study. Arch Sex Behav. 2013;42(1):15–21 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Temple JR, Paul JA, van den Berg P, Le VD, McElhany A, Temple BW. Teen sexting and its association with sexual behaviors. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(9):828–833 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Benotsch EG, Snipes DJ, Martin AM, Bull SS. Sexting, substance use, and sexual risk behavior in young adults. J Adolesc Health. 2013;52(3):307–313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gordon-Messer D, Bauermeister JA, Grodzinski A, Zimmerman M. Sexting among young adults. J Adolesc Health. 2013;52(3):301–306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Samimi P, Alderson KG. Sexting among undergraduate students. Comput Human Behav. 2014;31:230–241 [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Dir AL, Cyders MA, Coskunpinar A. From the bar to the bed via mobile phone: a first test of the role of problematic alcohol use, sexting, and impulsivity-related traits in sexual hookups. Comput Human Behav. 2013;29(4):1664–1670 [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Temple JR, Le VD, van den Berg P, Ling Y, Paul JA, Temple BW. Brief report: teen sexting and psychosocial health. J Adolesc. 2014;37(1):33–36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dake JA, Price JH, Maziarz L, Ward B. Prevalence and correlates of sexting behavior in adolescents. Am J Sex Educ. 2012;7(1):1–15 [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Houack CD, Barker D, Rizzo C, Hancock E, Norton A, Brown LK. Sexting and sexual behavior in at-risk adolescents. Pediatrics. 2014;133(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/133/2/e276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ferguson CJ. Sexting behaviors among young Hispanic women: incidence and association with other high-risk sexual behaviors. Psychiatr Q. 2011;82(3):239–243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Temple JR, Shorey RC, Fite P, Stuart GL, Le VD. Substance use as a longitudinal predictor of the perpetration of teen dating violence. J Youth Adolesc. 2013;42(4):596–606 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hinduja S, Patchin JW. Sexting: A brief guide for educators and parents. Cyberbullying Research Center. Available at: www.cyberbullying.us/Sexting_Fact_Sheet.pdf. Accessed February 10, 2014
  • 16.Lenhart A. Teens and Sexting Washington, DC: Pew Research Center; 2009. Available at: http://pewinternet.org/Reports/2009/Teens-and-Sexting.aspx. Accessed February 10, 2014 [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Pinkerton SD, Benotsch EG, Mikytuck J. When do simpler sexual behavior data collection techniques suffice? An analysis of consequent uncertainty in HIV acquisition risk estimates. Eval Rev. 2007;31(4):401–412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zimmerman RS, Morisky DE, Harrison L, Mark HD. Validity of behavioral measures as proxies for HIV-related outcomes. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2014;66(suppl 3):S285–S292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen; 1998–2010 [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Graham JW, Cumsille PE, Elek-Fisk E. Methods for handling missing data. In Shinka JA, Velicer WF, eds. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology: Vol 2. Research Methods in Psychology. New York, NY: Wiley; 2003:87–114 [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Flora DB, Curran PJ. An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychol Methods. 2004;9(4):466–491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lei PW. Evaluating estimation methods for ordinal data in structural equation modeling. Qual Quant. 2009;43(3):495–507 [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Yu CY. Evaluating Cutoff Criteria of Model Fit Indices for Latent Variable Models With Binary and Continuous Outcomes [doctoral dissertation]. Los Angeles, CA: University of California; 2002 [Google Scholar]
  • 24.MacKinnon DP. Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2008 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mackinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J. Confidence limits for the indirect effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behav Res. 2004;39(1):99–128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.National Campaign to Prevent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy. Sex and Tech: Results of a Survey of Teens and Young Adults Washington, DC: National Campaign to Prevent Teen and Unplanned Pregnancy; 2008. Available at: www.thenationalcampaign.org/SEXTECH/PDF/SexTech_Summary.pdf. Accessed February 10, 2014
  • 27.Drouin M, Vogel KN, Surbey A, Stills JR. Let’s talk about sexting, baby: computer-mediated sexual behaviors among young adults. Comput Human Behav. 2013;29(5):25–30 [Google Scholar]

Articles from Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Pediatrics

RESOURCES