Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
editorial
. 2014 Nov 7;8:904. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00904

Oscillatory “temporal sampling” and developmental dyslexia: toward an over-arching theoretical framework

Usha Goswami 1,*, Alan J Power 1, Marie Lallier 2, Andrea Facoetti 3
PMCID: PMC4224062  PMID: 25426052

Human cognitive systems such as language represent the sensory world as unitary. For example, the “speech signal” is perceived as a single auditory stimulus, and different visual features and textures in the visual field are perceived holistically as unitary objects. Yet sensory neuroscience demonstrates the diversity of encoding in the neural systems supporting sensory perception. Different aspects of sensory information are processed in parallel, often at different timescales and by different populations of neurons. Further, research into the function of neuronal oscillations (e.g., Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004) reveals a key role in processing sensory information. In this special issue, we investigate the developmental implications of one neuroscientific theory based on oscillations that is relevant to reading education and developmental dyslexia, the “temporal sampling” framework (hereafter TSF, Goswami, 2011). The TSF identifies specific oscillatory mechanisms that may be impaired in dyslexia. We expect that deeper understanding of neural mechanisms of information-processing will eventually enable deeper understanding of developmental disorders of learning, such as dyslexia. Developmental dyslexia is a disorder in the acquisition of successful reading and spelling skills that is found in ~7% of children across cultures.

The ability to read and write—the achievement of literacy—is one of the most complex and sophisticated cognitive skills developed by the brain. Literacy skills develop as a result of direct teaching, usually in childhood, and become fluent during years of practice. By adulthood, most brains have read millions of words. This makes it difficult to disentangle cause from effect when studying sensory and neural factors. Dyslexia is usually only diagnosed after 2–3 years of schooling, when the brain has already had considerable experience of reading and reading tuition. Dyslexia is diagnosed when children who are receiving adequate teaching and who have no overt sensory or neurological problems fail to develop fast, efficient and age-appropriate reading and spelling.

The TSF was proposed as the neural basis for the extraction of phonological information from speech via auditory oscillatory encoding (Goswami, in press). Based on work by Poeppel, Greenberg, Giraud, Ghitza and many others (Giraud and Poeppel, 2012, for a recent summary), the TSF linked “sampling” of the speech stream by auditory cortical networks operating at different timescales or oscillatory frequencies (delta, theta, gamma) to the emergence of phonological (linguistic) coding of speech by children. Poeppel and others argued that cortical oscillations enabled the representation of different temporal rates of amplitude modulation in the complex speech signal. These temporal frequency bands yield complementary “windows” of information relating to cognitive linguistic units such as syllables (theta rate) and phonemes (gamma rate, see Poeppel, 2003). Accordingly, the TSF proposed that atypical oscillatory sampling at one or more temporal rates in children with dyslexia could cause phonological difficulties in specifying linguistic units such as syllables.

Phonological difficulties in dyslexia are related to difficulties in the accurate perception of amplitude “rise time” (related to modulation rate). The TSF proposed that atypical oscillatory entrainment at syllable-relevant rates of amplitude modulation (delta [~ stressed syllable rate], theta [~ syllable rate]) could be one neural cause of the “phonological deficit” found in children and adults with dyslexia across languages and orthographies. This theory is about early developmental mechanisms, nevertheless impaired oscillatory sampling in auditory cortex could, over developmental time, lead to atypical functioning of the left-lateralized “reading network” identified in many fMRI studies of older children (Richlan, 2012, for a recent overview; Clark et al., 2014, for a relevant longitudinal study). This should be true across languages. Indeed, rise time perception is impaired in English, French, Spanish, Hungarian, Finnish, Chinese, and Dutch dyslexic children (see Goswami and Leong, 2013, for an overview). The “phonological deficit” in dyslexia is found in all of these languages, and manifests as difficulties in oral tasks such as recognizing prosodic stress, counting syllables, and counting or deleting phonemes (the smallest phonological units in a language; Ziegler and Goswami, 2005, for a cross-language review). These and other phonological tasks are considered by some of the auditory-based contributions to the special issue (Lehongre et al., 2013; Power et al., 2013; White-Schwoch and Kraus, 2013; Sela, 2014 this issue).

The aim of this special issue, however, was to simultaneously invite colleagues who work on visual sensory processing to consider whether atypical oscillatory “temporal sampling” may explain the pervasive visual processing deficits in dyslexia reported in many orthographies (e.g., Facoetti et al., 2010; Lallier and Valdois, 2012). Visual and auditory sensory theories of dyslexia are typically considered to compete with each other, indeed a recent review counted 12 competing theories of developmental dyslexia (Ramus and Ahissar, 2012). The act of reading of course depends upon many visual processes. Examples are (for alphabetic orthographies) serial letter recognition, visual grouping of repeating letter patterns in familiar words, and the left-to-right (or in some orthographies, right-to-left) horizontal linear tracking of print. Practice in reading (reading experience) will obviously train the brain in aspects of visual processing related to reading. Such visual practice is necessarily reduced in children with dyslexia (reading is effortful, so the child reads less). Disentangling the effects of reading experience on the brain across the many different sensory and cognitive components that support the development of reading and writing is thus experimentally challenging. Nevertheless, by studying particular aspects of non-linguistic visual processing in isolation (such as magnocellular function, or eye movements), research can begin to disentangle cause from effect in developmental dyslexia.

In this special issue, a number of the different aspects of impaired visual and visuo-spatial attentional processing found in dyslexia are studied and possible relations with oscillatory temporal sampling are considered (see De Luca et al., 2013; Lallier et al., 2013; Conlon et al., 2013; Gori et al., 2014; Ruffino et al., 2014; Varvara et al., 2014 this issue). Theoretically, these contributions consider whether spatiotemporal sampling of information by the visual system may be impaired in dyslexia (see Pammer, 2014; Vidyasagar, 2013 this issue). Indeed, Vidyasagar argues that a visual sampling impairment may be primary to the auditory difficulties in dyslexia documented by other contributors, a provocative claim which requires longitudinal studies. In fact, in order to establish the possible causal role of different visual and auditory sensory processes to reading development, and to identify their sequential contributions during the developmental learning trajectory, a range of developmental research designs are required.

At minimum, evidence is required that:

  1. the sensory/neural deficit precedes being taught to read

  2. the sensory/neural deficit affects aspects of cognitive development other than reading (e.g., musical development for auditory deficits, conceptual development for visual deficits) in predictable ways

  3. the sensory/neural deficit can be demonstrated when children with dyslexia are compared to younger children whose reading skills are matched with the dyslexics (this research design aims to equate the effect of reading experience on the brain; the reading level match research design)

  4. developmental trajectories are followed in longitudinal studies, exploring the complex interplay of auditory and visual sensory/neural and cognitive processes during the development of reading, thereby establishing the developmental primacy of the candidate deficit

  5. the sensory/neural deficit is consistent across different languages and orthographies

  6. training the candidate deficit has demonstrable effects upon subsequent reading development

Longitudinal studies, beginning before reading is taught and carried out across languages, are enormously important to the field (e.g., Boets et al., 2011; Franceschini et al., 2012). Sensory/neural deficits may change over developmental time. Perhaps a sensory factor critical for early development becomes less relevant when studying older children, or is no longer apparent when studying adults. Sensory/neural deficits may also manifest differently in different languages, for example as a consequence of factors such as orthographic grain size (e.g., the phonemic grain size is practiced by readers of alphabetic languages, whereas Japanese readers practice the syllable grain size) or phonology (e.g., rhythmic or phonetic differences, such as whether a language has many sonorant phonemes and is syllable-timed, such as Spanish, or many plosive phonemes and is stress-timed, such as English). Reading difficulties may be comorbid with other difficulties such as attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); the possible effects of co-morbid disorders on sensory processing must be taken into account (Thaler et al., 2009). The studies in this special issue document and calibrate some of these aspects of auditory and visual processing that seem to be important in developmental dyslexia. Incorporating all of these aspects of sensory processing into oscillatory studies will be the next task for developmental research into dyslexia.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Boets B., Vandermosten M., Cornelissen P., Wouters J., Ghesquiere P. (2011). Coherent motion sensitivity and reading development in the transition from prereading to reading stage. Child Dev. 82, 854–869. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01527.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Buzsáki G., Draguhn A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929. 10.1126/science.1099745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Clark K., Helland T., Specht K., Narr K., Manis F., Toga A., et al. (2014). Neuroanatomical precursors of dyslexia identified from pre-reading through age 11. Brain. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1093/brain/awu229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Conlon E., Lilleskaret G., Wright C. M., Stuksrud A. (2013). Why do adults with dyslexia have poor global motion sensitivity? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:859. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00859 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. De Luca M., Pontillo M., Primativo S., Spinelli D., Zoccolotti P. (2013). The eye-voice lead during oral reading in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:696. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00696 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Facoetti A., Trussardi A. N., Ruffino M., Lorusso M. L., Cattaneo C., Galli R., et al. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1011–1025. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Franceschini S., Gori S., Ruffino M., Pedrolli K., Facoetti A. (2012). A causal link between visual spatial attention and reading acquisition. Curr. Biol. 22, 814–819. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Giraud A. L., Poeppel D. (2012). Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 511–517. 10.1038/nn.3063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gori S., Cecchini P., Bigoni A., Molteni M., Facoetti A. (2014). Magnocellular-dorsal pathway and sub-lexical route in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:460. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00460 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Goswami U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 3–10. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Goswami U. (in press). Sensory theories of developmental dyslexia: three challenges for research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Goswami U., Leong V. (2013). Speech rhythm and temporal structure: converging perspectives? Lab. Phonol. 4, 67–92 10.1515/lp-2013-0004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Lallier M., Donnadieu S., Valdois S. (2013). Investigating the role of visual and auditory search in reading and developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:597. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lallier M., Valdois S. (2012). Sequential versus simultaneous processing deficits in developmental dyslexia, in Dyslexia - A Comprehensive and International Approach, eds Wydell T. N., Fern-Pollak L. (Rijeka, Croatia: In Tech; ), 73–108. [Google Scholar]
  15. Lehongre K., Morillon B., Giraud A.-L., Ramus F. (2013). Impaired auditory sampling in dyslexia: further evidence from combined fMRI and EEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:454. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00454 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Pammer K. (2014). Temporal sampling in vision and implications for dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:933. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Poeppel D. (2003). The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as ‘asymmetric sampling in time’. Speech Commun. 41, 245–255 10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00107-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Power A. J., Mead N., Barnes L., Goswami U. (2013). Neural entrainment to rhythmic speech in children with developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:777. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00777 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ramus F., Ahissar M. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: the difficulties of interpreting poor performance, and the importance of normal performance. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 29, 104–122. 10.1080/02643294.2012.677420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Richlan F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: dysfunction of a left hemisphere reading network. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:120. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ruffino M., Gori S., Boccardi D., Molteni M., Facoetti A. (2014). Spatial and temporal attention are both sluggish in poor phonological decoders with developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:331. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Sela I. (2014). Visual and auditory synchronization deficits among dyslexic readers as compared to non-impaired readers: a cross-correlation algorithm analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:364. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00364 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Thaler V., Urton K., Heine A., Hawelka S., Engl V., Jacobs A. M. (2009). Different behaviour and eye movement patterns of dyslexic readers with and without attentional deficits during single word reading. Neuropsychologia 47, 2436–2445. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Varvara P., Varuzza C., Sorrentino A. C. P., Vicari S., Menghini D. (2014). Executive functions in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:120. 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Vidyasagar T. R. (2013). Reading into neuronal oscillations in the visual system: implications for developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:811. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00811 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. White-Schwoch T., Kraus N. (2013). Physiologic discrimination of stop consonants relates to phonological skills in pre-readers: a biomarker for subsequent reading ability? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:899. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00899 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ziegler J. C., Goswami U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol. Bull. 131, 3–29. 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES