Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Sci. 2014 Jun 9;17(6):1029–1041. doi: 10.1111/desc.12191

Kin Rejection: Social Signals, Neural Response and Perceived Distress During Social Exclusion

Anirudh Sreekrishnan 1, Tania A Herrera 1, Jia Wu 1, Jessica L Borelli 2, Lars O White 3, Helena J V Rutherford 1, Linda C Mayes 1, Michael J Crowley 1
PMCID: PMC4211999  NIHMSID: NIHMS597746  PMID: 24909389

Abstract

Across species, kin bond together to promote survival. We sought to understand the dyadic effect of exclusion by kin (as opposed to non-kin strangers) on brain activity of the mother and her child and their subjective distress. To this end, we probed mother-child relationships with a computerized ball-toss game Cyberball. When excluded by one another, rather than by a stranger, both mothers and children exhibited a significantly pronounced frontal P2. Moreover, upon kin-rejection versus stranger-rejection, both mothers and children showed incremented left frontal positive slow waves for rejection events. Children reported more distress upon exclusion than their own mothers. Similar to past work, relatively augmented negative frontal slow wave activity predicted greater self-reported ostracism distress. This effect, generalized to the P2, was limited to mother or child- rejection by kin, with comparable magnitude of effect across kin identity (mothers vs. children). For both mothers and children, the frontal P2 peak was significantly pronounced for kin-rejection versus stranger rejection. Taken together, our results document the rapid categorization of social signals as kin-relevant and the specificity of early and late neural markers for predicting felt ostracism.


The relationship between mother and child is a fundamental and defining human connection, profoundly impacting the biology and identity of both individuals (Bowlby, 1958; Francis, Szegda, Campbell, Martin, & Insel, 2003; Harlow & Suomi, 1971; Harlow & Zimmermann, 1958). For mothers, investment in the next generation is of obvious evolutionary value. For the child, the relationship with his/her mother is vital to survival and optimal growth—its disruption may jeopardize cognitive, emotional, and social development (Harlow & Zimmermann, 1958; Klann-Delius & Hofmeister, 1997; Main & Goldwyn, 1984). Children experiencing rejection by their parental figures often display impaired communicative, emotive, and cognitive skills (Benoit & Parker, 1994; Klann-Delius & Hofmeister, 1997; Malatesta, Culver, Tesman, & Shepard, 1989; Moss & St-Laurent, 2001; van, 1995; Ward & Carlson, 1995). Additionally, these children are more likely to become parents that exhibit the same behaviors with their offspring (Main & Goldwyn, 1984; Sullivan, Perry, Sloan, Kleinhaus, & Burtchen, 2011). Given the significant deleterious consequences of disruption of the mother-child relationship, there is a strong incentive for both mother and child to maintain the bond.

Broadly conceived, attachment theory provides a frame from which to view the putative evolutionary and psychological basis of the mother-child relationship (Bowlby, 1988). From this view, infants are pre-programmed to form attachments with others to promote survival (Bowlby, 1958). The ethological root of separation distress likely emerges from the survival benefit of close proximity to a caregiver (Bowlby, 1988). Research has leveraged the attachment concept to probe separation distress and understand how it relates to mother-child attachment (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969; Bowlby, Ainsworth, Boston, & Rosenbluth, 1956; Eisenberger, 2012; Flacking et al., 2012; Schaffer & Emerson, 1964; White et al., 2012). A large body of work documents how individual differences in young children’s responses to separation and reunion with caregivers concurrently and prospectively tracks parent-child relationship quality and a broad range of psychosocial outcomes (See Thompson (2008), for a review).

Social Ostracism as a Probe of Attachment Processes

Social ostracism refers to the act of ignoring and excluding a person from the group (Williams, 2007). Consistent with the idea that attachment patterns generalize to new encounters, ostracism experiences with putative unfamiliar peers evoke neural responses that are associated with the quality of attachment (DeWall et al., 2012; Riem, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Huffmeijer, & van Ijzendoorn, 2013; White et al., 2012; White, Wu, Borelli, Mayes, & Crowley, 2013). In children, this type of exclusion is associated with poorer academic performance, dysregulated emotion, and loss of physical control (Kim, Koh, & Leventhal, 2005; Nesdale & Flesser, 2001; Schwartz, Gorman, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 2008). A number of investigators examining social exclusion draw connections between social pain and physical pain, acknowledging overlap at the conceptual or linguistic level (as revealed by common metaphorical descriptions – “broken hearts,” “hurt feelings”), though accounts vary in terms of the extent to which the neuroanatomical substrates underlying these two experiences are distinct (Cacioppo et al., 2013; Eisenberger, 2006; Eisenberger, Gable, & Lieberman, 2007; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003; Macdonald & Leary, 2005). Social pain is thought to have its roots in the survival value it confers to the individual as a dependent offspring and later as a member of a social group.

Much of the research examining social exclusion uses a standardized computerized task called Cyberball to probe the neural and behavioral effects of ostracism distress (Coyne, Gundersen, Nelson, & Stockdale, 2011; Crowley, Wu, Molfese, & Mayes, 2010; Salvy et al., 2011; Williams & Jarvis, 2006). In this task participants ostensibly connect with two or three players via the Internet to pass a virtual ball back and forth. Unbeknownst to the participant, their co-players are computer-based and move from fair play, where the ball is equally tossed among all players, to an exclusion phase, where the participant is left out of the game (i.e., the other two “players” pass the ball exclusively to one another). Studies employing fMRI during Cyberball reveal that the experiences of social exclusion engage neural circuitry relevant for the experience of distress and self-regulation (i.e., ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC), medial PFC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, posterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)) during exclusion (Bolling et al., 2011; Eisenberger et al., 2003; Masten et al., 2009; Sebastian et al., 2011).

Cyberball has also been used to probe real time brain activity during social ostracism using event-related potentials (ERPs)(Crowley et al., 2009). To date, much of the work with ERPs and Cyberball focuses on frontal slow wave responses for rejection events. In particular, greater negative left frontal/central slow waves are associated with greater experienced distress (Crowley et al., 2009; Crowley et al., 2010; White et al., 2012). Importantly, a number of studies also implicate pronounced left-lateralized frontal slow waves in processing positive or appetitive stimuli and decreased anticipation of negative outcome (Cunningham, Espinet, DeYoung, & Zelazo, 2005; Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2010; Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2013; Graham & Cabeza, 2001; van de Laar, Licht, Franken, & Hendriks, 2004). Thus, the relatively more negative ERP slow waves for rejection events (during exclusion), which tend to be accompanied by less positive appraisal, may also reflect reduced approach motivation.

ERP studies tend to focus on one, and sometimes two brain potentials; for an interpersonal game, like Cyberball, these may include the N2 (expectancy violation) and the P300 (event saliency). Importantly, most if not all ERP work with Cyberball has involved play with unfamiliar peers. Adapting the Cyberball task to include familiar others (i.e., mother and child) could engage other attention-related components given the high saliency of the relationship and its ongoing reciprocal nature. One likely candidate is the frontal P2 response, which occurs early in the ERP waveform and has long been considered an incidence detector, thought to reflect selective attention and sensory processing of target arrays with simple (popout) stimulus features (Key, Dove, & Maguire, 2005; Luck & Hillyard, 1994; Mueller, Brehmer, von Oertzen, Li, & Lindenberger, 2008; Smith, Johnstone, & Barry, 2004). Thus, we might expect rejection by kin (mother or child) to preferentially engage the P2 as compared to rejection by an unfamiliar other.

While many published studies employ Cyberball to study exclusion by strangers (Bolling et al., 2011; Crowley et al., 2010; Eisenberger et al., 2003; Jamieson, Harkins, & Williams, 2010; Masten et al., 2009; C. L. Sebastian et al., 2011; C. Sebastian, Viding, Williams, & Blakemore, 2010; van Beest & Williams, 2006; White et al., 2012; White et al., 2013), to date the paradigm has not been used to probe processes within the mother-child relationship. Presuming the aforementioned evolutionary and interpersonal value of kin, we would expect responses for kin rejection to differ markedly from unfamiliar others.

While many have studied the behavioral impact of rejection on children in one form or another (e.g., Isabella, 1993), the effects of exclusion on mothers by their kin remains unknown. Available data collected through surveys of mothers points to associations between the effects of exclusion by child on maternal mood and the appearance of depressive symptoms (Eastwood et al., 2013). The paucity of available work here stands in contrast to the many clinical cases of a child who has either rejected one’s parents or refused to participate in reunion post-separation (Fidler & Bala, 2010; Friedlander & Walters, 2010).

Bowlby, along with the researchers who followed him, differentiated the “attachment system,” which describes the child’s bond with his/her primary caregivers, from the “caregiving system,” which describes caregivers’ affectionate bonds to their children (Bowlby, 1958; George & Solomon, 1996, 2008). Although there are important parallels between the attachment and caregiving system, the two systems are thought to serve different functions (George & Solomon, 2008). For instance, one of the central goals of the child within the parent-child attachment relationship relate to getting his/her physical and emotional needs met, whereas for the parent a central goal is providing for the child (George & Solomon, 2008). Put simply, the driving force in the parent-child relationship is to ensure that the parent meets the needs of the child. Thus, rejection within the dyad ought to affect mothers and children differently following from this essential difference in their relationship roles. In particular, rejection from the parent to the child should be more aversive than rejection from the child to the parent in that this behavior violates the child’s central need in the relationship. In contrast, healthy parents should be able to tolerate rejection by the child with less distress than the child experiences upon receiving rejection by the parent. However, no work has examined this issue. Bringing together mothers’ and children’s responses to exclusion helps piece together the potentially unique psychological and neuronal effects of this primary social relationship.

Goals of the Current Study

Even in a relatively benign situation such as Cyberball, where a virtual ball is either thrown among all players (fair play) or not (exclusion), exclusion by kin may threaten strong biological and psychological bonds built since infancy for both mother and child. For this reason, we adapted the Cyberball paradigm to probe mother-child interaction in middle childhood. In our version of the game, mothers and children were both led to believe they were interacting with one another along with a stranger (Figure 1). Whereas in previous work, Cyberball involved a participant and two putative strangers, whose behavior and neural responses could be considered comparable, we distinguished between one player who was kin (mother or child) and a stranger.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Design of analysis parameters for mother-child Cyberball. (A) A schematic design of the Cyberball set-up for both child (left) and mother (right) participants. Children “played” the game against two computerized players, one of whom believed was their own mother. Mothers played in the same set-up, except they believed one of the players was their child. The game alternated between a condition of fair play, where the ball could be passed between all players (as indicated by all the arrows), and a condition of exclusion, where the ball was passed between the computerized players (as indicated by arrows marked *). (B) Frontal left electrodes, white, were chosen in examination of fair play and rejection-based ERPs.

Recognizing the nested nature of our design, with mother-child dyad as one unit of analysis, we approached our data from a hierarchical linear model framework. We expected that dyad (mother and child participants), would account for variability in our model. We have previously shown that neural frontal slow wave responses during exclusion from play by unfamiliar peers predict feelings of ostracism distress (Crowley et al., 2009; Crowley et al., 2010; White et al., 2012). Accordingly, we expected self-reported feelings of ostracism and ERP rejection-event slow waves to be associated in mothers and children. Secondly, we expected to see differences in the P2 peak; a greater amplitude P2 peak for kin rejection compared to stranger rejection would suggest greater attention engagement for that event for both mother and children in line with the proposed salience of kin-bonds over others. Thirdly, we hypothesized that the negative left frontal ERP slow waves would inversely predict greater distress for all participants. We also expected that rejection from kin (mother or child) would induce a more positive slow wave than rejection by a stranger, reflecting generally increased approach motivation upon signs of threat to this relationship. Finally, given the asymmetric relationship of the mother-child dyad (i.e., mothers aid their offspring’s survival, but not vice versa), and that mothers are likely more adept at emotion regulation, we expected children to be more distressed by exclusion than their mothers (George & Solomon, 2008).

Method

Participants

Twenty-one children (9 male, ages 9 to 12 years old, Mage = 11.048, SDage = 1.071) were recruited along with their mothers (ages 40 to 52, Mage = 45.095, SDage = 3.646) via mass mailings. Both mothers and children received $40 compensation for participating in the visit. Mothers provided written consent for themselves and permission for their children—children provided written assent. Yale University’s Human Investigation Committee approved the study protocol.

Procedure

Cyberball paradigm

Participants sat 60 cm from a 17 in LCD screen in a dimly lit sound attenuated room while participating in the game Cyberball. In this computerized game, participants must throw and receive a virtual ball, along with two pre-programmed players. Participants were led to believe the two other players in the game were real. In our mother-child version of Cyberball, children were told they were playing with a stranger child and their own mother—mothers were led to believe they were playing with a stranger mother and their own child. The game was divided into two phases: fair play, a series of trials where the ball is evenly thrown between all players, and exclusion, a series of trials where the ball is only thrown between the preprogrammed players.

The play screen was programmed such that each participant views his/her own glove at the bottom center and the other players’ gloves on the opposite sides at the top of the screen next to pictures of the other players. One of the pictures was the mother or child of the participant and the other picture was a demographic and sex-matched photo (of either another mother or child). In order to choose whom to pass to, the participant used their left and right index fingers on a response pad to throw the ball during play.

Authenticity of the game was enhanced with a Google™page with a “Cyberball” listing that was linked to a false “loading screen.” Participants were able to choose different gloves for play, different sound effects occurred for throws and catches, and balls thrown varied in trajectory. Before beginning the game, the experimenter vocally hinted at making sure that “additional players” were getting ready to play. The falsity of the game was revealed during a debriefing after completion of the experiment.

This adapted version of Cyberball had two conditions, 108 trials (throws) of fair play and 47 trials of exclusion. Fair play was further divided into 36 “my turn” events, where the participant received the ball from the other players, and 72 “not my turn” events, where the ball was thrown between the other players. The game was fixed with a waiting time to receive the ball, waiting 0, 1, 2 or 3 trials before receiving it again (frequency 12, 12, 10 and 2, respectively).

Immediately following fair play the game transitioned into an exclusion phase. In this condition there were 44 “rejection” events, where the ball was thrown between the other players, and 3 “my turn” events. This resulted in exclusion on 96% of the trials. For the purpose of analysis, the first five trials, the three “my turn” events, and following trial where the participant throws the ball were excluded. Thus only 36 trials of rejection-based events were ultimately examined for analysis.

Behavioral Measures of Ostracism

After the Cyberball session, participants filled out the Need Threat Scale to measure ostracism distress. This adapted 21-question version has been shown to be reliable and valid in past research (Crowley et al., 2010; Jamieson et al., 2010; C. Sebastian et al., 2010; van Beest & Williams, 2006). Feelings of distress, rated 1 (“Not at all”) to 5 (“Extremely”), were gathered and final scores were rekeyed such that higher scores indicated with higher levels of distress.

EEG Recording and Preprocessing

Standard protocol was used to obtain a high-density EEG with a 128 Ag/AgCL electrode system (Electrical Geodesics Incorporated (EGI), Netstation v.4.2 software (EGI), and high impedance amplifiers (sampled at 250 Hz: 0.1 Hz high pass, 100 Hz low pass) acquired with a Cz reference. Impedances were all under 40k Ohms at the outset of the assessment. Stimulus presentation was conducted using E-prime v. 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.).

Post-collection processing followed standard procedures, including offline low pass filtering at 30 Hz and segmentation with a 100 ms baseline and 900 ms post-stimulus onset and re-referencing to an average reference. An artifact detection threshold was set at 200 μV, with an eye movement/blink threshold of 150 μV. Any channels/segments that did not fit these criteria were removed from further analysis. Ocular artifact removal (OAR) was implemented to correct eye movements/blinks for all participants (Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1983). We relied on an analogous channel cluster as used in our previous examination of frontal slow wave negativity and Cyberball (White et al., 2012). EGI Hydrocell net channels 19, 18, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 32, 33, 38 were used for these trials (Figure 1). For the purposes of analysis, the ERPs that correspond to any throws between the participant and one of the players during fair play were referred to as favor-based ERPs. Similarly, the ERPs that corresponded to throws between the other players during exclusion were referred to as rejection-based ERPs. We further distinguished between throws that involved kin (mother or child) or a stranger. A throw by the kin to the stranger (as opposed to the participant) during exclusion was considered a rejection-based ERP of kin. Conversely, a throw by the stranger to the kin (as opposed to the participant) during exclusion was considered a rejection-based ERP of stranger. For the purposes of this analysis, the number of events designated as “rejection events” was 36, 18 trials for kin rejection and 18 trials for stranger rejection. After signal processing, the numbers of trials available for averaging was comparable across conditions (Stranger Rejection: Mean, 15.10; SD, 2.93; range, 6-18; Kin Rejection: Mean, 15.55; SD, 2.86; Range 8-18).

Dyadic Analysis

Because the data were collected in terms of mother-child dyads, a hierarchical linear mixed model ANOVA was implemented to account for this level of analysis. Factors examined were role (2 levels: mother or child), identity (2 levels: stranger or kin), and condition (2 levels: fair play or exclusion); ostracism distress was added to the model as a covariate.

Results

Following our previous work, we examined the left frontal cortical region (White et al., 2012) (Figure 1). Figure 2 depicts current density spline maps for kin and stranger rejection. The frontal left-lateralized nature of the effects appears across all voltage maps. Figure 3 depicts ERP waveforms for the slow wave and P2 responses for stranger and kin. Rejection-based ERPs were examined with SPSS v.19 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois, USA). As a preliminary analysis we examined associations between sex, age and our ERP measurements of interest (left slow wave, left frontal P2). Sex of the children was not related to any of the questionnaire scores or the ERP recordings, with all t values t(19) < 1.674 and all associated p values p > 0.111. Additionally, children’s age was not related to any of the measures with all r values < 0.333 and associated p-values > 0.141.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Voltage maps of rejection-based ERPs during Cyberball with frontal left electrodes overlaid (black dots). (A) Mother P2 response at 200 ms for rejection-based ERPs for strangers (left) and kin (right). (B) Child P2 response at 200 ms for rejection-based ERPs for strangers. (C) Mother slow wave response at 675 ms for rejection-based ERPs for strangers (left) and kin (right). (B) Child slow wave response at 675 ms for rejection-based ERPs for strangers.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Rejection-based ERPs of the P2 peak, 100 – 300 ms, and the slow wave, 450 – 900 ms, for both mother and children during Cyberball. (A) Rejection-based ERPs for the average frontal left electrodes for children playing with a stranger and their mother. (B) Rejection-based ERPs for the average frontal left electrodes for mothers playing with a stranger and their child.

Slow wave differences between stranger and kin

A linear mixed model ANOVA: role (mother or child) x identity (stranger or kin) x distress (score from 21 to 105) revealed a significant effect of identity for the slow wave (450-900 ms), F(1, 31.698) = 37.972, p < 0.001, as well as a identity x distress interaction, F(1, 37.103) = 11.427, p = 0.002.

In order to further understand the significant effect of identity, follow-up paired t-tests were conducted separately for mothers and children. Children showed significantly more negative rejection-based ERPs for strangers than for their mothers, t(40)= −5.702, p < 0.001 (means = −5.046 v. 3.083) (for a bar graph see Figure 4). Similarly, mothers showed a similar pattern of differences in the rejection-based ERPs between strangers and their children, t(40)= −9.150, p < 0.001 (means = −4.827 v. 4.186) (for a bar graph see Figure 4). Additionally, children exhibited greater self-reported scores of distress as compared to their mothers, t(20)= 3.517, p = 0.002 (means = 45.095 v. 32.428).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Average slow wave amplitude (μV), 450-900 ms, and average P2 peak amplitude (μV), 100-300 ms, of rejection-based ERPs for mothers and children. (A) Both mothers and their children showed similar brain responses to rejection, with a significantly more positive slow wave for rejection-based ERPs from kin than strangers during exclusion (mothers: t(40)= −9.150, p < 0.001; children: t(40)= −5.702, p < 0.001). (B) Similarly, mothers and their children showed greater P2 peaks during rejection-based ERPs from kin than strangers during exclusion (mothers: t(40)= 12.238, p < 0.001; children: t(40)= 8.638, p < 0.001).

The interaction of identity x distress was analyzed post-hoc in two different ways. A simple bivariate correlation was conducted between slow wave and ostracism distress indicating there was a significant association for kin, r = −0.481, p = 0.001, but there was not a significant association for strangers, r = 0.243, p = 0.121 (for scatter plots see Figure 5). This analysis was followed by a partial correlation controlling for dyad (mother-child pair), still revealing a significant association for kin, r = −0.409, p = 0.008, but not for stranger, r = 0.241, p = 0.129.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Scatter plot of ostracism distress scores against average slow wave (top row) and P2 peak (bottom row) for rejection-based ERPs for strangers (left) and kin (right). There was no significant correlation for strangers (Slow wave: r = 0.243, p = 0.121; P2 peak: r = 0.144, p = 0.362). However there was a significant correlation for kin, where greater distress correlated with a more negative slow wave (r = −0.481, p = 0.001) and negative P2 peak (r = −0.410, p = 0.007).

P2 peak differences between stranger and kin

Analysis of the rejection-based P2 peak (100-300ms) followed the same approach as that for the slow wave (Figure 3). The same linear mixed model ANOVA: role (2) x identity (2) x distress revealed a significant effect of identity for the P2 peak F(1, 35.285) = 28.007, p < 0.001, as well as a identity x distress, F(1, 42.928) = 8.700, p = 0.005.

Since comparable interactions were observed as with the slow wave, analogous post-hoc tests were conducted. These revealed mothers showed more positive P2 peaks for kin as compared to strangers, t(40)= 12.238, p < 0.001 (means = 0.516 v. 5.969) (for a bar graph see Figure 4). A comparable pattern was seen in children, t(40)= 8.638, p < 0.001 (means = 1.239 v. 6.068) (for a bar graph see Figure 4).

Similar to the analysis of the slow wave, the P2 peak was analyzed for association with distress, with and without control for dyad membership. Bivariate correlations revealed a strong association for kin, r = −0.410, p = 0.007, and a non-significant association with distress for strangers, r = 0.144, p = 0.362 (for scatter plots see Figure 5). The partial correlation controlling for dyad yielded comparable results, a significant association for kin, r = −0.409, p = 0.008, but no association for stranger, r = 0.146, p = 0.363 (for scatter plots see Figure 5).

Given the negligible difference between the correlation for slow wave kin rejection and ostracism and the correlation between the P2 for kin rejection and ostracism (−0.48 vs. −0.41), it is possible that our effects are driven by a sustained response, beginning early at the time of the P2 that is superimposed on the slow wave. As an exploratory analysis, we examined the extent to which the P2 and the slow wave were distinct components by way of a principal component analysis with varimax rotation. One principal component factor, coinciding with the slow wave, covered 560 - 900 ms and accounted for 43.16% of the variance. Another principal component factor, spanning 112 - 396 ms, corresponded with the P2 response, accounting for 14.66% of the variance. This analysis suggests that the P2 and the slow wave may be considered distinct from a statistical variability perspective.

Discussion

We examined social exclusion in the context of mother-child dyads and the potential moderating role of this relationship on neural responding during social exclusion. Confirming our predictions, the magnitude of the left-frontal slow wave and the P2 increased in response to kin-rejection as compared to stranger-rejection. In addition, self-reported distress assessed post-exclusion was uniquely associated with neural response for kin-rejection as opposed to stranger-rejection, suggesting that when participants reported on felt exclusion this specifically reflected their responses to rejection by kin.

Ample evidence implicates the P2 in preferential processing and perception of salient stimuli (e.g., Luck & Hillyard, 1994) as well as incidence detection (Key et al., 2005). Against this backdrop, our findings here of augmented P2 upon kin rejection imply that rejection by kin, as opposed to strangers, engages more attentional resources and suggests that participants were generally more responsive to rejection events by their kin than by a stranger. More broadly, because the events in ERP Cyberball essentially reflect positions on the screen, where the player’s knowledge of the other social agent’s identity (mother, child or stranger) frames the meaning of the event, we believe we have tapped into a unique social signal reflecting preferential attention allocation to a “high salience” rejection event, one by kin.

Due to the temporal nature of the P2 and the discrepancies observed between kin and stranger-rejection, our results suggest that this event is processed rapidly, in less than a quarter of a second. Furthermore, greater distress was associated with a smaller P2 peak, in line with research showing a negative correlation with this peak and fear states (Lyby, Aslaksen, & Flaten, 2011; Sarlo & Munafo, 2010). While a large P2 peak may suggest greater responsiveness to external cues, the smaller P2 peak for more distressed participants could reflect greater disengagement from external events (throws) for these subjects or possibly greater avoidance. Relatedly, reduced P2 amplitudes have been observed in phobics responding to feared stimuli and also to reduced placebo responding among those with greater fear of pain (Lyby et al., 2011; Sarlo & Munafo, 2010).

Concerning our slow wave findings, previous work links left-lateralized slow waves to appetitive motivation and positive appraisals (e.g., Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2013). Compared to stranger-rejection, kin-rejection may therefore also prompt stronger tendencies to approach and regain proximity to kin given the high evolutionary premium put on this specific bond relative to encounters with strangers. Moreover, given the links of frontal slow wave activity to more positive appraisals and diminished ostracism distress (e.g., Crowley et al., 2010; White et al., 2012), kin may build on previous warm interactions with one another (as should be the overall trend in the healthy sample studied here) to anticipate a more conciliatory outcome in the face of current rejection.

Looking across the P2 and slow wave responses, we note that kin and stranger differences beginning at the P2 carry over as amplitude differences maintained across the slow wave. As well, the P2 and the slow wave for Kin rejection events relate similarly to felt ostracism. One possibility is that the P2 reflects a continuation of the slow wave effect. We conducted a PCA, which suggested at least at the level of statistical variability the P2 and slow wave are distinct. The common pattern of relation of the P2 and the slow wave with ostracism could just as easily reflect a cascade of processes such as attentional engagement (P2) followed by slow wave reactivity which could reflect a range of cognitive processes reflecting that may be more variable in time, such as appraisal and affect-regulation.

At the level of self-reported ostracism distress, our findings dovetail with classic work in the attachment literature, demonstrating that maternal departure elicits stronger negative affect than that of a stranger and that strangers cannot replace caregivers as key sources of comfort (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969). Yet, our results also showed that mothers and children responded similarly for rejection ERP events by kin and strangers during social exclusion—rejection by a stranger tended to induce a more negative frontal slow wave than did rejection by kin (Figure 3). Interestingly, both mothers and children showed relatively more positive slow wave rejection-based ERPs for interactions with each other as compared to interactions with a stranger (Figure 3). Likewise, maturation of cognitive capacity and coping skills may also partly account for the lower levels of distress in mothers versus children (Gullone, Hughes, King, & Tonge, 2010) as suggested by findings of age-related increases in neural activation of left-lateralized frontal areas during rejection (Gunther Moor, van Leijenhorst, Rombouts, Crone, & Van der Molen, 2010).

In terms of ERP-ostracism associations, since extant studies find that greater distress was associated with a more negative frontal slow wave for exclusion by strangers, we expected to observe this association here (Crowley et al., 2010; White et al., 2012). Instead, despite yielding more positive slow waves overall, only kin-based slow wave rejection events, and not stranger rejection events, were associated with ostracism distress (Figure 5). In interpreting these findings, it is important to note that while stranger and kin ERPs could be distinguished, ostracism distress was measured globally. Specifically, items on the Need Threat scale did not differentiate how the participant felt when excluded by a stranger versus how they felt when excluded by their kin. Because the participants demonstrated neural indicators of distress in response to the kin-initiated exclusion, we infer that the psychological reports of ostracism distress pertained to those kin-initiated exclusion events. The fact that self-reports only mapped onto neural responses to rejection by kin rather than strangers concords with our neural evidence of increased salience of kin-rejection (P2).

As expected, children reported greater levels of global distress after the game as compared to their mothers. This pattern may support the notion that mothers are less affected by rejection from their children than vice versa (George & Solomon, 2008). In other words, the observed differences in responding may be due to the differences in expectations mothers and children hold regarding their relationship with one another. Whereas children expect to receive care from their own mothers, mothers are less likely to look to their children for loving care and therefore ought to be less disturbed by rejecting behavior (George & Solomon, 2008). Our findings also meshes with parent-offspring conflict theory, in that offspring seek to maximize parental investment while parents need to balance their investment across all offspring as well as maintain their own fitness. Accordingly, ruptures to the mother-child bond may evoke greater levels of protest and distress from children than from parents (e.g., Barrett & Dunbar, 1994).

Importantly, we did not assess parent-child relationship quality or attachment status in this study, leaving open the question of the extent to which these variables might mitigate or accentuate the neural or self-report aspects of social exclusion by kin. The majority of research in the attachment field involves examining concurrent and prospective correlates of individual differences in attachment. Studies generally find that attachment security is associated with more optimal development and health among both children and adults (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van, 2009; De Wolff & van Ijzendoorn, 1997; Fonagy et al., 1996; Kobak, Cole, Ferenz-Gillies, Fleming, & Gamble, 1993; Roisman et al., 2007). Our study provides evidence that both mothers and children exhibit greater neural signs of distress in response to rejection from kin than from strangers. While we cannot speak to the impact of the attachment relationship, we propose that our findings open a new avenue for attachment research—that of exploring the role of individual differences in children’s and mother’s neural and subjective response to ostracism. As social beings, humans extrapolate from early experience to predict how future relational events will unfold (Bowlby, 1958). Therefore, we would expect that for children with extensive histories of rejection from caregivers (such as those with insecure or disorganized attachments), simulated ostracism may result in stronger neural reactions to distress as compared to the responses of children with relatively fewer experiences of rejection (such as those with secure attachments).

Importantly, we have framed this study from the perspective of mother-child attachment, but kin and strangers also differ on a second fundamental variable, familiarity. Studies of familiarity typically involve neural response to faces of familiar others (posterior P2) (Caharel et al., 2002; Webb et al., 2010). In our version of Cyberball, the face images remained on the screen for the entire game. Thus, the events that we focused on reflected rejection throws as “actions” by familiar others versus strangers rather than a physical representation of a person. Nonetheless, our effects may reflect social factors not directly specific to kin or to the mother-child relationship in particular, such as greater familiarity or a broadly construed ingroup versus outgroup bias. For instance in a pain perception study, P2 empathic responses were significantly reduced toward racial out-group faces compared to racial in-group faces (Sheng and Han, 2012). Notably, we observed larger P2 responses for rejection by kin compared to a stranger. From another perspective, while social exclusion by ingroup versus outgroup members gives rise to elevated self-reported distress and increased ACC activation (Bernstein, Sacco, Young, Hugenberg, & Cook, 2010; Krill & Platek, 2009), we documented a more positive frontal late slow wave for kin (associated with less distress) than for strangers (not associated with less distress). Also, in a related behavioral study, participants reported lower levels of need threat and self-reported distress when rejected by their romantic partner and a stranger than when rejected by two strangers (Capezza, Reed, Arriaga, & Williams, 2009 cited by Wesselmann & Williams, 2013). These studies, in combination with our findings, lead us surmise that rejection by individuals with whom we share an attachment may potentially yield specific effects beyond what might be accounted for by greater familiarity. Ultimately however, contrasting kin, familiar others and strangers as players in Cyberball probably reflects the most straightforward approach to disentangling potential familiarity and kin effects. As well, examining attachment quality variables with Cyberball (White, Wu, Borelli, Mayes, & Crowley, 2013; White et al., 2012) could provide support for a unique role of kin in response to social exclusion.

In light of our findings, there are some limitations worthy of note. The Need Threat scale that was used to evaluate ostracism includes general questions about distress that are not specific to the mother-child relationship. Implementation of specific questions to gauge the distress observed when excluded by one’s mother or child may be beneficial for future studies. Similarly, our findings may not hold for other age groups. Especially in adolescence, normative distancing may occur within parent-child relationships (Kerns, 2008), potentially rendering rejection more common (and less salient).

From another vantage point, assessment of attachment related variables in mothers and children could also shed light on this issue if attachment security uniquely accounted for responding to rejection by kin. Finally, there is also a possibility that the deception employed in this study was not as effective for the mothers; in other words, they were aware that they were participating in a study and suspected they were not playing with their child over the computer. This could also account for the differential (lower) ostracism distress reported by mothers, although this point can be viewed against several other qualifying factors. First, ostracism distress and neural response were similarly associated across mothers and children. Second, and relatedly, the act of measuring ostracism distress is likely to make participants aware of the deception (the items communicate that exclusion was pre-ordained). Despite this possibility, felt distress still tracked neural response. Moreover, work that frames Cyberball as a hypothetical game (without deception) still yields similar patterns of felt ostracism (Crowley et al., 2009; Zadro, Williams, & Richardson, 2004).

This study is the first we know of to examine the effects of exclusion in both mothers and their child. We have shown that both mothers and children show similar and rapid neural responses (for social signals) to kin rejection events that similarly track ostracism distress, though more strongly for children. The similarities in neural responses among these kin members are consistent with an inherent value placed on relationships with kin versus those with others. Our findings represent a promising new area of inquiry for relationship research. This methodology could potentially be used to probe in other types of relationships in real time (e.g. friends, intimate partners) or potentially assesses aspects of relationship quality not accessible through questionnaires or behavioral observations. More broadly, with our ERP approach to Cyberball, a dynamic task superimposed on static events, we show that neural processing of naturalistic social signals can be studied in real time and is sensitive to specific information of agent identity at a very early processing stage. As such, our study has relevance for the pursuit of biomarkers and endophenotypes in psychiatry. This is, to our knowledge, the first “real-time” ERP social signal putatively acquired in the context of a real social relationship, where the neural response reflects differential perception of the action of one agent (kin) versus another (stranger). Thus our approach could be useful for studying social cognition in autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the Bial Foundation (MJC).NIH grant: DA034125

References

  1. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1978. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ainsworth MDS, Wittig B. Attachment and exploratory behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. In: Foss B, editor. Determinants of infant behaviour. Vol. 4. Methuen; London: 1969. pp. 111–136. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. The first 10,000 Adult Attachment Interviews: distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical and non-clinical groups. Attachment & Human Development. 2009;11(3):223–263. doi: 10.1080/14616730902814762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett L, Dunbar R. Not now dear, I’m busy. New Scientist. 1994;142(1920):30–34. [Google Scholar]
  5. Benoit D, Parker KC. Stability and transmission of attachment across three generations. Child Development. 1994;65(5):1444–1456. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00828.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bernstein MJ, Sacco DF, Young SG, Hugenberg K, Cook E. Being "in" with the in-crowd: the effects of social exclusion and inclusion are enhanced by the perceived essentialism of ingroups and outgroups. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 2010;36(8):999–1009. doi: 10.1177/0146167210376059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bolling DZ, Pitskel NB, Deen B, Crowley MJ, Mayes LC, Pelphrey KA. Development of neural systems for processing social exclusion from childhood to adolescence. Developmental Science. 2011;14(6):1431–1444. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01087.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bowlby J. The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis. 1958;39:350–373. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bowlby J. A secure base. Routledge; London: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bowlby J, Ainsworth M, Boston M, Rosenbluth D. The effects of mother - child separation: a follow-up study. British Journal of Medical Psychology. 1956;29(3-4):211–247. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1956.tb00915.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cacioppo S, Frum C, Asp E, Weiss RM, Lewis JW, Cacioppo JT. A Quantitative Meta-Analysis of Functional Imaging Studies of Social Rejection. Scientific Reports. 2013;3 doi: 10.1038/srep02027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Capezza NM, Reed JT, Arriaga XB, Williams KD. Does being ostracized by a romantic partner partner hurt as much as being ostracized by a stranger?. Paper presented at the Annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology; Tampa, FL. 2009. [Google Scholar]
  13. Caharel S, Poiroux S, Bernard C, Thibaut F, Lalonde R, Rebai M. ERPs associated with familiarity and degree of familiarity during face recognition. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;112(12):1499–1512. doi: 10.1080/00207450290158368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Coyne SM, Gundersen N, Nelson DA, Stockdale L. Adolescents’ prosocial responses to ostracism: an experimental study. Journal of Social Psychology. 2011;151(5):657–661. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2010.522625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Crowley MJ, Wu J, McCarty ER, David DH, Bailey CA, Mayes LC. Exclusion and micro-rejection: event-related potential response predicts mitigated distress. Neuroreport. 2009;20(17):1518–1522. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328330377a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Crowley MJ, Wu J, Molfese PJ, Mayes LC. Social exclusion in middle childhood: rejection events, slow wave neural activity, and ostracism distress. Social Neuroscience. 2010;5(5-6):483–495. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2010.500169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cunningham WA, Espinet SD, DeYoung CG, Zelazo PD. Attitudes to the right- and left: Frontal ERP asymmetries associated with stimulus valence and processing goals. Social Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;28:827–834. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. De Wolff MS, van Ijzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: a meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development. 1997;68(4):571–591. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. DeWall CN, Masten CL, Powell C, Combs D, Schurtz DR, Eisenberger NI. Do neural responses to rejection depend on attachment style? An fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7(2):184–192. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Eastwood JE, Jalaludin B, Kemp L, Phung H, Barnett BA, Tobin J. Erratum: Social exclusion, infant behavior, social isolation, and maternal expectations independently predict maternal depressive symptoms. Brain and Behavior. 2013;3(3):327. doi: 10.1002/brb3.107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Eisenberger NI. Identifying the Neural Correlates Underlying Social Pain: Implications for Developmental Processes. Human Development. 2006;49(5):273–293. [Google Scholar]
  22. Eisenberger NI. The neural bases of social pain: evidence for shared representations with physical pain. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2012;74(2):126–135. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182464dd1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eisenberger NI, Gable SL, Lieberman MD. Functional magnetic resonance imaging responses relate to differences in real-world social experience. Emotion. 2007;7(4):745–754. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Williams KD. Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science. 2003;302(5643):290–292. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fidler B. Jo, Bala N. Children resisting postseparation contact with a parent: Concepts, controversies, and conundrums. Family Court Review. 2010;48(1):10–47. [Google Scholar]
  26. Flacking R, Lehtonen L, Thomson G, Axelin A, Ahlqvist S, Moran VH, Dykes F. Closeness and separation in neonatal intensive care. Acta Paediatrica. 2012;101(10):1032–1037. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2012.02787.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fonagy P, Leigh T, Steele M, Steele H, Kennedy R, Mattoon G, Gerber A. The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and response to psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64(1):22–31. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.1.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Francis DD, Szegda K, Campbell G, Martin WD, Insel TR. Epigenetic sources of behavioral differences in mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6(5):445–446. doi: 10.1038/nn1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Friedlander S, Walters MG. When a child rejects a parent: tailoring the intervention to fit the problem. Family Court Review. 2010;48(1):98–111. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gable Philip A., Harmon-Jones E. Late positive potential to appetitive stimuli and local attentional bias. Emotion. 2010;10(3):441–446. doi: 10.1037/a0018425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gable PA, Harmon-Jones E. Does arousal per se account for the influence of appetitive stimuli on attentional scope and the late positive potential? Psychophysiology. 2013;50(4):344–350. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. George C, Solomon J. Representational models of relationships: Links between caregiving and attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1996;17:198–216. [Google Scholar]
  33. George C, Solomon J. The Caregiving System: A Behavioral Systems Approach to Parenting. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 833–856. [Google Scholar]
  34. Graham RC, Cabeza R. Event-related potentials of recognizing happy and neutral faces. Neuroreport. 2001;12(2):245–248. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E. A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1983;55(4):468–484. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gullone E, Hughes EK, King NJ, Tonge B. The normative development of emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: a 2-year follow-up study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2010;51(5):567–574. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02183.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gunther Moor B, van Leijenhorst L, Rombouts SARB, Crone EA, Van der Molen MW. Do you like me? Neural correlates of social evaluation and developmental trajectories. Social Neuroscience. 2010;5(5-6):461–482. doi: 10.1080/17470910903526155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Harlow HF, Suomi SJ. Social recovery by isolation-reared monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1971;68(7):1534–1538. doi: 10.1073/pnas.68.7.1534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Harlow HF, Zimmermann RR. The development of affective responsiveness in infant monkeys. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society. 1958;102:501–509. [Google Scholar]
  40. Isabella RA. Origins of attachment: Maternal interactive behavior acrosss the first year. Child Development. 1993;64:605–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02931.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jamieson JP, Harkins SG, Williams KD. Need threat can motivate performance after ostracism. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 2010;36(5):690–702. doi: 10.1177/0146167209358882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kerns KA. Attachment in middle childhood. In: Cassidy PSJ, editor. Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 366–382. [Google Scholar]
  43. Key AP, Dove GO, Maguire MJ. Linking brainwaves to the brain: an ERP primer. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;27(2):183–215. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2702_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kim YS, Koh YJ, Leventhal B. School bullying and suicidal risk in Korean middle school students. Pediatrics. 2005;115(2):357–363. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0902. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Klann-Delius G, Hofmeister C. The development of communicative competence of securely and insecurely attached children in interactions with their mothers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 1997;26(1):69–88. doi: 10.1023/a:1025012221407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kobak RR, Cole HE, Ferenz-Gillies R, Fleming WS, Gamble W. Attachment and emotion regulation during mother-teen problem solving: a control theory analysis. Child Development. 1993;64(1):231–245. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Krill A, Platek SM. In-group and out-group membership mediates anterior cingulate activation to social exclusion. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience. 2009;1:1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.18.001.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Luck SJ, Hillyard SA. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology. 1994;31(3):291–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02218.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lyby PS, Aslaksen PM, Flaten MA. Variability in placebo analgesia and the role of fear of pain--an ERP study. Pain. 2011;152(10):2405–2412. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Macdonald G, Leary MR. Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131(2):202–223. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Main M, Goldwyn R. Predicting rejection of her infant from mother’s representation of her own experience: implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational cycle. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1984;8(2):203–217. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(84)90009-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Malatesta CZ, Culver C, Tesman JR, Shepard B. The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1989;54(1-2):1–104. discussion 105-136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Borofsky LA, Pfeifer JH, McNealy K, Mazziotta JC, Dapretto M. Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: understanding the distress of peer rejection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2009;4(2):143–157. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Moss E, St-Laurent D. Attachment at school age and academic performance. Developmental Psychology. 2001;37(6):863–874. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mueller V, Brehmer Y, von Oertzen T, Li SC, Lindenberger U. Electrophysiological correlates of selective attention: a lifespan comparison. BMC Neuroscience. 2008;9:18. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nesdale D, Flesser D. Social identity and the development of children’s group attitudes. Child Development. 2001;72(2):506–517. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Riem MM, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Huffmeijer R, van Ijzendoorn MH. Does intranasal oxytocin promote prosocial behavior to an excluded fellow player? A randomized-controlled trial with Cyberball. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(8):1418–1425. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Roisman GI, Holland A, Fortuna K, Fraley RC, Clausell E, Clarke A. The Adult Attachment Interview and self-reports of attachment style: an empirical rapprochement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92(4):678–697. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Salvy SJ, Bowker JC, Nitecki LA, Kluczynski MA, Germeroth LJ, Roemmich JN. Impact of simulated ostracism on overweight and normal-weight youths’ motivation to eat and food intake. Appetite. 2011;56(1):39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2010.11.140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sarlo M, Munafo M. When faces signal danger: event-related potentials to emotional facial expressions in animal phobics. Neuropsychobiology. 2010;62(4):235–244. doi: 10.1159/000319950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schaffer HR, Emerson PE. The Development of Social Attachments in Infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1964;29:94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schwartz D, Gorman AH, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Friendships with peers who are low or high in aggression as moderators of the link between peer victimization and declines in academic functioning. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(5):719–730. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9200-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sebastian CL, Tan GC, Roiser JP, Viding E, Dumontheil I, Blakemore SJ. Developmental influences on the neural bases of responses to social rejection: implications of social neuroscience for education. NeuroImage. 2011;57(3):686–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sebastian C, Viding E, Williams KD, Blakemore SJ. Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain and Cognition. 2010;72(1):134–145. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sheng F, Han S. Manipulations of cognitive strategies and intergroup relationships reduce the racial bias in empathic neural responses. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):786–797. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Smith JL, Johnstone SJ, Barry RJ. Inhibitory processing during the Go/NoGo task: an ERP analysis of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004;115(6):1320–1331. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sullivan R, Perry R, Sloan A, Kleinhaus K, Burtchen N. Infant bonding and attachment to the caregiver: insights from basic and clinical science. Clinics in Perinatology. 2011;38(4):643–655. doi: 10.1016/j.clp.2011.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Thompson RA. Early Attachment and Later Development: Familiar Questions, New Answers. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. Guilford Press; New York: 2008. pp. 348–365. [Google Scholar]
  69. van Beest I, Williams KD. When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts. Journal of Personality Social Psychology. 2006;91(5):918–928. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. van de Laar MC, Licht R, Franken IHA, Hendriks VM. Event-related potentials indicate motivational relevance of cocaine cues in abstinent cocaine addicts. Psychopharmacology. 2004;177(1):121–129. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1928-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. van IJzendoorn MH. Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: a meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117(3):387–403. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ward MJ, Carlson EA. The predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview for adolescent mothers. Child Development. 1995;66:69–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00856.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Webb SJ, Jones EJ, Merkle K, Murias M, Greenson J, Richards T, Aylward E, Dawson G. Response to familiar faces, newly familiar faces, and novel faces as assessed by ERPs is intact in adults with autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2010;77(2):106–117. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wesselmann ED, Williams KD. Ostracism and stages of coping. In: DeWall CN, editor. The Oxford handbook of social exclusion. Oxford University Press; New York: 2013. pp. 20–30. [Google Scholar]
  75. White LO, Wu J, Borelli JL, Rutherford HJV, David DH, Kim–Cohen J, Crowley MJ. Attachment dismissal predicts frontal slow wave ERPs during rejection by unfamiliar peers. Emotion. 2012;12(4):690–700. doi: 10.1037/a0026750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. White LO, Wu J, Borelli JL, Mayes LC, Crowley MJ. Play it again: neural responses to reunion with excluders predicted by attachment patterns. Developmental Science. 2013;16(6):850–863. doi: 10.1111/desc.12035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Williams KD. Ostracism. Annual Review Psychology. 2007;58:425–452. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Williams KD, Jarvis B. Cyberball: a program for use in research on interpersonal ostracism and acceptance. Behavior Research Methods. 2006;38(1):174–180. doi: 10.3758/bf03192765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Zadro L, Williams KD, Richardson R. How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2004;40(4):560–567. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES