Skip to main content
World Psychiatry logoLink to World Psychiatry
. 2014 Oct 1;13(3):224–232. doi: 10.1002/wps.20145

Cognitive impairments in psychotic disorders: common mechanisms and measurement

Deanna M Barch 1, Julia M Sheffield 1
PMCID: PMC4219054  PMID: 25273286

Abstract

Decades of research have provided robust evidence of cognitive impairments in psychotic disorders. Individuals with schizophrenia appear to be impaired on the majority of neuropsychological tasks, leading some researchers to argue for a “generalized deficit”, in which the multitude of cognitive impairments are the result of a common neurobiological source. One such common mechanism may be an inability to actively represent goal information in working memory as a means to guide behavior, with the associated neurobiological impairment being a disturbance in the function of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Here, we provide a discussion of the evidence for such impairment in schizophrenia, and how it manifests in domains typically referred to as cognitive control, working memory and episodic memory. We also briefly discuss cognitive impairment in affective psychoses, reporting that the degree of impairment is worse in schizophrenia than in bipolar disorder and psychotic major depression, but the profile of impairment is similar, possibly reflecting common mechanisms at the neural level. Given the recent release of the DSM-5, we end with a brief discussion on assessing cognition in the context of diagnosis and treatment planning in psychotic disorders.

Keywords: Cognitive control, working memory, episodic memory, cognitive deficits, schizophrenia, psychotic disorders, generalized deficit, DSM-5


The last four decades have produced an impressive body of research on cognition in schizophrenia, in part prompted by the evidence that cognitive function is a critical determinant of quality of life and everyday functioning in people with this disorder, potentially more so than the severity of symptoms such as hallucinations and delusions (13).

A strikingly consistent finding within the cognitive neuroscience literature is that patients with schizophrenia display deficits on a huge variety of neuropsychological tasks (4,5). Historically, researchers had hypothesized impairments in specific cognitive domains with pockets of intact functioning in these patients, but there has been a recent push to re-conceptualize the range of deficits in schizophrenia as reflecting a “generalized” or “global” cognitive deficit, implying that cognitive impairments across domains share a common neurobiological source (610).

One such common mechanism may be an inability to actively represent goal information in working memory as a means to guide behavior, with the associated neurobiological impairment being a disturbance in the function of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and its interactions with other brain regions such as the parietal cortex, the thalamus, and the striatum, and the influence of neurotransmitter systems such as dopamine, GABA and glutamate (1113).

In this paper, we provide a discussion of the evidence for such impairment in schizophrenia, and how it manifests in domains typically referred to as cognitive control, working memory (WM) and episodic memory (EM). We also briefly discuss how cognitive impairments manifest across psychotic disorders in both the non-affective and affective psychosis domains. We end with an overview of the assessment of cognition in the DSM-5.

COGNITIVE CONTROL AND GOAL REPRESENTATIONS IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

In recent years, cognitive impairment in schizophrenia has been conceptualized as a deficit in the function of proactive cognitive control (12,1416), or the ability to proactively maintain goal representations that can be used to guide ongoing behavior.

This conceptualization builds upon earlier ideas on the use of context information in psychosis (e.g., 17-19), to argue for flexible mechanisms of cognitive control that allow humans to deal with the diversity of challenges they face in everyday life. In this theory, termed dual mechanisms of control (12,14,15), a distinction is made between proactive and reactive modes of cognitive control.

The proactive control mode can be thought of as a form of ‘‘early selection'', in which goal-relevant information is actively maintained in a sustained or anticipatory manner, before the occurrence of cognitively demanding events. This allows for the biasing of attention, perception, and action systems in a goal-driven manner. Goal information refers to information about what one needs to accomplish in a particular task or situation, or the intended outcome of a series of actions or mental operations. In real life, such goals may include the need to avoid eating a piece of cake while on a diet, maintaining points one wishes to communicate in a conversation, or overriding habits (e.g., driving straight home) to accomplish a specific goal (pick up one's dry cleaning).

In contrast, in the reactive mode, attentional control is recruited as a ‘‘late correction'' mechanism that is mobilized only when needed, such as after a high-interference event is detected. For example, such a reactive control mechanism might be engaged if you encounter an unexpected distracting stimulus and need to retrieve the topic of your conversation, or if your mind wanders and you suddenly find yourself at a critical intersection, where one direction leads home and the other leads to the dry cleaners. Thus, proactive control relies on the anticipation and prevention of interference before it occurs, whereas reactive control relies on the detection and resolution of interference after its onset.

This dual mechanisms of control theory, similar to other theories about cognitive control, suggests that proactive control depends on actively representing information in lateral prefrontal cortex (20), using this information to coordinate activity with other psychological and neural systems (21,22), and that the updating and maintenance of such information depends on precise inputs from neurotransmitter systems such as dopamine into prefrontal cortex (20). As such, proactive control may be particularly vulnerable to disruption, since it is resource demanding, and dependent upon precise dopamine-prefrontal interactions. Thus, we have suggested that populations characterized by disordered prefrontal and dopamine function (such as people with schizophrenia) may rely more heavily on reactive control, as it may be more robust in the face of such dysfunction (12).

There is convincing evidence for an association between impairments in DLPFC activity and deficits of proactive control in schizophrenia (2326), for both medicated (27) and unmedicated patients (17,28), as well as those at risk for the development of the disorder (29,30). These findings were strengthened by a meta-analysis of imaging studies of executive control and proactive control, which demonstrated reduced activity in DLPFC in schizophrenia (25). Further, growing evidence suggests a critical role for impaired connectivity between the DLPFC and other cognitive control related brain regions (3136), as well as for GABAergicly mediated (37) impairments in neural oscillations that may support representations in DLPFC (38,39). A relationship between dopaminergic function and DLPFC activity in schizophrenia (40), and a positive impact of dopamine enhancement on cognitive control in psychosis (41,42), have also been documented.

WORKING MEMORY IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

Although many studies have focused on understanding cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia, an even larger amount of research has been devoted to the cognitive neuroscience of WM (43), leading to an overwhelming amount of evidence in support of WM impairments in schizophrenia (e.g., 5,44).

WM traditionally refers to temporary storage and manipulating information “on-line”, typically in the service of some goal (45), but it is not a unitary construct. For example, Baddeley's model of WM suggests that it is comprised of a central executive resource system, two slave subsidiary systems (the phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketchpad), and an episodic buffer (45).

There is relatively little evidence that WM deficits can be unambiguously attributed to dysfunction in either the verbal or visual-spatial buffer systems, as individuals with schizophrenia exhibit abnormalities on WM tasks with many different material types, with relatively little evidence for selective deficits for one material type over another (5,44). This has led to the suggestion that WM deficits in schizophrenia might primarily reflect deficits in the central executive resource system, or the active maintenance and manipulation of information over time, an interpretation consistent with a central role for deficits in the proactive control of behavior.

However, there is debate about the degree to which WM impairments in schizophrenia really reflect deficits in the maintenance of information, versus the initial encoding or representation of information. For example, in one meta-analysis (44), the effect sizes of WM impairment across studies did not change as a function of the delay period used, implying that deficits in the initial generation of representations could impact the stability of such representations, and therefore the ability to accurately maintain them over time. Consistent with this hypothesis, studies examining encoding deficits have demonstrated that patients with schizophrenia exhibit deficits even in the absence of a delay (e.g., 46). At the same time, a number of studies have provided evidence for deficits in the ability to maintain information across time in schizophrenia, even after controlling for encoding differences (e.g., 46,47).

Prefrontal recruitment during working memory in schizophrenia

Similar to the literature on cognitive control and DLPFC function, there is a robust functional neuroimaging literature demonstrating the presence of abnormalities in prefrontal cortex recruitment associated with WM dysfunction in schizophrenia.

The majority of findings suggest that regions comprising the dorsal frontal-parietal network are affected in patients and may be contributing to WM abnormalities. Specifically, reductions in DLPFC (Brodmann's area 9/46) activation have been documented while patients perform WM tasks, suggesting that patients exhibit task-related “hypofrontality” (17,48). These findings have also been confirmed through quantitative meta-analytic studies (49,50). Such DLPFC deficits are present even in medication naïve individuals (17), and also occur, albeit to a lesser extent, in the first-degree relatives of individuals with schizophrenia (e.g., 29), suggesting a potential role as an endophenotypic marker.

Further, as with proactive cognitive control, there is evidence suggesting a key role for impaired connectivity between DLPFC and other WM related regions (e.g., parietal cortex, thalamus and striatum) in explaining WM impairments in schizophrenia (5155), as well as evidence for altered gamma and theta oscillatory activity in prefrontal regions associated with WM impairments in this disorder (e.g., 56-58).

The above discussion focused on decreased DLPFC activity associated with proactive control and WM. However, there have been discrepant findings with regard to whether individuals with schizophrenia show decreased or increased DLPFC activity during WM (5961). To explain this, some work has focused on the idea that WM capacity may be dependent on the level of recruitment of DLPFC, which is thought to operate according to an inverted U model (62). Such a model suggests that, with increasing WM demands, there is a concomitant parametric DLPFC signal increase. However, as WM load demands reach and exceed capacity limitations, DLPFC signals begin to drop, presumably due to information load exceeding available computational resources (62).

In line with this hypothesis, evidence suggests that patients with schizophrenia may exhibit a shifted inverted U function, such that capacity limitations are reached faster (i.e., with lower WM load levels), which may result in over- or under-recruitment when compared to healthy controls, depending on the level of WM load at which the groups are compared (6365). In other words, at low difficulty levels, patients may find performance more effortful and may have to recruit more prefrontal cortex resources than healthy controls to accomplish the same task, leading to findings of “hyperactivity” in prefrontal cortex. Consistent with this model, a meta-analysis (50) demonstrated that the magnitude of WM performance differences between patients and healthy controls was positively correlated with the magnitude of activation differences in dorsal-lateral prefrontal regions.

Another way to understand the mixed directions of WM related DLPFC activation in schizophrenia is to think about the temporal course of WM. If WM impairments in schizophrenia also reflect impairments in proactive control and DLPFC mediated function, then more specific predictions can be made about the timing of altered brain activation in WM tasks in schizophrenia. A failure to use proactive control would suggest that patients may show reduced activity during encoding and/or maintenance in lateral prefrontal regions. When a response is needed, they may need to try to retrieve the memoranda, potentially resulting in increased activation in brain regions associated with memory retrieval or response selection.

A number of studies that examined the time course of activity during WM trials have shown evidence for reduced activity during encoding and maintenance periods in DLPFC, as well as other WM related brain regions (12,6669). Further, studies that have specifically examined retrieval related activity have found evidence for increased activation among individuals with schizophrenia in either the same or different regions that showed reduced encoding/maintenance related activation (12,65). Thus, it may be useful in future research to more specifically tease apart the components of WM, as well as to examine the role of overall level of performance.

EPISODIC MEMORY IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

Similarly to WM, EM is not a unitary construct, but instead involves a number of different functional components and associated neural systems. The current literature on EM posits critical roles for both the medial temporal lobe, with a particular focus on the hippocampus, and prefrontal regions.

A common theme in theories regarding the role of the hippocampus in EM is the idea that it is critical for the rapid binding of novel configurations of information (e.g., 70,71). Consistent with such theories, numerous imaging studies have shown activation of the hippocampus during the encoding or retrieval of novel relational information (e.g., 72), and have shown that enhanced hippocampal/parahippocampal activity at the time of encoding predicts subsequent successful retrieval of that information (e.g., 73,74). Furthermore, work in amnestic patients demonstrates the importance of hippocampal structures in relational processing (e.g., 75).

More recent models of EM also suggest differential roles for hippocampal versus perirhinal regions of the medial temporal lobes in encoding of item versus relational memory (76). At the same time, there are also clearly important contributions from prefrontal regions. Damage to the prefrontal cortex can lead to EM deficits, among other cognitive impairments (e.g., 77,78). Further, activity during encoding in a number of prefrontal regions (e.g., Brodmann's areas 45 and 47) predicts subsequent memory when participants are asked to process verbal information using semantic elaboration strategies (79,80). In addition, there is work suggesting that DLPFC may contribute specifically to successful relational memory formation and retrieval (8183).

As discussed above, much of the EM literature has argued that the hippocampus is critical for binding information in memory. A number of studies have examined whether individuals with schizophrenia have binding deficits by exploring whether they are more impaired on memory for associative information (e.g., the association of previously unrelated words or items) as compared to memory for individual items. For example, Achim and Lepage (84) conducted a meta-analysis comparing performance on associative and item memory tests in individuals with schizophrenia, and concluded that there was evidence for a 20% greater impairment in associative as compared to item memory. However, a number of the associative memory studies included in this meta-analysis were tests of source memory (i.e., memory for the time and place in which an event occurred) rather than associations of novel pairs of items, and the human neuropsychological and imaging literatures suggests that PFC function may make an important contribution to source memory (85).

More recently, clinical researchers have begun to use tasks derived from the animal literature on hippocampal function, such as the transitive interference test, which measures the ability to learn the relationships among hierarchically arranged stimulus pairs, as well as the transitive patterning test, in which individuals have to learn about relationships between items for correct selection. Individuals with schizophrenia are impaired on critical conditions of these tasks requiring relational processing, but not on conditions that require simpler associative reinforcement mappings (8688), though not in all studies (89).

Other work has used eye-movement measures of relational memory, shown to be impaired in patients with hippocampal lesions (e.g., 90), to identify relational memory impairments in schizophrenia (e.g., 91,92). There is also work indicating impairments in both item and relational retrieval for information that was relationally encoded in schizophrenia (93). Still other work has provided evidence for greater deficits in recollection than familiarity in schizophrenia, which have also been interpreted as reflecting relational memory impairments (e.g., 94). It is certainly possible that this pattern of EM deficits in schizophrenia suggest hippocampally mediated impairments (95). However, as noted above, prefrontal structures also contribute to EM, and this may be particularly important for control functions, such as the ability to generate and apply effective memory strategies that help bind novel information into memory. Accordingly, a number of studies suggest that individuals with schizophrenia are impaired in their ability to generate effective mnemonic strategies, and that providing people with schizophrenia with effective memory strategies enhances EM function (for a review, see 96).

Importantly, a meta-analysis of brain activity alterations during EM performance in schizophrenia showed consistent evidence for reduced activation in both ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and DLPFC, but did not find consistent evidence for altered hippocampal activity (97). Recent work on relational memory encoding and retrieval has shown evidence for impaired DLPFC function associated with impaired relational memory function (98) and autobiographical memory (99) in schizophrenia, though other recent work has also implicated hippocampal function (100).

Taken together, these findings suggest potential roles for both hippocampal and prefrontal function in EM, and also suggest the possibility that cognitive control deficits may contribute to EM deficits in schizophrenia.

COGNITION ACROSS PSYCHOTIC DISORDERS

A key question is whether the nature and/or severity of cognitive impairment found in affective psychoses is similar or different to that found in schizophrenia. If qualitatively different, this would argue for a fundamentally different role for cognition in those psychoses. However, if the pattern or profile of cognitive impairment is similar, such a result would be consistent with the hypothesis that there are common dimensions of psychopathology across the affective and non-affective psychoses (101).

Both empirical and meta-analytic studies have fairly consistently shown that the degree of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is worse than in bipolar disorder (for a review, see 102) and psychotic major depression (103), with an effect size typically in the range of 0.3 to 0.5 (103107). The literature on the comparison of schizoaffective disorder to schizophrenia is mixed, with some studies finding very similar magnitudes of cognitive impairments in these two disorders (108110) and others reporting worse impairment in schizophrenia (107,111).

Despite the evidence of a larger magnitude of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia as compared to affective psychoses, the literature is fairly consistent in demonstrating that the profile of cognitive impairment is similar across schizophrenia and affective psychoses (112,113). In other words, the relative severity of impairments across different cognitive domains tends to be very similar in bipolar disorder, psychotic major depression and schizoaffective disorders as compared to schizophrenia (e.g., 104,105,109,114).

Perhaps one of the clearest examples of such a result was provided by Reichenberg et al (114). These researchers compared individuals with consensus research diagnoses of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, major depressive disorder with psychotic features, and bipolar disorder with psychotic features. The individuals with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder were overall more impaired than the individuals with psychotic affective disorders, and the prevalence of cognitive impairment was higher in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. However, the individuals within all four groups showed the same relative pattern of impairment across cognitive domains, with the greatest impairment in verbal memory, and the least impairment in visual processing and general verbal ability.

Depp et al (104) provided another compelling example in their study comparing patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and healthy controls. The profile of impairment was very similar in the two patient groups, with the most impairment in information processing speed and the least in crystallized IQ. In addition, there is evidence that the factor structure of cognition is very similar across schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (115,116). There are, however, some exceptions to these results, and some studies that have shown differences across psychotic disorders in the pattern of cognitive impairment (e.g., 111).

Thus, the bulk of the evidence suggests that all psychoses (affective and non-affective) are associated with some level of cognitive impairment. This impairment may be equally severe in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder, but less severe in individuals with psychotic bipolar disorder and psychotic major depression. However, the profile or pattern of cognitive impairment across affective psychoses is very similar to that seen in schizophrenia. This finding is consistent with the idea that there are common mechanisms that lead to cognitive dysfunction across psychotic disorders and with a growing emphasis on identifying core neural systems that contribute to impairments cutting across traditional diagnostic boundaries (117).

MEASURING COGNITION IN THE DSM-5

As reviewed above, there is ample evidence that a large percentage of individuals with schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders suffer from impairments in a range of cognitive domains (e.g., 114), and growing evidence that the level of cognitive impairment predicts functional abilities (social, occupational, living status) (e.g., 118,119-121).

Despite the importance of cognition in understanding function in schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders, the DSM-5 psychosis committee did not propose to include cognitive deficits as a criterion A symptom of schizophrenia or any other psychotic disorder. This is because cognition may not be useful as a differential diagnosis tool. As described above, the profile of cognitive impairments is similar across the non-affective and affective psychoses (103105,109,114,122), though the level of impairment may be greater in non-affective psychoses (103106). However, the wealth of data suggests that this separation is not sufficient to justify inclusion of cognition as a criterion A symptom of schizophrenia.

Nonetheless, it remains clear that cognitive function is important for understanding functional status in schizophrenia (121,123,124), as well as other psychotic disorders, including bipolar disorder (125128), and that cognitive deficits are not well treated by current antipsychotic medications (e.g., 129). Thus, the DSM-5 psychosis committee included a dimensional assessment of cognition, in order to highlight the potential need for additional treatments specifically targeting cognitive remediation in schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders (e.g., 130,131).

This assessment is a single dimension that collapses across all potential aspects of cognitive impairment. Ideally, one might have a separate dimensional rating of most major domains of cognitive impairment in psychosis separately, as it is possible to see dissociations across the level of impairment in one domain versus another within an individual (e.g., relatively impaired in WM, more so than in EM). However, this is not feasible from a practical standpoint, and the pretense of at least a single global dimension serves to highlight the need to attend to cognitive impairment when conceptualizing treatment and prognosis for an individual with psychosis.

Information about cognitive function is not something that is typically possible to assess as part of the standard psychiatric interview. Ideally, one would obtain a formal clinical neuropsychological assessment in individuals with psychosis to fully understand the nature and severity of their cognitive impairments. Such assessment may be of particular value early in the course of illness, when considering plans for further education and vocational functioning. If it is not possible to obtain a full neuropsychological evaluation, a number of studies have shown that several different brief assessment approaches provide clinically useful information concerning a patient's general level of cognitive impairment (7,8,132136). However, brief screening instruments developed for use in the detection of frank dementia, such as the Mini-Mental Status Exam, are not sensitive to the types of impairments that are typically observed in patients with schizophrenia and therefore their use is discouraged in this context.

The growing research on other methods for assessing cognitive function (e.g., self-report, clinician interview) suggests that they have limited correlation with objective measures of cognitive performance (137), though they may still have utility in predicting functional status (137144). If a formal assessment of cognition is not possible, it is still important for the clinician to use the best available information to make a judgment about the individual's cognitive function, including the clinician's interactions with the patient and/or reports of family members or clinical staff. However, it is likely that, without objective assessments, such ratings may have less than optimal reliability and validity, though hopefully they will still serve to highlight the critical need for treatments that address this debilitating aspect of psychotic disorders.

CONCLUSIONS

Individuals with schizophrenia show significant deficits in a number of different cognitive domains, including cognitive control, WM and EM, and the pattern of deficits is similar to those observed in affective psychotic disorders. Given the emerging re-conceptualization of cognition in schizophrenia (or all psychotic disorders) as reflecting a core neurobiological abnormality, we suggest that an impairment in proactive control can influence performance in a wide variety of cognitive domains, and therefore may represent a common mechanism contributing to these deficits.

Further, we suggest that, at the neural level, a common denominator to such deficits may be an impaired function of DLPFC, its connectivity with other brain regions, and the neurotransmitter systems that support precise updating and maintenance of goal representations which enable proactive control.

We do not mean to imply that all aspects of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia can be fully explained by these mechanisms. Schizophrenia is a complex disorder, and it is clear that it would be an oversimplification to suggest that a single mechanism could explain the diversity of impairments found in this illness. Nonetheless, we think it important to raise the possibility that there is a common core mechanism that can help explain at least a subset of impairments, and which may serve as a target for therapeutic interventions that could broadly enhance cognitive function and outcome in this illness.

Acknowledgments

Both authors contributed equally to the work presented here.

References

  • 1.Nuechterlein KH, Subotnik KL, Green MF, et al. Neurocognitive predictors of work outcome in recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(Suppl. 2):S33–40. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fett AK, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, et al. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:573–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Tolman AW, Kurtz MM. Neurocognitive predictors of objective and subjective quality of life in individuals with schizophrenia: a meta-analytic investigation. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:304–15. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mesholam-Gately RI, Giuliano AJ, Goff KP, et al. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:315–36. doi: 10.1037/a0014708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Forbes NF, Carrick LA, McIntosh AM, et al. Working memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2009;39:889–905. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dickinson D, Iannone VN, Wilk CM, et al. General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:826–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dickinson D, Ragland JD, Gold JM, et al. General and specific cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: Goliath defeats David? Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:823–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dickinson D, Ramsey ME, Gold JM. Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:532–42. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gold JM, Dickinson D. "Generalized cognitive deficit" in schizophrenia: overused or underappreciated? Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:263–5. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dickinson D, Harvey PD. Systemic hypotheses for generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a new take on an old problem. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:403–14. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Barch DM, Braver TS, Carter CS, et al. CNTRICS final task selection: executive control. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:115–35. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Edwards BG, Barch DM, Braver TS. Improving prefrontal cortex function in schizophrenia through focused training of cognitive control. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:32. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lesh TA, Niendam TA, Minzenberg MJ, et al. Cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia: mechanisms and meaning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:316–38. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Braver TS, Gray JR, Burgess GC. Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In: Conway RA, Jarrold C, Kane MJ, et al., editors. Variation in working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 76–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Braver TS, Paxton JL, Locke HS, et al. Flexible neural mechanisms of cognitive control within human prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:7351–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808187106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Haddon JE, Killcross S. Contextual control of choice performance: behavioral, neurobiological, and neurochemical influences. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2007;1104:250–69. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Barch DM, Carter CS, Braver TS, et al. Selective deficits in prefrontal cortex regions in medication naive schizophrenia patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;50:280–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Braver TS, Barch DM, Cohen JD. Cognition and control in schizophrenia: a computational model of dopamine and prefrontal function. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:312–28. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00116-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cohen JD, Barch DM, Carter C, et al. Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks. J Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108:120–33. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.1.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Braver TS. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:106–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cole MW, Reynolds JR, Power JD, et al. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:1348–55. doi: 10.1038/nn.3470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cole MW, Yarkoni T, Repovs G, et al. Global connectivity of prefrontal cortex predicts cognitive control and intelligence. J Neurosci. 2012;32:8988–99. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0536-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lesh TA, Westphal AJ, Niendam TA, et al. Proactive and reactive cognitive control and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia. Neuroimage Clin. 2013;2:590–9. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Eich TS, Nee DE, Insel C, et al. Neural correlates of impaired cognitive control over working memory in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.032. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Minzenberg MJ, Laird AR, Thelen S, et al. Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:811–22. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Barbalat G, Chambon V, Franck N, et al. Organization of cognitive control within the lateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:377–86. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Holmes AJ, MacDonald A, 3rd, Carter CS, et al. Prefrontal functioning during context processing in schizophrenia and major depression: an event-related fMRI study. Schizophr Res. 2005;76:199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.MacDonald A, Carter CS, Kerns JG, et al. Specificity of prefrontal dysfunction and context processing deficits to schizophrenia in a never medicated first-episode psychotic sample. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:475–84. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.MacDonald AW, 3rd, Thermenos HW, Barch DM, et al. Imaging genetic liability to schizophrenia: systematic review of fMRI studies of patients' nonpsychotic relatives. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:1142–62. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Fusar-Poli P, Perez J, Broome M, et al. Neurofunctional correlates of vulnerability to psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:465–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kyriakopoulos M, Dima D, Roiser JP, et al. Abnormal functional activation and connectivity in the working memory network in early-onset schizophrenia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:911–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Baker JT, Holmes AJ, Masters GA, et al. Disruption of cortical association networks in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71:109–18. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Yoon JH, Minzenberg MJ, Ursu S, et al. Association of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction with disrupted coordinated brain activity in schizophrenia: relationship with impaired cognition, behavioral disorganization, and global function. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1006–14. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07060945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fornito A, Yoon J, Zalesky A, et al. General and specific functional connectivity disturbances in first-episode schizophrenia during cognitive control performance. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cole MW, Anticevic A, Repovs G, et al. Variable global dysconnectivity and individual differences in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Repovs G, Csernansky JG, Barch DM. Brain network connectivity in individuals with schizophrenia and their siblings. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:967–73. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lewis DA, Cho RY, Carter CS, et al. Subunit-selective modulation of GABA type A receptor neurotransmission and cognition in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1585–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Minzenberg MJ, Firl AJ, Yoon JH, et al. Gamma oscillatory power is impaired during cognitive control independent of medication status in first-episode schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:2590–9. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cho RY, Konecky RO, Carter CS. Impairments in frontal cortical gamma synchrony and cognitive control in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:19878–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609440103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Miletich RS, Kohn PD, et al. Reduced prefrontal activity predicts exaggerated striatal dopaminergic function in schizophrenia. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:267–71. doi: 10.1038/nn804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Barch DM, Carter CS. Amphetamine improves cognitive function in medicated individuals with schizophrenia and in healthy volunteers. Schizophr Res. 2005;77:43–58. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.McClure MM, Harvey PD, Goodman M, et al. Pergolide treatment of cognitive deficits associated with schizotypal personality disorder: continued evidence of the importance of the dopamine system in the schizophrenia spectrum. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1356–62. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14:477–85. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lee J, Park S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:599–611. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Baddeley AD. The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:417–23. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tek C, Gold JM, Blaxton T, et al. Visual perceptual and working memory impairments in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;56:146–53. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.2.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Badcock JC, Badcock DR, Read C, et al. Examining encoding imprecision in spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;100:144–52. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Callicott JH, Ramsey NF, Tallent K, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain mapping in psychiatry: methodological issues illustrated in a study of working memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:186–96. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00096-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Glahn DC, Ragland JD, Abramoff A, et al. Beyond hypofrontality: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of working memory in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:60–9. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Van Snellenberg JX, Torres IJ, Thornton AE. Functional neuroimaging of working memory in schizophrenia: task performance as a moderating variable. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:497–510. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.5.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Barch DM, Csernansky JG. Abnormal parietal cortex activation during working memory in schizophrenia: verbal phonological coding disturbances versus domain-general executive dysfunction. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:1090–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.7.1090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Karlsgodt KH, van Erp TG, Poldrack RA, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of the superior longitudinal fasciculus and working memory in recent-onset schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:512–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Henseler I, Falkai P, Gruber O. Disturbed functional connectivity within brain networks subserving domain-specific subcomponents of working memory in schizophrenia: relation to performance and clinical symptoms. J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44:364–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Quide Y, Morris RW, Shepherd AM, et al. Task-related fronto-striatal functional connectivity during working memory performance in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013;150:468–75. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Unschuld PG, Buchholz AS, Varvaris M, et al. Prefrontal brain network connectivity indicates degree of both schizophrenia risk and cognitive dysfunction. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:653–64. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Basar-Eroglu C, Brand A, Hildebrandt H, et al. Working memory related gamma oscillations in schizophrenia patients. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;64:39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Barr MS, Farzan F, Tran LC, et al. Evidence for excessive frontal evoked gamma oscillatory activity in schizophrenia during working memory. Schizophr Res. 2010;121:146–52. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.05.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Barr MS, Radhu N, Guglietti CL, et al. Age-related differences in working memory evoked gamma oscillations. Brain Res. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.05.043. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Verchinski BA, et al. Complexity of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia: more than up or down. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:2209–15. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Walton E, Geisler D, Lee PH, et al. Prefrontal inefficiency is associated with polygenic risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt174. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Brandt CL, Eichele T, Melle I, et al. Working memory networks and activation patterns in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: comparison with healthy controls. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;204:290–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.129254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Goldman-Rakic PS, Muly EC, Williams GV. D1 receptors in prefrontal cells and circuits. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000;31:295–301. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00045-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Deserno L, Sterzer P, Wustenberg T, et al. Reduced prefrontal-parietal effective connectivity and working memory deficits in schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2012;32:12–20. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3405-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Metzak PD, Riley JD, Wang L, et al. Decreased efficiency of task-positive and task-negative networks during working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:803–13. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Johnson MR, Morris NA, Astur RS, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of working memory abnormalities in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Anticevic A, Repovs G, Barch DM. Working memory encoding and maintenance deficits in schizophrenia: neural evidence for activation and deactivation abnormalities. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39:168–78. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Driesen NR, Leung HC, Calhoun VD, et al. Impairment of working memory maintenance and response in schizophrenia: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:1026–34. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Schlosser RG, Koch K, Wagner G, et al. Inefficient executive cognitive control in schizophrenia is preceded by altered functional activation during information encoding: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:336–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bittner RA, Linden DE, Roebroeck A, et al. The when and where of working memory dysfunction in early-onset schizophrenia - a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Cereb Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu050. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Cohen NJ, Eichenbaum H. From conditioning to conscious recollection. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Squire LR. Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Wendelken C, Bunge SA. Transitive inference: distinct contributions of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22:837–47. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Brewer J, Zhao ZH, Gabrieli JDE. Parahippocampal and frontal responses to single events predict whether those events are remembered or forgotten. Science. 1998;281:1185–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wagner AD, Schacter D, Rotte M, et al. Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science. 1998;281:1188–91. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5380.1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Bowles B, Crupi C, Pigott S, et al. Double dissociation of selective recollection and familiarity impairments following two different surgical treatments for temporal-lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:2640–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Davachi L. Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:693–700. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Janowsky JS, Shimamura AP, Kritchevsky M, et al. Cognitive impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. Behav Neurosci. 1989;103:548–60. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.3.548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Duarte A, Ranganath C, Knight RT. Effects of unilateral prefrontal lesions on familiarity, recollection, and source memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:8333–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1392-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Spaniol J, Davidson PS, Kim AS, et al. Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1765–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Kim H. Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: a meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies. NeuroImage. 2011;54:2446–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Blumenfeld RS, Parks CM, Yonelinas AP, et al. Putting the pieces together: the role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in relational memory encoding. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:257–65. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Blumenfeld RS, Ranganath C. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. J Neurosci. 2006;26:916–25. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2353-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Blumenfeld RS, Ranganath C. Prefrontal cortex and long-term memory encoding: an integrative review of findings from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Neuroscientist. 2007;13:280–91. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Achim AM, Lepage M. Is associative recognition more impaired than item recognition memory in schizophrenia? A meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. 2003;53:121–4. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00092-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Mitchell KJ, Johnson MK. Source monitoring 15 years later: what have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychol Bull. 2009;135:638–77. doi: 10.1037/a0015849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Armstrong K, Kose S, Williams L, et al. Impaired associative inference in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:622–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Armstrong K, Williams LE, Heckers S. Revised associative inference paradigm confirms relational memory impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. 2012;26:451–8. doi: 10.1037/a0028667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Heckers S, Zalesak M, Weiss AP, et al. Hippocampal activation during transitive inference in humans. Hippocampus. 2004;14:153–62. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Williams LE, Avery SN, Woolard AA, et al. Intact relational memory and normal hippocampal structure in the early stage of psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:105–13. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Hannula DE, Ranganath C. The eyes have it: hippocampal activity predicts expression of memory in eye movements. Neuron. 2009;63:592–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Hannula DE, Ranganath C, Ramsay IS, et al. Use of eye movement monitoring to examine item and relational memory in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:610–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Williams LE, Must A, Avery S, et al. Eye-movement behavior reveals relational memory impairment in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:617–24. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.05.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Ragland JD, Ranganath C, Barch DM, et al. Relational and Item-Specific Encoding (RISE): task development and psychometric characteristics. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:114–24. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.van Erp TG, Lesh TA, Knowlton BJ, et al. Remember and know judgments during recognition in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008;100:181–90. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Heckers S, Konradi C. Hippocampal pathology in schizophrenia. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:529–53. doi: 10.1007/7854_2010_43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Barch DM. The cognitive neuroscience of schizophrenia. In: Cannon T, Mineka S, editors. Annual review of clinical psychology. Washington: American Psychological Association; 2005. pp. 321–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Ragland JD, Laird AR, Ranganath C, et al. Prefrontal activation deficits during episodic memory in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:863–74. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08091307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Ragland JD, Blumenfeld RS, Ramsay IS, et al. Neural correlates of relational and item-specific encoding during working and long-term memory in schizophrenia. NeuroImage. 2012;59:1719–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Cuervo-Lombard C, Lemogne C, Gierski F, et al. Neural basis of autobiographical memory retrieval in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;201:473–80. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.099820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Hanlon FM, Houck JM, Pyeatt CJ, et al. Bilateral hippocampal dysfunction in schizophrenia. NeuroImage. 2011;58:1158–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Craddock N, O'Donovan MC, Owen MJ. Psychosis genetics: modeling the relationship between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and mixed (or "schizoaffective") psychoses. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:482–90. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Lewandowski KE, Cohen BM, Ongur D. Evolution of neuropsychological dysfunction during the course of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychol Med. 2011;41:225–41. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Hill SK, Keshavan MS, Thase ME, et al. Neuropsychological dysfunction in antipsychotic-naive first-episode unipolar psychotic depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:996–1003. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Depp CA, Moore DJ, Sitzer D, et al. Neurocognitive impairment in middle-aged and older adults with bipolar disorder: comparison to schizophrenia and normal comparison subjects. J Affect Disord. 2007;101:201–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.11.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Schretlen DJ, Cascella NG, Meyer SM, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:179–86. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Krabbendam L, Arts B, van Os J, et al. Cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a quantitative review. Schizophr Res. 2005;80:137–49. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Hill SK, Reilly JL, Keefe RS, et al. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder: findings from the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) study. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:1275–84. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12101298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Gooding DC, Tallent KA. Spatial working memory performance in patients with schizoaffective psychosis versus schizophrenia: a tale of two disorders? Schizophr Res. 2002;53:209–18. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00258-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Smith MJ, Barch DM, Csernansky JG. Bridging the gap between schizophrenia and psychotic mood disorders: relating neurocognitive deficits to psychopathology. Schizophr Res. 2009;107:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Owoso A, Carter CS, Gold JM, et al. Cognition in schizophrenia and schizo-affective disorder: impairments that are more similar than different. Psychol Med. 2013;43:2535–45. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Heinrichs RW, Ammari N, McDermid Vaz S, et al. Are schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder neuropsychologically distinguishable? Schizophr Res. 2008;99:149–54. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Reilly JL, Sweeney JA. Generalized and specific neurocognitive deficits in psychotic disorders: utility for evaluating pharmacological treatment effects and as intermediate phenotypes for gene discovery. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:516–22. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Tamminga CA, Pearlson G, Keshavan M, et al. Bipolar and schizophrenia network for intermediate phenotypes: outcomes across the psychosis continuum. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(Suppl. 2):S131–7. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Reichenberg A, Harvey PD, Bowie CR, et al. Neuropsychological function and dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:1022–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Czobor P, Jaeger J, Berns SM, et al. Neuropsychological symptom dimensions in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9:71–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00428.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Schretlen DJ, Pena J, Aretouli E, et al. Confirmatory factor analysis reveals a latent cognitive structure common to bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and normal controls. Bipolar Disord. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12075. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, et al. Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167:748–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Heinrichs RW, Goldberg JO, Miles AA, et al. Predictors of medication competence in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Res. 2008;157:47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Cervellione KL, Burdick KE, Cottone JG, et al. Neurocognitive deficits in adolescents with schizophrenia: longitudinal stability and predictive utility for short-term functional outcome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:867–78. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318054678d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.McClure MM, Bowie CR, Patterson TL, et al. Correlations of functional capacity and neuropsychological performance in older patients with schizophrenia: evidence for specificity of relationships? Schizophr Res. 2007;89:330–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Green MF, Kern RS, Heaton RK. Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Bowie CR, Reichenberg A, McClure MM, et al. Age-associated differences in cognitive performance in older community dwelling schizophrenia patients: differential sensitivity of clinical neuropsychological and experimental information processing tests. Schizophr Res. 2008;106:50–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Bowie CR, Leung WW, Reichenberg A, et al. Predicting schizophrenia patients' real-world behavior with specific neuropsychological and functional capacity measures. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:505–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, et al. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the "right stuff"? Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Martinez-Aran A, Vieta E, Reinares M, et al. Cognitive function across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:262–70. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Tabares-Seisdedos R, Balanza-Martinez V, Sanchez-Moreno J, et al. Neurocognitive and clinical predictors of functional outcome in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder at one-year follow-up. J Affect Disord. 2008;109:286–99. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.12.234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Jaeger J, Berns S, Loftus S, et al. Neurocognitive test performance predicts functional recovery from acute exacerbation leading to hospitalization in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2007;9:93–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00427.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Gruber SA, Rosso IM, Yurgelun-Todd D. Neuropsychological performance predicts clinical recovery in bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 2008;105:253–60. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Davis SM, et al. Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic medications in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE Trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:633–47. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Marder SR, Fenton W. Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia: NIMH MATRICS initiative to support the development of agents for improving cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:5–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Marder SR. Drug initiatives to improve cognitive function. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67(Suppl. 9):31–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Hurford IM, Marder SR, Keefe RS, et al. A brief cognitive assessment tool for schizophrenia: construction of a tool for clinicians. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:538–45. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Velligan DI, DiCocco M, Bow-Thomas CC, et al. A brief cognitive assessment for use with schizophrenia patients in community clinics. Schizophr Res. 2004;71:273–83. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Wilk CM, Gold JM, Humber K, et al. Brief cognitive assessment in schizophrenia: normative data for the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status. Schizophr Res. 2004;70:175–86. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Gold JM, Queern C, Iannone VN, et al. Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status as a screening test in schizophrenia I: sensitivity, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:1944–50. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.12.1944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Keefe RS, Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, et al. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:283–97. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Kern RS, et al. Functional co-primary measures for clinical trials in schizophrenia: results from the MATRICS Psychometric and Standardization Study. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:221–8. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Ventura J, Reise SP, Keefe RS, et al. The Cognitive Assessment Interview (CAI): development and validation of an empirically derived, brief interview-based measure of cognition. Schizophr Res. 2010;121:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Chia MY, Chan WY, Chua KY, et al. The Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale: validation of an interview-based assessment of cognitive functioning in Asian patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178:33–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Harvey PD, Keefe RS, Patterson TL, et al. Abbreviated neuropsychological assessment in schizophrenia: prediction of different aspects of outcome. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009;31:462–71. doi: 10.1080/13803390802251386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Keefe RS, Harvey PD, Goldberg TE, et al. Norms and standardization of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) Schizophr Res. 2008;102:108–15. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Hill SK, Sweeney JA, Hamer RM, et al. Efficiency of the CATIE and BACS neuropsychological batteries in assessing cognitive effects of antipsychotic treatments in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008;14:209–21. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Kaneda Y, Sumiyoshi T, Keefe R, et al. Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: validation of the Japanese version. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007;61:602–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01725.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Bralet MC, Falissard B, Neveu X, et al. Validation of the French version of the BACS (the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia) among 50 French schizophrenic patients. Eur Psychiatry. 2007;22:365–70. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from World Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of The World Psychiatric Association

RESOURCES