Abstract
Whether upper limb sensorimotor control is affected in schizophrenia and how underlying pathological mechanisms may potentially intervene in these deficits is still being debated. We tested voluntary force control in schizophrenia patients and used a computational model in order to elucidate potential cerebral mechanisms underlying sensorimotor deficits in schizophrenia. A visuomotor grip force-tracking task was performed by 17 medicated and 6 non-medicated patients with schizophrenia (DSM-IV) and by 15 healthy controls. Target forces in the ramp-hold-and-release paradigm were set to 5N and to 10% maximal voluntary grip force. Force trajectory was analyzed by performance measures and Principal Component Analysis (PCA). A computational model incorporating neural control signals was used to replicate the empirically observed motor behavior and to explore underlying neural mechanisms. Grip task performance was significantly lower in medicated and non-medicated schizophrenia patients compared to controls. Three behavioral variables were significantly higher in both patient groups: tracking error (by 50%), coefficient of variation of force (by 57%) and duration of force release (up by 37%). Behavioral performance did not differ between patient groups. Computational simulation successfully replicated these findings and predicted that decreased motor inhibition, together with an increased signal-dependent motor noise, are sufficient to explain the observed motor deficits in patients. PCA also suggested altered motor inhibition as a key factor differentiating patients from control subjects: the principal component representing inhibition correlated with clinical severity. These findings show that schizophrenia affects voluntary sensorimotor control of the hand independent of medication, and suggest that reduced motor inhibition and increased signal-dependent motor noise likely reflect key pathological mechanisms of the sensorimotor deficit.
Introduction
Since the very first description of schizophrenia, motor deficits had been noted [1], [2], in particular in gait and voluntary upper limb movements, but even today, there is no consensus on the form, the specificity and the cause of motor disturbances in schizophrenia (reviews: [3]–[6]). For skilled upper limb movements, anticipatory planning of action sequences was incriminated [7]–[9], i.e. the cognitive aspects of actions [10], [11], rather than motor control per se (but see [12]).
We investigated visuomotor grip force control, a key element of manual dexterity and a quintessential property of the sensorimotor system. It was reported that the grip/load force relation during object grasp was not affected in schizophrenia patients [7] or, if so, was in part a side effect of anti-psychotic medication [13]. Nonetheless, clinical tests and scales indicate that manual control is affected in schizophrenia [14]–[17]. Motor signs (assessed by neurological soft signs, NSS) have often been found in patients ([18]–[21]. There is little doubt that manual control is affected on a qualitative level, but it would be advantageous if quantifiable evidence of such a deficit were available.
Furthermore, it has been put forward that cortical inhibition is deficient in schizophrenia (review: [22]), including inhibition in the upper limb motor system [23]. For motor control, inhibition of muscle contraction is functionally as important as muscular excitation: it has been shown that cortical inhibition in healthy subjects, probed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) measuring short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI), is regulated during grip force control, and varies inversely with force [24]–[25]. On the other hand it is less clear how inhibition might be affected in schizophrenia. It has been shown that patients with schizophrenia have morphological changes in cortical inhibitory interneurons mediated by γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) [26]. Several studies have found reduced motor cortical SICI in medicated and non-medicated patients with schizophrenia [23], [27], [28], consistent with this finding.
Our goal was twofold: (i) to provide a systematic and quantitative description of a potential upper limb motor deficit in schizophrenia, (ii) to establish whether motor inhibition, probed behaviorally and computationally, is affected in schizophrenia. To this aim we used a visuomotor paradigm involving increase, maintenance and decrease of grip force, a task where modulation of motor inhibition is critical [24]. Furthermore, using a computational approach by modeling sensorimotor integration of visual, tactile and inhibitory signals for the control of force, we explored whether deficient motor inhibition might (on its own or not) explain the empirically observed deficits. We present a model based on the key assumption that only the signal gains differ between patients and controls, and no other sensorimotor mechanism.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Patients and control subjects were recruited from SHU-Sainte-Anne and tested in the Centre d'Evaluation et de Recherche Clinique (CERC). Three groups were included: (i) Seventeen stabilized patients (5 females, 12 males, mean age 30.0±8.4 (SD) years) that met DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia. All were on stable atypical anti-psychotic medication for >3 months prior to the study. Concomitant medication: only stable doses of antidepressants were permitted. This group is subsequently referred to as ‘medicated patients’. (ii) Six non-medicated patients (for >6 months prior to the study) that also met DSM-IV-TR criteria for schizophrenia (2 females, 4 males, mean age 32.3±6.8 years). This group is subsequently referred to as ‘non-medicated patients’. (iii) Fifteen healthy control subjects (7 females, 8 males among the hospital staff, mean age 29.5±10.5 years). Control subjects, screened by a standardized interview, had no previous history of neurological or psychiatric illness, no familial psychiatric history and no particular dexterous skills. Demographic and clinical details for each patient and group are given in Table 1. Subjects provided informed and written consent. The study received approval by the Paris-Cochin Ethical committee and complied with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Table 1. Clinical and demographic details.
Medicated patients | Gender | Age (years) | Treatment duration (years) | Treatment (CE mg/day) | PANSS total | PANSS pos score | PANSS neg score | Moberg left/right (s) | MVC grip force left/right (kg) |
1 | M | 33 | 4 | 133 | 63 | 7 | 22 | 13/14 | 42/45 |
2 | F | 45 | 10 | 1875 | 47 | 7 | 19 | 20/21 | 26/28 |
3 | F | 21 | 0.2 | 150 | 50 | 7 | 13 | 17/16 | 28/31 |
4 | M | 20 | 0.3 | 3750 | 48 | 8 | 12 | 18/18 | 41/43 |
5 | M | 39 | 2 | 500 | 41 | 7 | 12 | 13/15 | 44/44 |
6 | F | 26 | 1 | 470 | 49 | 9 | 10 | 18/16 | 29/32 |
7 | M | 40 | 3 | 400 | 55 | 7 | 20 | 18/18 | 46/43 |
8 | M | 37 | 5 | 200 | 42 | 7 | 12 | 17/17 | 55/54 |
9 | F | 24 | 0.2 | 400 | 83 | 9 | 26 | 12/12 | 22/24 |
10 | M | 19 | 0.2 | 300 | 50 | 7 | 22 | 15/19 | 39/39 |
11 | M | 44 | 3 | 583 | 74 | 17 | 23 | 20/23 | 50/49 |
12 | M | 30 | 10 | 330 | 42 | 7 | 14 | 18/21 | 40/38 |
13 | M | 33 | 0.5 | 1000 | 63 | 8 | 21 | 22/17 | 36/34 |
14 | M | 35 | 0.2 | 133 | 42 | 9 | 10 | 12/13 | 44/43 |
15 | F | 24 | 1 | 400 | 43 | 12 | 12 | 17/17 | 35/33 |
16 | M | 23 | 1 | 1650 | 45 | 14 | 11 | 15/15 | 48/57 |
17 | M | 34 | 4 | 200 | 71 | 10 | 21 | 19/15 | 25/25 |
Mean SD | 12M,5F | 31.0±8.4 | 3.2±3.4 | 709±867 | 53.4±12.8 | 8.9±2.9 | 16.5±5.4 | 16.7/16.9±3.0/2.9 | 38.2/38.9±9.6/9.6 |
Non-medicated patients | |||||||||
1 | M | 36 | na | na | 53 | 13 | 12 | 14/16 | 40/48 |
2 | F | 42 | na | na | 67 | 14 | 13 | 12/12 | 27/27 |
3 | M | 30 | na | na | 45 | 10 | 9 | 12/12 | 45/49 |
4 | M | 25 | na | * | 47 | 13 | 9 | 19/15 | 32/39 |
5 | F | 25 | na | na | 68 | 14 | 13 | 18/17 | 34/29 |
6 | M | 36 | na | na | 63 | 7 | 15 | 13/13 | 51/52 |
Mean SD | 4M, 2F | 32.3±6.8 | na | na | 57.2±10.2 | 11.8±2.8 | 11.8±2.4 | 14.7/14.2±3.1/2.1 | 38.2/40.7±8.9/10.7 |
Control subjects | |||||||||
Mean SD | 8M,7F | 29.5±10.5 | na | na | na | na | na | 12.9/12.6±2.0/2.0 | 40.1/41.7±11.2/10.9 |
Gender, age, duration of ongoing treatment, dose of antipsychotic treatment in chlorpromazine equivalent (CE), PANSS [32], PANSS positive score, PANSS negative score, Moberg pick up test rating (left and right hand), MVC grip force in kg (left and right hand) assessed with the Jamar hand dynamometer. Note that 40 kg corresponds to 392 N. * under antidepressants.
Visuomotor force-tracking task
A visuomotor power grip force-tracking task previously described [29] was used to assess the accuracy of force control (Fig. 1A). Grip force was recorded at 1 kHz using strain gauge force sensors linked to a CED 1401 running Spike2. The task consisted of a series of visually displayed ramp-hold-and-release target force trajectories to be followed as closely as possible with a cursor (moving vertically as a linear function of grip force), while the target force trajectory scrolled continuously over the screen from right to left. Upcoming force was thus predictable. For both low and high force conditions, the pre-ramp (inter-trial) period lasted 3 s, the ramp period 2 s, and the hold period 4 s, after which the target force dropped instantaneously to baseline (0N). Force-tracking was performed once with the right and once with the left hand (pseudo-randomized across subjects). While tracking with one hand, the other (resting) hand remained passive but still gripped a manipulandum, so that unwanted motor overflow could be quantified. Each task condition consisted of 16 trials. Condition_1: low absolute force level (5N). Condition_2: higher relative force level (10% maximal voluntary grip force, MVC). Trials were grouped by force level in blocks of four trials, and four blocks were performed at each force level (total of 32 trials). Subjects were instructed to minimize the distance (error) between the applied and the target force and to release force immediately at the end of the hold phase. All subjects were familiarized with the task before testing.
The following single-trial performance measures were computed [29] based on the 100 Hz down-sampled force signal:
Relative error (dimensionless). Total error over time (area) between the applied force and the target force trajectory normalized to the target force level. Note: an identical total error at two different force levels will lead, due to normalization, to a smaller relative error at the higher level (c.f. [30]).
Variability. Coefficient of variation (CV) of force (i.e., SD/mean). The CV expresses variability relative to the mean force level.
Release duration (ms). Time taken to abruptly reduce the grip force from 75% to 25% of the target force.
Release onset (ms). Time of initial force reduction, quantified as the time when the slope (dF/dt) first crossed a negative threshold, i.e., dF/dt<-6N/s.
Force onset (ms). The time when the slope (dF/dt) of the applied force crossed a positive threshold, i.e., dF/dt>0.2*max (dF/dt). Expressed with respect to target ramp onset.
Motor overflow (N). Mean force of the resting hand for three separate 1 s periods (during baseline, ramp and hold) relative to those of the tracking hand.
Data analysis and statistics
Force data were analyzed using Matlab v7 (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Statistical analysis was performed using Statistica 10 (StatSoft, Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA). Relative error was analyzed using a general linear model repeated measures ANOVA with one GROUP factor (medicated/non-medicated/controls) and three within-subject factors: PHASE (ramp/hold), HAND (dominant/non-dominant) and FORCE (5N/10%). CV, release duration, and release onset were analyzed in the same way. Post-hoc Fisher LSD tests were applied for significant differences. The level of significance was set to p≤0.05.
Principal Component Analysis (PCA) may identify underlying control strategies, e.g., for human grasp kinematics [31]. We performed a PCA on the mean force with the aim to split the time-varying force profile into PCs and to check whether these were different for the three groups. This PCA was performed across all subjects in order to compare groups with respect to their common PCs (Fig. 2). We used the nonlinear iterative partial least squares (NIPALS) method on the average force trace for each subject. The PCA data consisted of a 900×38 matrix (900 force samples representing 9 s of the trial- and condition-averaged force trace, times 38 subjects). PC factor scores were compared in an ANOVA with one GROUP factor. In addition, a separate PCA for the control subjects (900×15) and for the patients (900×23) was performed for a more qualitative comparison between groups (Fig. 3). We use the term ‘qualitative’ since there is no mathematical guarantee that the resulting PCs in each group are co-linear and ordered identically (in terms of explained variance) due to the difference in the underlying covariance matrix of each group.
Relations between force tracking variables were investigated using Pearson's correlation, and relations between force tracking variables and symptom severity (PANSS score [32]) were investigated using Spearman rank correlations (RS). A Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was applied.
Computational model of sensorimotor integration
A simple computational model of sensorimotor integration was implemented under Simulink (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). The goal was to replicate empirical (behavioral) differences between the patient and the control group and to explain these differences in terms of the underlying neural control signals used in the model. This model (Fig. 4) considered three types of time-varying input signals to be integrated and to form a motor command: (i) visual information on the ramp-hold-and-release target force trajectory, (ii) inhibition modulated as a function of target force, and (iii) tactile/proprioceptive feedback. Each input signal had its own gain: V_Gain, I_Gain and TP_Gain, respectively. This motor command then entered a negative feedback-loop where the grip force (the model output) was dynamically regulated as a function of the error between the motor command and the actual grip force. Signal-dependent noise was added to the grip force. The following assumptions have been implemented:
Neural input signals
Visual input: a time-varying form according to the empirically used target force trajectory.
Inhibitory input: a time-varying form shown in Fig. 4 and consistent with empirical observations [24], [25]. Inhibition decreases with increasing force [24], remains constant during hold, and there is a phasic increases at the moment of force release [25]. White noise was added to the visual and inhibitory signals.
Tactile/proprioceptive input: a feedback (downscaled copy) of the time-varying grip force, consistent with tactile and proprioceptive afferents [33], as well as with cells in somatosensory cortex [34], that have been shown to increase their firing with force.
Gains on input signals: visual gain (V_Gain), inhibitory gain (I_Gain) and gain of tactile/proprioceptive input (TP_Gain). These add up to unity.
Sensorimotor integration
Motor command: the sum of the visual, inhibitory and tactile/proprioceptive input. For the motor command to scale as a function of the target force, the sum of the gains of the three input signals needs to add up to unity.
Output signal
Grip force: is controlled by a negative feedback loop that integrates the error, i.e. the difference between actual grip force and the motor command. Linearly increasing signal-dependent noise (SDN) was added to the grip force (in line with [35]).
Simulation
General assumption: patients differ from control subjects in terms of altered gains, i.e. there is no alteration in the mechanism of sensorimotor integration or in the temporal characteristics of the input signals. If so, altered gains should be sufficient to simulate the empirically observed behavioral differences between patients and controls.
Simulation procedure: twenty runs (each trial with different, pseudo-randomized seeds for the noise) were computed for each condition. Performance measures were calculated per trial and averaged across trials for a given condition (as for the empirical data).
Assumptions on gains: V_Gain was arbitrarily set to 0.5 and constant for all conditions. For control subjects I_Gain was set to 0.2, corresponding to 20% of inhibitory cortical cells and synapses [36], and therefore TP_Gain was 0.3.
Tunable parameters: SDN_Gain for control subjects. SDN_Gain and I_Gain (TP_Gain) for patients. All other parameters remained constant for all conditions.
Results
Clinical and functional assessment
Medicated patients, non-medicated patients and controls did not differ in age, gender, tactile sensitivity (Semmes-Weinstein test) or MVC grip strength (Table 1). Medicated patients had greater negative PANSS score (p = 0.004) than non-medicated patients, but did not differ in the other scores (Table 1). Both medicated and non-medicated patients took longer time to complete the functional dexterity (Moberg pick-up) task, but only medicated patients differed significantly (p<0.01) (Table 1).
Sensorimotor force tracking performance
All subjects completed the tracking task successfully (Fig. 1B, C), in the 5N condition (low target force), as well as in the 10% MVC condition (high target force, i.e. 38±9N in patients, 40±10N in controls). Target force in the 10% MVC condition was therefore about 8 times higher than in the 5N condition. Compared to healthy control subjects, medicated and non-medicated patients showed a decreased accuracy of visuomotor grip force control. All three performance measures were affected: relative error during ramp and hold (Fig. 1D), CV of force (Fig. 1E), and release duration (Fig. 1F). The ANOVA showed a significant effect of GROUP for relative error (F(2,35) = 10.7, p<0.001, η2 p = 0.379), with both groups of patients producing ∼50% more error than controls. Post-hoc comparisons showed differences between medicated patients and controls (p<0.001) and between non-medicated patients and controls (p = 0.02). Release duration also differed significantly according to GROUP (F(2,35) = 4.7, p = 0.01, η2 p = 0.21) and was ∼35% longer in patients. Post-hoc comparisons showed differences between controls and medicated patients (p = 0.005), but not between non-medicated patients and controls (p = 0.11). Force variability (CV) was also increased in both groups of patients (GROUP: F(2,35) = 4.7, p = 0.01, η2 p = 0.21), by ∼50%. Post-hoc comparisons showed differences between controls and medicated (p = 0.01) and non-medicated (p = 0.01) patients. There was no difference between medicated and non-medicated patients in error, release duration or CV (post-hoc error: p = 0.35; release duration: p = 0.58; CV: p = 0.58).
Error and release duration were similar in dominant and non-dominant hands, i.e. no effect of HAND (p>0.05), whereas, CV was higher in the dominant hand (F(1,35) = 11.5, p = 0.002, η2 p = 0.25). Posthoc tests showed that this difference in CV between hands was present in the three groups, and only at the 5N level (p<0.05).
ANOVA showed a significant FORCE effect on relative error (F(1,35) = 52.1; p<0.001; η2 p = 0.60) and CV (F(1,35) = 141.6; p<0.001; η2 p = 0.80), and a smaller effect on release duration (F(1,35) = 4.6; p = 0.04; η2 p = 0.12). For all three groups post-hoc comparisons revealed higher relative error (Fig. 1D) and a higher CV (Fig. 1E) at 5N compared to 10% MVC. No interaction was found between FORCE*GROUP. Mean error (across hands) correlated with mean release duration (r = 0.50, p = 0.001) and mean CV (r = 0.51, p = 0.003). Timing of force onsets and offsets were similar in all three groups (p>0.2).
Motor overflow to the resting hand was not visibly increased in patients (Fig. 1B, C) and no GROUP difference was found (F(2,35) = 0.6; p = 0.58; η2 p = 0.03).
Principal Component Analysis of force tracking across subjects
The PCA identified 19 PCs. The first three PCs explained a total of 83% of the variation in the force trace (PC1 explained 44%, PC2 31%, PC3 8%). The remaining 16 PCs explained less than 5% each. Fig. 2A shows the grand average force trace across all subjects and Fig. 2B–D show the corresponding loading scores for the first three PCs. The loading represents the information shared by (correlation between) a given PC and the time-varying force data. Qualitatively, the time-varying PC1 loading resembled the target force trajectory (Fig. 2B), the envelope of the PC2 loading resembled the inverse of the force trajectory (Fig. 2C), whereas the shape of the PC3 loading resembled the inverse of the first derivative of force (Fig. 2D). The factor scores represent the distance of each subject's data to the origin of each PC: a significant GROUP effect was found for PC2 scores (F(2,35) = 9.1; p<0.001; η2 p = 0.34), with PC2 factor scores being lower in controls compared to both medicated (p = 0.006) and non-medicated patients (p = 0.03) (asterisk in Fig. 2E). No group difference was found in PC1 or PC3 factor scores.
Separate principal component analysis of force tracking for controls and patients
For a more qualitative comparison we also performed a separate PCA for the two groups. For control subjects (N = 15) the first three PCs explained a total variance of 85% (PC1 explained 55%, PC2 24%, PC3 6%). Similarly for the patients (N = 23), the first three PCs explained a total variance of 84% (PC1 explained 52%, PC2 23%, PC3 9%). Figure 3A shows the average force trace for each subject, for controls (left) and for patients (right). Except for somewhat more variability during the baseline, there was no obvious difference between controls and patients. However, the loading profiles of the first three PCs showed qualitative differences (Fig. 3B–D). The PC1 loading profile of the controls was comparable to the force trace, which was not obvious for the patients. Nonetheless, the PC1 loading across all subjects (Fig. 2B) represented (on first approximation) the average of the two separate PC1 loadings. The PC2 loadings were qualitatively similar between the two groups (Fig. 3C) and similar to the profile across all subjects (Fig. 2C). The PC3 loadings were highest during the force transitions, for controls as well as for patients (Fig. 3D), as was the case for the PCA across subjects.
Relation between force tracking, PCA and clinical scales
For the PCA across subjects, a positive correlation was found between PC2 factor score and release duration across all subjects (Fig. 2F), but not for PC1 or PC3. Relative error also correlated positively with PC2 (R = 0.58, p<0.001) and negatively with PC3 (R = −0.66, p<0.001). Furthermore, in patients a positive correlation was observed between PC2 factor scores and the total PANSS scores (Fig. 2G) and with positive, negative and general sub-scores. No other significant correlations were obtained between clinical scales and force tracking data.
Model predictions
Simulation procedure
We modeled low-force trials and 8-times larger high-force trials, corresponding to the empirical difference between the 5N and 10% MVC condition. Three performance measures were calculated similar to the empirical data: relative error as well as CV during the hold period, and release duration.
Fitting procedure
At the group level (Table 2), SDN_Gain was first set such that the ratio of relative error (and of CV) between low and high force conditions was as close as possible to the empirical observed ratios. This was done separately for the grand average of controls and of patients. Second, I_Gain (and therefore TP_Gain) was set such that release duration was as close as possible to empirical values, again done separately for the grand average of controls and of patients (Table 2). In addition, for comparison at the individual level (Fig. 5B) this fitting procedure was applied subject-by-subject.
Table 2. Comparison between model and empirical performance in grip force tracking.
Model data | Empirical data | ||||
Low force | High force | Low force (5N) | High force 10% MVC | ||
Relative hold error | Controls | 0.75 (ref) | 62% | 9.8 (ref) | 61% |
SCZ | 134% | 106% | 145% | 108% | |
CV hold | Controls | 0.03 (ref) | 62% | 0.028 (ref) | 57% |
SCZ | 133% | 106% | 129% | 89% | |
Release duration | Controls | 73 ms (ref) | 98% | 73 ms (ref) | 99% |
SCZ | 140% | 138% | 143% | 127% |
Relative error during the hold period (Relative hold error), Coefficient of variation of force during the hold period (CV hold), release duration for controls and for patients (SCZ). For each variable, the ‘low force, control subject' data was taken as reference (ref) and the comparative values of the three other conditions expressed as %reference. Note that the large difference between the relative hold error (the reference values of the controls) between the model and empirical data is arbitrary, since the modeled force is dimensionless. This is the reason for the percentage-based comparison.
Results
Figure 5A shows simulated single-trial force-tracking for controls (left) and for patients (right). Optimal group gain-settings for controls were: SDN_Gain = 0.016, I_Gain = 0.2, TP_Gain = 0.3. With these settings relative error and CV decreased in the high force condition, whereas release duration remained unchanged, as was shown empirically (Table 2, c.f. Fig. 1D–F). For patients optimal group settings were: SDN_Gain = 0.028, I_Gain = 0.12, TP_Gain = 0.38. With these conjoint settings (larger SDN_gain, smaller I_Gain) the model performance showed increased relative error, CV and release duration in both low and high force conditions compared to controls, consistent with our empirical data (Table 2, c.f. Fig. 1D–F). These gain differences between controls and patients are also illustrated in Fig. 5C, F (stippled lines).
Systematic variation of these two critical gains showed that increased SDN in the motor output produces more error (and higher CV, not shown), but does not affect release duration (Fig. 5C, D). In contrast, reduced inhibition generates longer release duration, but has little or no effect on tracking error (Fig. 5D, F) or CV (not shown).
Fitting I_Gain and SDN_Gain to each subject's performance and plotting the gain space revealed that I_Gain was significantly and negatively correlated to SDN_Gain (Fig. 5B). This held across all subjects (r = −0.53, p<0.001), as well as within the patient group (r = −0.42, p<0.05). Again, patients tended to have lower I_Gains and higher SDN_Gains, in spite of substantial overlap. Furthermore, across subjects I_Gain also correlated significantly (r = −0.54, p<0.001) with the PC2 factor scores of the PCA.
Discussion
We found that schizophrenia patients (medicated and non-medicated) had significant deficits in visuomotor grip force-tracking: patients showed less accuracy in force modulation (higher tracking error), increased force variability (higher CV) and prolonged stopping of grip force (longer release duration). Nonetheless, all subjects achieved task completion. These significant motor deficits were also present in non-medicated patients, although sample size was small. This suggests that atypical antipsychotics cannot account for the observed alterations. This finding, however, needs confirmation in a larger sample (note on feasibility: pharmacological treatment was initiated without delay when clinically indicated). The relatively low PANNS scores of the non-medicated patients indicate that they had not yet relapsed and were thus comparable to medicated patients.
Model predictions on neural mechanisms
The potential mechanisms underlying the three observed tracking deficits have been investigated by a computational model which successfully replicated the behavioral characteristics of control subjects and of schizophrenia patients. The model suggests that reduced motor inhibition as well as increased SDN in the motor output account for these deficits: neither inhibition on its own, nor SDN explained the three empirically observed deficits (error, CV, release duration), but the combination of the two did. These two clear-cut model-predictions require empirical verification. Furthermore, I_Gain and SDN_Gain were correlated: this suggests that decreased motor inhibition may cause increased SDN. These results need to be understood within the four key-assumptions (constraints) of the model:
Inhibition was not constant, but varied as a function of force. Its time-varying form is consistent with empirical data: it was shown that short-latency intracortical inhibition (SICI) decreases with increasing grip force [24] and increases with muscle relaxation [25]. This increase with relaxation was modeled as a phasic inhibition during release: in the absence of this signal, variations of I_Gain did no longer affect release duration. Furthermore, this inhibitory profile is akin to decreased firing with increasing grip force found in subpopulations of primary motor (M1) and sensorimotor (S1) neurons in the non-human primate [34], [37]. In schizophrenia, several studies have found reduced SICI in medicated and non-medicated patients [23], [27], [28], in accordance with our modeling results.
SDN was added to the motor output (force) and white noise to visual and inhibitory signals, consistent with noise being present in all stages of sensorimotor control [38], including sensory [39] and signal-dependent motor noise [40]. Reduced signal-to-noise ratio in cortical processing has been highlighted in previous computational models of schizophrenia [41], [42] and is in line with less focused cortical processing observed in schizophrenia [43]–[45].
Multimodal sensory (visual and tactile/propriocptive) signals were summed to form the motor command. The interaction of tactile/proprioceptive [33] and visual cues [46] for grasp and force control has been extensively studied. Although the mathematical form of the integration remains unknown, there is no doubt that multi-modal interaction occurs at the cortical level (reviews: [47], [48]).
Signal gains: we assumed that patients with schizophrenia differed in gains, but not in other terms. This seems justified since patients with schizophrenia had no problems with task completion and no deficits in tactile or visual perception. Adding different time-varying signals for different groups would have lead to self-fulfilling results. A lower inhibitory gain in patients is compatible with patients being different from controls in terms of weaker modulation, but not in terms of absence of motor inhibition. Many studies showed an inhibition deficit in oculomotor [49] and manual tasks [50]: in particular, Badcock et al. [51] concluded, in line with our results, that patients had a deficit in modulating inhibition during movement execution.
PCA and motor inhibition
If PCs extracted from behavioral data reflect underlying control strategies (e.g. [31]), then our PCA across subjects would suggest altered neural control strategies in schizophrenia patients. The time-varying loading scores of each PC represented a distinct envelope: resembling the target force trajectory (PC1), its inverse (PC2) and its inverted derivative (PC3). Interestingly, PC loading profiles resembled cortical single cell activity: many M1 cells ([34], [37]), qualitatively similar to PC1 loading, increased their firing during ramp-and-hold precision grip force control, whereas some cells were activated with changes in force (as PC3), and some decreased firing as force increased (as PC2).
Remarkably, the PC2 scores, which we interpreted as an inhibition, were significantly different in patients compared to control subjects, but not between patient groups. These PCA results are thus entirely coherent with the predictions from the computational model: both suggest a task-related deficiency (weaker modulation) of motor inhibition in schizophrenia similar to that found in the oculomotor system [49], [58].
Separate PCA for the control group and for patients showed qualitative differences in the time-varying loading scores: this was obvious for PC1, whereas the loadings for PC2, which potentially reflect the time-course of inhibition, showed only marginal differences. This suggests that not the time-varying profile, but the gain (reflected by the PC factor score) varies among control subjects and patients, consistent with the model-prediction.
Furthermore, PC2 factor scores correlated positively with the empirically observed release duration, as well as with the PANSS symptom score. This suggests that the PC2 component may be a marker of reduced inhibition and may be indicative of more generalized pathological mechanisms, in line with other studies indicating that inhibition is linked to disease severity [59], [60].
Interpretational limits of the model
The interpretational limits of the computational model depend on both the incorporated and the non-incorporated constraints. The rational of the model was not to provide a novel theory of motor control but to quantify the motor consequences of gain changes in the interaction of sensory and motor signals. The model is compatible with most control theories, since its basic element is the feedback loop [52]. We will examine three limits: the exclusion of other explaining factors, neural topology and clinical specificity. (i) The model does not exclude other factors that may account for schizophrenia-specific deficits. In particular, the substantial overlap in gain values between schizophrenia patients and controls (Fig. 5B) suggests that additional factors intervene in schizophrenia-specific sensorimotor integration deficits. Nonetheless, the model-based result that reduced inhibition and increased SDN both produce motor deficits are two clear-cut predictions, which require further empirical verification. Moreover, the model also shows that these two factors are not completely independent suggesting that decreased inhibition may cause increased SDN. (ii) Since the model did not include any topological (structural) constraints, it does not strictu senso predict that the motor cortex is the neural structure which produces the modeled (and behaviorally observed) deficits. Therefore, an implication of the basal ganglia in force control [53] and/or the cerebellum [54] cannot be excluded. Nonetheless, we think that altered inhibition in the cortical motor areas is likely the cause of the observed motor deficit, as M1 is not only the main locus for motor execution, but also for stopping a motor action [55]. However, this needs further empirical verification. In accordance with our assumption, terminating a motor task has been shown to rely on inhibition within M1 [56], [25], and reduced inhibition within M1 correlates with increased disease severity in schizophrenia [28]. (iii) Whether the observed motor deficits and their computational explanation are specific to schizophrenia remains debatable. The computational results, including deficient cortical inhibition and altered motor noise as potential mechanisms, do not imply any specificity. Whether quantitatively similar behavioral deficits occur in other patient groups (e.g. Parkinson disease [57]) needs further investigation.
Conclusions
There is no reason to suppose that the sensorimotor system is exempt from the hypothesized neuro-developmental disorder underlying schizophrenia [61]. Consistent with this, our study shows clear, quantitative visuomotor grip force control deficits, independent of medication. The three affected performance measures may form a behavioral marker of schizophrenia. Our computational model as well as the PCA suggest that reduced modulation of inhibition and increased signal-dependent motor noise likely reflect the neuropathological mechanisms of the observed sensorimotor deficit in schizophrenia.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank all participants and the staff of the Centre d'Evaluation et de Recherche Clinique (CERC) and of the Service Hospital-Universitaire (SHU) Sainte-Anne.
Data Availability
The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are within the paper.
Funding Statement
MMT was supported by a PhD fellowship (CIFRE #2013/0226) from the French national agency for research and technology (ANRT) and Sensix (Poitiers, France). This project was in part funded by the French national research agency (grant # ANR-08-MNPS-004), by Fondation Pierre Deniker, by the European Research Area Net (ERA-NET Neuron 2010) and by Fondation de France. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Bleuler E (1911) Dementia Praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien. Leipzig, Germany: Deuticke, (Trans. J. Zinkin), Zinkin J, editor. New York, NY: International Universities Press; 1950.
- 2.Kraepelin E (1919) Dementia praecox and paraphrenia. (Translated by Barclay RM; edited by Robertson GM). New York: Robert E Krieger, 1971.
- 3. Manschreck TC, Maher BA, Rucklos ME, Vereen DR (1982) Disturbed voluntary motor activity in schizophrenic disorder. Psychol Med 12: 73–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Heinrichs DW, Buchanan RW (1988) Significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 145: 11–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Wolff AL, O'Driscoll GA (1999) Motor deficits and schizophrenia: the evidence from neuroleptic-naïve patients and populations at risk. J Psychiatry Neurosci 24(4): 304–14. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Putzhammer A, Klein HE (2006) Quantitative analysis of motor disturbances in schizophrenic patients. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 8(1): 123–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Delevoye-Turrell Y, Giersch A, Danion JM (2002) A deficit in the adjustment of grip force responses in schizophrenia. Neuroreport 13(12): 1537–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Delevoye-Turrell Y, Giersch A, Danion JM (2003) Abnormal sequencing of motor actions in patients with schizophrenia: evidence from grip force adjustments during object manipulation. Am J Psychiatry 160(1): 134–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Grootens KP, Vermeeren L, Jan Verkes R, Buitelaar JK, Bernard G, et al. (2009) Psychomotor planning is deficient in recent-onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 107: 294–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Delevoye-Turrell Y, Giersch A, Wing AM, Danion JM (2007) Motor fluency deficits in the sequencing of actions in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 116(1): 56–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Giersch A, Wilquin H, Capa RL, Delevoye-Turrell YN (2013) Combined visual and motor disorganization in patients with schizophrenia. Front Psychol 4: 620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Rosen AJ, Lockhart JJ, Gants ES, Westergaard CK (1991) Maintenance of grip-induced muscle tension: a behavioral marker of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 100(4): 583–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Nowak DA, Connemann BJ, Alan M, Spitzer M (2006) Sensorimotor dysfunction of grasping in schizophrenia: a side effect of antipsychotic treatment? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77(5): 650–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Lehoux C, Everett J, Laplante L, Emond C, Trépanier J, et al. (2003) Fine motor dexterity is correlated to social functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 62(3): 269–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Manschreck TC, Maher BA, Candela SF (2004) Earlier age of first diagnosis in schizophrenia is related to impaired motor control. Schizophr Bull 30(2): 351–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Midorikawa A, Hashimoto R, Noguchi H (2008) Impairment of motor dexterity in schizophrenia assessed by a novel finger movement test. Psychiatry Res 159(3): 281–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Lafargue G, Franck N, Sirigu A (2006) Sense of motor effort in patients with schizophrenia. Cortex 42: 711–719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Krebs M-O, Gut-Fayand A, Bourdel M-C, Dischamp J, Olie J-P (2000) Validation and factorial structure of a standardized neurological examination assessing neurological soft signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 45: 245–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Bombin I, Arango C, Buchanan RW (2005) Significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia: two decades later. Schizophr Bull 31(4): 962–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Chan RC, Xu T, Heinrichs RW, Yu Y, Wang Y (2010) Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull 36(6): 1089–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Le Seac'h A, Picard H, Gorsane MA, Vidal PP, Amado I, et al. (2012) A step toward an objective quantification of subtle neurological signs in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 198(2): 230–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Radhu N, de Jesus DR, Ravindran LN, Zanjani A, Fitzgerald PB, et al. (2013) A meta-analysis of cortical inhibition and excitability using transcranial magnetic stimulation in psychiatric disorders. Clin Neurophysiol 124(7): 1309–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Daskalakis ZJ, Christensen BK, Chen R, Fitzgerald PB, Zipursky RB, et al. (2002) Evidence for impaired cortical inhibition in schizophrenia using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59: 347–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Rantalainen T, Weier A, Leung M, Brandner C, Spittle M, et al. (2013) Short-interval intracortical inhibition is not affected by varying visual feedback in an isometric task in biceps brachii muscle. Front Hum Neurosci 7: 68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Motawar B, Hur P, Stinear J, Seo NJ (2012) Contribution of intracortical inhibition in voluntary muscle relaxation. Exp Brain Res 221(3): 299–308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Benes FM, McSparren J, Bird ED, SanGiovanni JP, Vincent SL (1991) Deficits in small interneurons in prefrontal and cingulate cortices of schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48(11): 996–1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Daskalakis ZJ, Christensen BK, Fitzgerald PB, Moller B, Fountain SI, et al. (2008) Increased cortical inhibition in persons with schizophrenia treated with clozapine. J Psychopharmacol 22(2): 203–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Liu SK, Fitzgerald PB, Daigle M, Chen R, Daskalakis ZJ (2009) The relationship between cortical inhibition, antipsychotic treatment, and the symptoms of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 65(6): 503–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Bleton JP, Teremetz M, Vidailhet M, Mesure S, Maier MA, et al. (2014) Impaired force control in writer's cramp showing a bilateral deficit in sensorimotor integration. Mov Disord 29(1): 130–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Lindberg P, Ody C, Feydy A, Maier MA (2009) Precision in isometric precision grip force is reduced in middle-aged adults. Exp Brain Res 193(2): 213–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Santello M, Flanders M, Soechting JF (2002) Patterns of hand motion during grasping and the influence of sensory guidance. J Neurosci 22(4): 426–1435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA (1987) The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 13(2): 261–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Johansson RS, Flanagan JR (2009) Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(5): 345–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Wannier TM, Maier MA, Hepp-Reymond MC (1991) Contrasting properties of monkey somatosensory and motor cortex neurons activated during the control of force in precision grip. J Neurophysiol 65(3): 572–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394(6695): 780–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Binzegger T, Douglas RJ, Martin KA (2004) A quantitative map of the circuit of cat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 24(39): 8441–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Maier MA, Bennett KM, Hepp-Reymond MC, Lemon RN (1993) Contribution of the monkey corticomotoneuronal system to the control of force in precision grip. J Neurophysiol 69(3): 772–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM (2008) Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 9(4): 292–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Churchland MM, Afshar A, Shenoy KV (2006) A central source of movement variability. Neuron 52(6): 1085–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Jones KE, Hamilton AF, Wolpert DM (2002) Sources of signal-dependent noise during isometric force production. J Neurophysiol 88(3): 1533–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Spitzer M (1997) A cognitive neuroscience view of schizophrenic thought disorder. Schizophr Bull 23(1): 29–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Rolls ET, Deco G (2011) A computational neuroscience approach to schizophrenia and its onset. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35(8): 1644–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Fornito A, Zalesky A, Pantelis C, Bullmore ET (2012) Schizophrenia, neuroimaging and connectomics. Neuroimage 62(4): 2296–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Frantseva M, Cui J, Farzan F, Chinta LV, Perez Velazquez JL, et al. (2014) Disrupted Cortical Conductivity in Schizophrenia: TMS-EEG Study. Cereb Cortex 24(1): 211–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Uhlhaas PJ (2013) Dysconnectivity, large-scale networks and neuronal dynamics in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23(2): 283–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Flanagan JR, Bowman MC, Johansson RS (2006) Control strategies in object manipulation tasks. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16(6): 650–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Rizzolatti G, Luppino G (2001) The cortical motor system. Neuron 31(6): 889–901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Davare M, Kraskov A, Rothwell JC, Lemon RN (2011) Interactions between areas of the cortical grasping network. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21(4): 565–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Krebs MO, Bourdel MC, Cherif ZR, Bouhours P, Lôo H, et al. (2010) Deficit of inhibition motor control in untreated patients with schizophrenia: further support from visually guided saccade paradigms. Psychiatry Res 179(3): 279–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Lipszyc J, Schahar R (2010) Inhibitory control and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. J Intl Neuropsychol Soc 16: 1064–1076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Badcock JC, Michie PT, Johnson L, Combrinck J (2002) Acts of control in schizophrenia: Dissociating the components of inhibition. Psychol Medicine 32: 287–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Scott SH (2008) Inconvenient truths about neural processing in primary motor cortex. J Physiol. 2008 Mar 1 586(5): 1217–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Prodoehl J, Corcos DM, Vaillancourt DE (2009) Basal ganglia mechanisms underlying precision grip force control. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33(6): 900–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Manto M, Bower JM, Conforto AB, Delgado-García JM, da Guarda SN, et al. (2012) Consensus paper: roles of the cerebellum in motor control–the diversity of ideas on cerebellar involvement in movement. Cerebellum 11(2): 457–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Stinear CM, Coxon JP, Byblow WD (2009) Primary motor cortex and movement prevention: where Stop meets Go. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33(5): 662–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Coxon JP, Stinear CM, Byblow WD (2006) Intracortical inhibition during volitional inhibition of prepared action. J Neurophysiol 95(6): 3371–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Neely KA, Planetta PJ, Prodoehl J, Corcos DM, Comella CL, et al. (2013) Force control deficits in individuals with Parkinson's disease, multiple systems atrophy, and progressive supranuclear palsy. PLoS One 8(3): e58403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Raemaekers M, Jansma JM, Cahn W, Van der Geest JN, van der Linden JA, et al. (2002) Neuronal substrate of the saccadic inhibition deficit in schizophrenia investigated with 3-dimensional event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59(4): 313–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Clark LK, Warman D, Lysaker PH (2010) The relationships between schizophrenia symptom dimensions and executive functioning components. Schizophr Res 124(1–3): 169–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Donohoe G, Corvin A, Robertson IH (2006) Evidence that specific executive functions predict symptom variance among schizophrenia patients with a predominantly negative symptom profile. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 11(1): 13–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Rapoport JL, Giedd JN, Gogtay N (2012) Neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: update 2012. Mol Psychiatry 17(12): 1228–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All relevant data are within the paper.